Confirmation of QTL Effects and Evidence of Genetic Dominance of Honeybee Defensive Behavior: Results of Colony and Individual Behavioral Assays

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Confirmation of QTL Effects and Evidence of Genetic Dominance of Honeybee Defensive Behavior: Results of Colony and Individual Behavioral Assays"

Transcription

1 Behavior Genetics, Vol. 32, No. 2, March 2002 ( 2002) Confirmation of QTL Effects and Evidence of Genetic Dominance of Honeybee Defensive Behavior: Results of Colony and Individual Behavioral Assays Ernesto Guzmán-Novoa, 1,3 Greg J. Hunt, 2 José L. Uribe, 1 Christine Smith, 2 and Miguel E. Arechavaleta-Velasco 2 Received 23 Mar Final 28 Oct The stinging and guarding components of the defensive behavior of European, Africanized, hybrid, and backcross honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) were compared and analyzed at both colony and individual levels. Hybrid and Africanized backcross colonies stung as many times as Africanized ones. European backcross colonies stung more than European bees but not as many times as Africanized or Africanized backcross colonies. The degree of dominance for the number of times that worker bees stung a leather patch was estimated to be 84.3%, 200.8%, and 145.8% for hybrid, backcross European, and backcross Africanized colonies, respectively. Additionally, both guards at the colony entrance and fast-stinging workers of one European backcross colony had a significantly higher frequency of an Africanized DNA marker allele, located near sting1, a QTL previously implicated in stinging behavior at the colony level. However, guards and faststinging bees from a backcross to the Africanized parental colony did not differ from control bees in their frequency for the Africanized and European markers, as would be expected if large genetic dominance effects for sting1 exist. These results support the hypothesis that genetic dominance influences the defensive behavior of honeybees and confirm the effect of sting1 on the defensiveness of individual worker bees. KEY WORDS: Apis mellifera; defensive behavior; Africanized honeybees; genetic dominance; QTL. INTRODUCTION The evolution of complex social organizations is an important issue in biology. Insect colonies may serve as model of complex systems, and honeybee colonies stand as an example of social organization because of their large number of members and their well-known division of labor (Winston, 1987). Honeybee colonies are typically 1 CENIFMA-INIFAP, Santa Crúz 29-B, Las Haciendas, Metepéc, Edo. de Méx., Mexico. 2 Department of Entomology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN To whom all correspondence should be addressed. Tel: (011-52) Fax: guzmane@inifap. conacyt.mx 95 composed of one reproductive (queen) and thousands of nonreproductive females (workers). The workers perform most of the tasks upon which the fitness of the colony depends. Female honeybees are diploid, but the males (drones) are haploid because they develop from unfertilized eggs (reviewed by Winston, 1987). A queen bee mates while flying with up to 17 males (Adams et al., 1977) and stores the sperm in a spermatheca for the rest of her life (Page, 1986). As a consequence, honeybee colonies with open-mated queens contain many subfamilies of workers, with each worker in a subfamily inheriting the same genetic contribution from the father (all the sperm cells produced by an individual male carry identical genomes; Page and Laidlaw, 1988). Natural selection acting at the level of colonies has probably been responsible for many observed features /02/ / Plenum Publishing Corporation

2 96 Guzmán-Novoa, Hunt, Uribe, Smith, and Arechavaleta-Velasco of colony organization (Crozier and Consul, 1976; Owen, 1986; Seeley, 1989). Selection acts on colonies, but the genes that affect heritable differences in colony social structure reside in the colony residents. So, the genotypic structures of colonies must change with respect to heritable components of division of labor, where individuals repeatedly perform specific tasks. Therefore, it is important to identify and study traits of individual workers that have heritable variability expressed at the colony level on which natural selection can operate. Heritable differences in the likelihood that individual nestmate workers perform specific tasks have been demonstrated repeatedly (Hellmich et al., 1985; Moritz and Hillesheim, 1985; Robinson and Page, 1988, 1989; Calderone and Page, 1988, 1991, 1992; Hillesheim et al., 1989; Rothenbuhler and Page, 1989; Breed et al., 1990; Breed and Rogers, 1991; Robinson et al., 1990; Page and Robinson, 1991; Guzmán-Novoa and Gary, 1993; Guzmán-Novoa et al., 1994; Giray et al., 2000). However, there are not many genetic studies that establish the connection between individual behavior and colony behavior. Finding evidence that establishes a relationship between genetically influenced individual behavior and colony behavior is important to demonstrate how individuals with different likelihoods to perform a task interact to influence a colony phenotype. Recently, a few studies have identified QTLs with effects on behavior in honeybees based on whole-colony measurements. Several QTLs that influence pollen foraging were identified in crosses between strains of colonies that were selected for either large or small amounts of pollen stored in the combs. These QTLs were subsequently shown to influence individual foraging behavior and physiology, using several independent crosses (Hunt et al., 1995; Page et al., 1995, 2000). We identified QTLs that influenced the numbers of stings deposited in whole-colony defensive-behavior assays, in crosses derived from Africanized honeybees (Hunt et al., 1998). One QTL (sting1) controlled experiment-wise error at p (LOD 3.57). Several other QTLs were also identified for their effect on the production of alarm pheromone components found in the stinging apparatus of worker bees (Hunt et al., 1999). These QTLs were independent of the previously identified loci that influenced stinging behavior at the colony level. However, no relationship has yet been established between QTLs influencing colony-level traits and defensive behavior of individual worker bees. Africanized honeybees are hybrids of African (Apis mellifera scutellata) and European honeybee races (primarily A. mellifera ligustica). Africanized bees originated in Brazil in 1956 (Kerr, 1967) and have since spread throughout most of the Americas. They were detected in Mexico in 1986 (Moffett et al., 1987) and in the United States in 1990 (Sugden and Williams, 1991). Africanized honeybees appear to have retained their highly defensive behavior and predominantly African genotype (Hall and Muralidharan, 1989; Lobo et al., 1989; Smith et al., 1989; Hall, 1990; Muralidharan and Hall, 1990; Rinderer et al., 1991; Sheppard et al., 1991; Hall, 1992a,b; Moritz and Meusel, 1992; Quezada-Euán and Medina, 1998). Africanized bees also outcompete European races in warmer climates. In our study area in Mexico, the frequency of African mitochondrial type in feral swarms increased from 29% to 96% in five years time (Guzmán-Novoa and Page 1999). High defensiveness is the most noticeable characteristic of Africanized bees (Stort, 1975a c; Collins et al., 1982; Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1993, 1994a). In Mexico, these bees have caused thousands of stinging incidents that resulted in the deaths of more than 300 people as well as in several thousand animal fatalities (Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1994b; Cajero, 1995). In the United States, only eight people have died as a consequence of stinging incidents since the arrival of Africanized bees (E. H. Erickson, pers. com.), but the number of fatalities could increase in the future if these bees continue to spread. The defensive behavior of Africanized bees is heritable in the broad sense (Stort, 1975c; Collins et al., 1984; Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1994a), but the mode of inheritance is unclear. Stort (1975c) proposed that the highly defensive behavior of Africanized bees showed dominance. However, Collins et al. (1988) found primarily additive effects. Later, several studies supported the dominance hypothesis for whole-colony measures of stinging behavior, but the degree of dominance remains unclear (Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1993, 1994a; DeGrandi-Hoffman et al., 1998). The two major components of the defensive behavior of honeybees are the tasks of guarding and stinging. Guard bees patrol the entrance to their colony. They inspect incoming bees, exclude bees (or other invertebrates) that are foreign to their nest, and alert other colony workers about intruders. Stinger bees fly out of the colony and sting intruders. A few studies have shown evidence of a correlation between the level of guarding behavior and the level of colony defensiveness in European honeybees (Breed et al., 1988; Robinson and Page, 1988; Breed and Rogers, 1991), but no studies on this subject have

3 Genetics and Honeybee Defensive Behavior 97 been conducted with Africanized bees. In contrast, many studies have reported evidence of genetic effects on the stinging behavior of Africanized bee colonies (Stort, 1975a c; Collins et al., 1982, 1984; Collins, 1986; Villa, 1988; Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1993, 1994a; DeGrandi-Hoffman et al., 1998; Hunt et al., 1998), primarily measured at the colony level. Whether the defensive behavior of honeybee colonies shows additive or dominant inheritance may have implications for the fitness of honeybee colonies, for human and animal health, and for honeybee breeding. If the defensive behavior of honeybees is inherited additively, colonies of hybrid origin would be expected to become less fit in environments in which benefits associated with defensiveness for reducing predation outweigh the negative aspects of defensiveness, such as loss of worker bees that sting and the disruption of colony activities. However, additive inheritance would also make it easier to breed gentler bees, and fewer stinging incidents would be expected to occur. If dominance effects influence this behavior, breeding gentler bees would be more difficult to accomplish and more human and animal lives could be at risk. We describe results from behavioral assays of individual bees and linkage disequilibrium with a QTL in samples of bees performing tasks related to colony defense. We also present a more thorough analysis of the genetic dominance of defensive behavior. MATERIAL AND METHODS Experimental Colonies The experiments were conducted in Ixtapan de la Sal, Mexico (19 N, 99 W). Hybrid European- Africanized colonies were produced, and from these colonies, other colonies containing backcross workers were generated. The European colonies used for the experiments were derived from queens that had been imported previously from the United States and that had been maintained with instrumental insemination by a local beekeeper. The Africanized source was derived from colonies obtained locally. Morphometric (Sylvester and Rinderer, 1987) and mitochondrial DNA (Hall and Smith, 1991; Nielsen et al., 2000) analyses of the queens progeny were used to confirm the strain origin of our source colonies, but it is possible that some European-strain DNA may have been introgressed into the Africanized line. The morphometric analyses were based on the smaller wing sizes of the African subspecies. The mitochondrial assays were based on diagnostic restriction sites that are present in European strains but not in the African subspecies. A single-drone instrumental insemination (one drone for each queen) was used in all the crosses. Twelve Africanized and 9 European bee colonies were produced. To produce the colonies, queens were reared from six (randomly selected) of the source colonies (three Africanized and three European). Each of these queens was inseminated with the semen of one drone obtained from a colony of her respective origin, to produce virgin daughter queens in families that were closely related because they shared the same haploid father. Inseminated queens were identified by colored, numbered plastic tags (Graze KG, Weinstadt, Germany) glued to their thoraces and their right wings were clipped. Nine hybrid colonies were derived from these colonies (containing F 1 workers) by using either Africanized or European colonies as the drone source for each F 1 cross. An additional generation of 40 backcross colonies was produced, using the same procedure. Nineteen F 1 queens were each mated to an Africanized drone (19 drones), and the other 21 were each mated to a European drone (21 drones). Therefore, we had five different sets of experimental colonies: Africanized, European, F 1, Africanized backcross, and European backcross colonies. The experimental colonies were located in five different apiaries approximately 500 m apart. We positioned hives at least 5 m apart to minimize interhive drifting of workers. Behavioral Assays Each experimental colony was tested on three occasions with a defensive behavior assay. Fourteen days before conducting the defensive trials, the populations of the colonies were equalized by removing bees and frames of brood from the most populous colonies. Each of the colonies contained about 4,000 cm 2 (four frames) with capped brood and five frames with adult bees after equalization. Colony equalization was necessary to control for differences in colony population that may affect defensive behavior tests. We used a modification of a previous behavioral assay (Villa, 1988) to test the stinging behavior of the experimental colonies (see Guzmán-Novoa et al., 1999, for more details about methods for testing the defensiveness of honeybee colonies). The assay consisted of a 10 cm 8 cm black suede patch (a flag ) suspended on a piece of white wood (0.7 cm 0.5 cm 100 cm). To provide a stimulus to trigger the defensive response

4 98 Guzmán-Novoa, Hunt, Uribe, Smith, and Arechavaleta-Velasco of colonies, a flag was rhythmically elevated (about 4 cm) and lowered (about 4 cm; two swings per second) approximately 5 to 10 cm in front of the entrance of each hive. We removed the flag after the first 5 to 10 bees had stung the leather patch. These bees were grabbed with forceps from the leather patch (to which they were attached by their stingers) and stored in 96% ethanol until subsequent DNA analyses. These fast-responding bees were classified as first stingers. Immediately after removing the flag with the first stingers, another flag was presented in front of the colony entrance by another manipulator, to permit the bees to continue stinging until a period of 60 seconds was completed. After each trial, we packed and sealed the leather patches (from the second flag) for subsequent sting counts. All colonies within an apiary were simultaneously tested. Two operators who did not know the genotype of the colonies tested each colony. In addition to these three trials, each of two backcross colonies, one European (colony 33) and one Africanized (colony 60), was tested on 14 to 15 additional occasions (as described previously) to obtain at least 100 first stingers. Guard Bees Samples of at least 100 guard bees were collected with a vacuum device (Gary and Lorenzen, 1987) from each of the same backcross colonies where 100 first stingers were obtained (colonies 33 and 60). Guard bees were detected by observing their typical behavior (Ribbands, 1954) at the hive entrance for a period of 5 minutes before taking the sample. Guard bees stand with their forelegs off the ground, with their antennae held forwards to touch and inspect incoming bees. Additionally, we obtained random samples of worker bees ( controls ) from each of these backcross colonies. Between 45 and 100 control bees were collected from the outer combs of their respective hives. We chose not to collect from inner brood combs because brood combs would be more likely to contain nurse bees that are younger than guards. Samples were stored at 20 C in 96% ethanol until subsequent DNA analyses. Genetic Models The actual values obtained from the defensive trials of the experimental colonies were compared with expected values for number of stings that were calculated using two models. Both models assume genotypic additivity, meaning that individuals of a particular genotype would exhibit the same degree of defensiveness, regardless of whether they were living in a colony containing individuals of the same genotype as themselves or one that contained bees of another genotype. It is possible that bees of different genotypes interact to alter response thresholds, in which case a more complex model would be needed to describe the system (see Page and Robinson, 1991), but without knowing the nature of the interaction, it is premature to construct and use such a model. The first model assumes complete genetic dominance for the Africanized trait of high defensiveness (Equation 1). The second model assumes genetic additivity (i.e., hybrids are intermediate between European and Africanized workers, Equation 2). The expected values for stings in patches (E) were obtained as follows: E d P H R A (P E R E ) (P A R A ) (1) E a P H (R A R E )/2 (P E R E ) (P A R A ) (2) Where P H is the proportion of expected hybrid genotypes in the progeny P E is the proportion of expected European genotypes in the progeny P A is the proportion of expected Africanized genotypes in the progeny R A is the mean Africanized response R E is the mean European response Additionally, we estimated the degree of dominance (% d ) of the defensive behavior of hybrid and backcross colonies as follows: % d (R p E a )/(E d E a ) 100 (3) Where R p is the mean progeny response DNA Analyses To estimate the proportion of bees carrying the European and/or the African alleles for sting1 (which presumably affects the stinging behavior of honeybees at the colony level (see Hunt et al., 1998), randomly collected subsamples of at least 90 guard, 93 first stinger, and 45 control bees per each of the two selected backcross colonies were analyzed using sequencetagged site (STS) primers in the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) to amplify a marker linked to sting1. DNA was extracted from individual workers (Hunt, 1997) and dissolved in 300 L of H 2 O. Two microliters were used in PCR with primers that amplify fragments linked to sting1. One set of primers amplified stsn The other set amplified a fragment, sts , that was previously mapped to 8.5 cm distal from the most likely location for sting1 (Hunt et al., 1998). The

5 Genetics and Honeybee Defensive Behavior 99 primer sequences for the stsn were 5 CAA ATT AAA GTC TAC ACT AAA AAA and GCG AAG GTT GGT AAA TGG AA. The primers used to amplify the sts locus were 5 GCA GCC CCA TAA CCA AAG AAA AAT and GCA GCC CCA TTC CAT TTT TAG T. The PCR product for stsn was digested with the restriction enzyme, FOK1. This resulted in codominant markers for both loci. The phase of the marker alleles was determined by comparing the markers of the workers (guards, first stingers, and controls) to those of the haploid father of the F 1 queen. Statistical Analyses Data from the three defensive trials for each colony were summed. Analysis of variance and regression analyses were performed on these data. The data from the genotypic frequency of individual guards, first stingers, and control bees from two backcross colonies (colonies 33 and 60) were subjected to chi square analyses. We used a goodness-of-fit test to compare data from different behavioral groups to the 1:1 segregation that we expect for a backcross colony. RESULTS Differences were found among treatment colonies for the number of stings deposited by the bees in leather patches (Fig. 1). Significant associations between the marker linked to sting1 and the likelihood of individuals to guard the colony entrance or to sting the patches were found among individuals of the European backcross genotype, but not among individuals of the Africanized backcross genotype (Table I). Stings in Patches The number of stings deposited by the bees in leather patches, varied significantly among treatments (F 4, , P ; test based on ln (x) transformed data; Fig. 1) from an average of 38 in European colonies to an average of 136 in Africanized backcross colonies. There were no significant differences among F 1, Africanized backcross, and Africanized colonies for numbers of stings. European backcross colonies were different from European but not different from F 1 colonies. However, the backcross European colonies were also different from Africanized and backcross Africanized colonies. We compared responses generated by the different treatments with the expected values of the two proposed models. Results more closely resembled a model Fig. 1. Number of stings per minute for European (P. EU), African- (B. AF), and Euro- ized (P. AF), F 1 hybrid, Africanized backcross pean backcross (B. EU) colonies. Expected values for the genetic ad- presented as thin lines. ditivity and genetic dominance models are Actual values are presented by open boxes and SE bars, connected with thick lines. Different letters indicate significant differences based on protected LSD tests of ln (x) transformed data. of genetic dominance (Equation 1, Fig. 1). Results differed from the linear, additive model, as demonstrated by a significant quadratic regression coefficient ( , P 0.005). Additionally, the expected values of the F 1 and backcross colonies clearly lie outside of the 95% confidence intervals. Degree of Dominance The degree of dominance for the number of stings left by the bees in the leather patches, calculated with Table I. Number of Homozygous European (E/E), Heterozygous (A/E), and Homozygous Africanized (A/A) First-stinger Bees, Guard Bees, and Control Bees for Marker N4-.245, from a European Backcross Colony (B. EU), and from an Africanized Backcross Colony (B. AF). Africanized Alleles were Significantly Overrepresented in bees from B. EU, but No Significant (ns) Overrepresentation was Recorded in Bees from B. AF or from Control Samples. Colony n A/A A/E E/E Chi-Square P B. EU (stingers) B. EU (guards) B. EU (control) ns B. AF (stingers) ns B. AF (guards) ns B. AF (control) ns

6 100 Guzmán-Novoa, Hunt, Uribe, Smith, and Arechavaleta-Velasco Equation 3, was estimated at 84.3%, 200.8%, and 145.8% for F 1, backcross European, and backcross Africanized colonies, respectively. Guard and First Stinger Bees The proportions of heterozygous (carrying both the Africanized and European parental alleles for marker N4-.245) versus homozygous (carrying only European parental alleles for marker N4-.245) guards and first stinger bees from the European backcross colony did not fit a 1:1 segregation (P 0.01 and P 0.05, respectively; Table I). Heterozygous bees from the European backcross colony were 21.3% and 27.7% more likely to be first stingers and guards, respectively, than bees of pure European genotype. But the proportions of heterozygous versus homozygous guard and first stinger bees from the Africanized backcross colony were not different from the 1:1 expected segregation (Table I). In the European backcross family, the map distance between stsn and the linked marker, sts , was 21.9 cm and 9.1 cm in the first stingers and guards, respectively. Previous estimates for map distance were about 8 cm. The increased distance in the first-stingers sample was probably caused by errors in scoring sts , because this marker was more difficult to read on gels. However, the linkage of the markers confirms that we detected the same loci in these new populations of bees. DISCUSSION Results from both behavioral assays and DNA analyses demonstrate dominance affecting the defensive behavior of honeybee colonies. Dominance for the highly defensive colony phenotype is inferred from the nearly identical responses of F 1, Africanized backcross, and Africanized colonies. Moreover, the responses of European backcross colonies were not different from those of F 1 colonies, even though they differed from the two parental types. The responses of the experimental colonies resembled much more the expected values of the dominance model than the expected values of the additive model. In fact, the responses of backcross colonies suggested a degree of overdominance. The backcross to the Africanized parental colony had a higher mean stinging value than predicted by the dominance model, even though this was not statistically significant. The nonlinear relationship between the colony treatments and their responses may have been due to both genetic dominance and genotypic interactions between individuals within colonies. Guzmán-Novoa and Page (1994a) showed that co-fostered F 1 bees and European bees interact to alter individual likelihood to sting. Their data did not show a nonadditive interaction between the two genotypes when looking at individual stinging behavior. However, interactions between individuals may result in nonadditive effects when measured as a colony-level response. Genetic nonadditivity is also supported by our results with individual bees, showing a higher likelihood for guarding and first stinging for workers carrying the Africanized allele for marker N in a European backcross colony. But in the Africanized backcross colony, heterozygous and homozygous Africanized genotypes for marker N did not differ in their likelihood for guarding and first stinging, as would be expected if large dominance effects for the locus sting1 exist. An alternative explanation for these results is that the two F 1 queens of these colonies had different genotypes for sting1. Although very unlikely, this situation could have occurred if the Africanized queen-mother used for the F 1 cross was not fixed for high-defensive alleles of sting1 and each F 1 daughter inherited a different allele. Other studies also support the hypothesis of genetic dominance for high stinging behavior (Stort, 1975c; Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1993, 1994a; De- Grandi-Hoffman et al., 1998), whereas Collins et al. (1988) suggested that the defensive behavior of honeybees was additive in inheritance, based on their observations of intermediate response of colonies with European queens that had been openly mated in an Africanized region. The lack of agreement of their results with ours may be due to their lack of control over mating or to differences in the sources of European or Africanized bees. Guzmán-Novoa and Page (1994a) hypothesized that behavioral dominance would occur for speed of stinging. Behavioral dominance may occur if an individual that has a low threshold of response to a specific stimulus performs a behavior elicited by the stimulus, thereby altering the probability that less sensitive individuals would perform that behavior (Page and Robinson, 1991). Therefore, worker bees with lower thresholds of response (presumably those carrying the Africanized allele for sting1) to the defensive stimulus (moving leather patch) are expected to be more likely to be the first to sting, and their actions may influence the behavior of nestmates through visual and pheromonal stimuli. This study is the first to confirm the effect of the locus sting1 on the defensive behavior of honeybees.

7 Genetics and Honeybee Defensive Behavior 101 Hunt et al. (1998) identified this locus affecting the stinging behavior of honeybees at the colony level, but the present study indicates this QTL also influences individual defensive behavior. These results agree with studies that demonstrate a relationship between guarding behavior and the relative defensiveness of a colony (Moore et al., 1987; Breed et al., 1988, 1990; Breed and Rogers, 1991; Hunt, unpublished data). In addition, the data provide strong evidence for the existence of genetic dominance effects in the defensive behavior of honeybees at both the colony and the individual levels. These results suggest that the production of less defensive stocks through selection would be relatively slow. Artificial introgression of European genes into honeybee populations (by frequent requeening and importation of drone sources) or maintenance of low levels of Africanization through selective breeding and management may be necessary to reduce stinging incidents. Despite the stated difficulties, the potential success of such breeding and management techniques was demonstrated in a 5-year study conducted with more than 3,000 colonies of bees in an Africanized area (Guzmán-Novoa and Page, 1999). Colonies reduced their defensiveness by more than 50%, on average, in response to frequent requeening and selection for European morphometrics, although not to the levels of European bee colonies. Although these studies showed a connection between colony-level and individual behavioral phenotype and QTL genotype, the genetic dominance at the colony level remains unexplained. Understanding the nature of this dominance can only come from data from further experiments to study interactions between individuals of different genotypes. Once we have data on how an individual s response thresholds are influenced by other individuals, we can construct more meaningful models to further our understanding of why Africanized bees have been very successful at colonizing new areas, how to breed gentler bees, and how to reduce human and animal fatalities. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank J. Antonio Zozaya, Adriana Correa, Daniel Prieto, Angel Rodríguez, José Calvo, Froylán Gutiérrez, and Enrique Estrada, for their assistance. Guillermo García, owner of Vita-Real, provided laboratory space. This study was funded by a grant from the U.S. National Institutes of Health (R29 GM54850) and by a grant from the National Autonomous University of Mexico (PAPIIT IN ). REFERENCES Adams, J., Rothman, E. D., Kerr, W. E., and Paulino, Z. L. (1977). Estimation of the number of sex alleles and queen matings from diploid male frequencies in a population of Apis mellifera. Genetics 86: Breed, M. D., and Rogers, K. B. (1991). The behavioral genetics of colony defense in honeybees: Genetic variability for guarding behavior. Behav. Genet. 21: Breed, M. D., Rogers, K. B., Hunley, J. A., and Moore, A. J. (1988). A correlation between guard behavior and defensive response in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 37: Breed, M. D., Robinson, G. E., and Page, R. E. (1990). Division of labor during honeybee colony defense. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27: Cajero, A. S. (1995). Achievements and actions of the national program for the control of the African bee. Proc. IX Seminario Americano Apicultura, Colima, Mexico (1995). Calderone, N. W., and Page, R. E. (1988). Genotypic variability in age polyethism and task specialisation in the honeybee, Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 22: Calderone, N. W., and Page, R. E. (1991). The evolutionary genetics of division of labor in colonies of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Am. Nat. 138: Calderone, N. W., and Page, R. E. (1992). Effects of interactions among genetically diverse nestmates on task specialisation by foraging honeybees (Apis mellifera). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 30: Collins, A. M. (1986). Bidirectional selection for colony defense in Africanized honeybees. Am. Bee J. 126: Collins, A. M., Rinderer, T. E., Harbo, J. R., and Bolten, A. B. (1982). Colony defense by Africanized and European honeybees. Science 218: Collins, A. M., Rinderer, T. E., Harbo, J. R., and Brown, M. A. (1984). Heritabilities and correlations for several characters in the honeybee. J. Hered. 75: Collins, A. M., Rinderer, T. E., and Tucker, K. W. (1988). Colony defense of two honeybee types and their hybrids. I. Naturally mated queens. J. Apic. Res. 27: Crozier R. H., Consul P. C. (1976). Conditions for genetic polymorphisms in social Hymenoptera under selection at the colony level. Theor Popul Biol 10:1 9. DeGrandi-Hoffman, G., Collins, A. M., Martin, J. H., Schmidt, J. O., and Spangler, H. G. (1998). Nest defense behavior in colonies from crosses between Africanized and European honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Insect Behav. 11: Gary, N. E., and Lorenzen, K. (1987). Vacuum device for collecting and dispensing honeybees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) and other insects into small cages. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 80: Giray, T., Guzmán-Novoa, E., Aron, C. W., Zelinsky, B., Fahrbach, S. E., and Robinson, G. E. (2000). Genetic variation in worker temporal polyethism and colony defensiveness in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Behav. Ecol. 11: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Gary, N. E. (1993). Genotypic variability of components of foraging behavior in honeybees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 86: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Page, R. E. (1993). Backcrossing Africanized honeybee queens to European drones reduces colony defensive behavior. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 86: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Page, R. E. (1994a). Genetic dominance and worker interactions affect honeybee colony defense. Behav. Ecol. 5: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Page, R. E. (1994b). The impact of africanized bees on Mexican beekeeping. Am. Bee J. 134: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Page, R. E. (1999). Selective breeding of honeybees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) in Africanized areas. J. Econ. Entomol. 92: Guzmán-Novoa, E., and Page, R. E., and Gary, N. E. (1994). Be-

8 102 Guzmán-Novoa, Hunt, Uribe, Smith, and Arechavaleta-Velasco havioral and life-history components of division of labor in honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 34: Guzmán-Novoa, E., Page, R. E., Spangler, H. G., and Erickson, E. H. (1999). A comparison of two assays to test the defensive behaviour of honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Apic. Res. 38: Hall, H. G. (1990). Parental analysis of introgressive hybridization between African and European honeybees using nuclear DNA RFLPs. Genetics 125: Hall, H. G. (1992a). DNA studies reveal processes involved in the spread of new world African honeybees. Florida Entomol. 75: Hall, H. G. (1992b). Further characterization of nuclear DNA RFLP markers that distinguish African and European honeybees. Arch. Ins. Biochem. Biophys. 19: Hall, H. G., and Muralidharan, K. (1989). Evidence from mitochondrial DNA that African honeybees spread as continuous maternal lineages. Nature 339: Hall, H. G., and Smith, D. R. (1991). Distinguishing African and European honeybee matrilines using amplified mitochondrial DNA. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 88: Hellmich, R. L., Kulincevic, J. M., and Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1985). Selection for high and low pollen-hoarding honeybees. J. Hered. 76: Hillesheim, E., Koeniger, N., and Moritz, R. F. A. (1989). Colony performance in honeybees (Apis mellifera capensis Esch.) depends on the proportion of subordinate and dominant workers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: Hunt, G. J. (1997). Insect DNA extraction protocol. In Micheli M. R. and R. Bova, eds. Fingerprinting Methods Based on Arbitrarily Primed PCR. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp Hunt, G. J., Page, R. E., Fondrk, M. K., and Dullum, C. J. (1995). Major quantitative trait loci affecting honeybee foraging behavior. Genetics 141: Hunt, G. J., Guzmán-Novoa, E., Fondrk, M. K., and Page, R. E. (1998). Quantitative trait loci for honeybee stinging behavior and body size. Genetics 148: Hunt, G. J., Collins, A. M., Rivera, R., Page, R. E., and Guzmán- Novoa, E. (1999). Quantitative trait loci influencing honeybee alarm pheromone levels. J. Hered. 90: Kerr, W. E. (1967). The history of the introduction of African bees to Brazil. S. Afr. Bee J. 39:3 5. Lobo, J. A., Lama, M. A. D., and Mestriner, M. A. (1989). Population differentiation and racial admixture in the Africanized honeybee (Apis mellifera L.). Evolution 43: Moffett, J. O., Maki, D. L., Andre, T., and Fierro, M. M. (1987). The Africanized bee in Chiapas, México. Am. Bee J. 127: Moore, A. J., Breed, M. D., and Moor, M. J. (1987). The guard honeybee: Ontogeny and behavioral variability of workers performing a specialized task. Anim. Behav. 35: Moritz, R. F. A., and Hillesheim, E. (1985). Inheritance of dominance in honeybees (Apis mellifera capensis Esch.). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17: Moritz, R. F. A., and Meusel, M. S. (1992). Mitochondrial gene frequencies in Africanized honeybees (Apis mellifera L.): Theoretical model and empirical evidence. J. Evol. Biol. 5: Muralidharan, K., and Hall, H. G. (1990). Prevalence of African DNA RFLP alleles in neotropical African honeybees. Arch. Ins. Biochem. Biophys. 15: Nielsen, D. I., Ebert, P. R., Page, R. E., Hunt, G. J., and Guzmán-Novoa, E. (2000). Improved polymerase chain reaction based mitochondrial genotype assay for identification of the Africanized honeybee (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 93:1 6. Owen, R. E. (1986). Colony-level selection in the social insects: Single locus additive and non-additive models. Theor. Popul. Biol. 29: Page, R. E. (1986). Sperm utilization in social insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 31: Page, R. E., and Laidlaw, H. H. (1988). Full sisters and super sisters: A terminological paradigm. Anim. Behav. 36: Page, R. E., and Robinson, G. E. (1991). The genetics of the division of labour in honeybee colonies. Adv. Insect Physiol. 23: Page, R. E., Jr., Waddington, K. D., Hunt, G. J., and Fondrk, M. K. (1995). Genetic determinants of honeybee foraging behavior. Anim. Behav. 50: Page, R. E., Jr., Fondrk, M. K., Hunt G. J., Guzmán-Novoa, E., Humphries, M. A., Nguyen, K., and Greene, A. (2000). Genetic dissection of honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) foraging behavior. J. Hered. 91: Quezada-Euán, J. J. G., and Medina, L. M. (1998). Hybridization between European and Africanized honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) in tropical Yucatan, Mexico: I. Morphometric changes in feral and managed colonies. Apidologie 29: Ribbands, C. R. (1954). The defence of the honeybee community. Proc. Roy. Soc. London (B) 142: Rinderer, T. E., Steltzer, J. A., Oldroyd, B. P., Buco, S. M., and Rubink, W. L. (1991). Hybridization between European and Africanized honeybees in the neotropical Yucatan Peninsula. Science 253: Robinson, G. E., and Page, R. E., and Page, R. E. (1988). Genetic determination of guarding and undertaking in honeybee colonies. Nature 333: Robinson, G. E., and Page, R. E. (1989). Genetic determination of nectar foraging, pollen foraging, and nest-site scouting in honeybee colonies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: Robinson, G. E., Page, R. E., and Fondrk, M. K. (1990). Intracolonial behavioral variation in worker oviposition, oophagy, and larval care in queenless honeybee colonies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 26: Rothenbuhler, W. C., and Page, R. E. (1989). Genetic variability for temporal polyethism in colonies consisting in similarly-aged worker honeybees. Apidologie 29: Seeley, T. D. (1989). The honeybee colony as a superorganism. Am. Sci. 77: Sheppard, W. S., Soares, A. E. E., and DeJong, D. (1991). Hybrid status of honeybee populations near the historic origin of Africanization in Brazil. Apidologie 22: Smith, D. R., Taylor, O. R., and Brown, W. M. (1989). Neotropical Africanized honeybees have African mitochondrial DNA. Nature 339: Stort, A. C. (1975a). Genetic study of the aggressiveness of two subspecies of Apis mellifera in Brazil. II. Time at which the first sting reached the leather ball. J. Apic. Res. 14: Stort, A. C. (1975b). Genetic study of the aggressiveness of two subspecies of Apis mellifera in Brazil. IV. Number of stings in the gloves of the observer. Behav. Genet. 5: Stort, A. C. (1975c). Genetic study of the aggressiveness of two subspecies of Apis mellifera in Brazil. V. Number of stings in the leather ball. J. Kans. Entomol. Soc. 48: Sugden, E. A., and Williams, K. R. (1991). October 15: The day the bee arrived. Gle. Bee Cult. 119: Sylvester, H. A., and Rinderer, T. E. (1987). Fast africanized bee identification system (FABIS) manual. Am. Bee J. 127: Villa, J. D. (1988). Defensive behaviour of Africanized and European honeybees at two elevations in Colombia. J. Apic. Res. 27: Winston, M. L. (1987). The Biology of the Honeybee. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Edited by Steven Maxson

Basics of Marker Assisted Selection

Basics of Marker Assisted Selection asics of Marker ssisted Selection Chapter 15 asics of Marker ssisted Selection Julius van der Werf, Department of nimal Science rian Kinghorn, Twynam Chair of nimal reeding Technologies University of New

More information

Chapter 9 Patterns of Inheritance

Chapter 9 Patterns of Inheritance Bio 100 Patterns of Inheritance 1 Chapter 9 Patterns of Inheritance Modern genetics began with Gregor Mendel s quantitative experiments with pea plants History of Heredity Blending theory of heredity -

More information

Castes in Social Insects

Castes in Social Insects Castes in Social Insects Module Chemical Ecology, Ayasse Outline Castes in Social Insects 1. definition 2. castes in social insects 3. caste determination A. psychophysiological caste determination B.

More information

Forensic DNA Testing Terminology

Forensic DNA Testing Terminology Forensic DNA Testing Terminology ABI 310 Genetic Analyzer a capillary electrophoresis instrument used by forensic DNA laboratories to separate short tandem repeat (STR) loci on the basis of their size.

More information

Biology 1406 - Notes for exam 5 - Population genetics Ch 13, 14, 15

Biology 1406 - Notes for exam 5 - Population genetics Ch 13, 14, 15 Biology 1406 - Notes for exam 5 - Population genetics Ch 13, 14, 15 Species - group of individuals that are capable of interbreeding and producing fertile offspring; genetically similar 13.7, 14.2 Population

More information

Mendelian Genetics in Drosophila

Mendelian Genetics in Drosophila Mendelian Genetics in Drosophila Lab objectives: 1) To familiarize you with an important research model organism,! Drosophila melanogaster. 2) Introduce you to normal "wild type" and various mutant phenotypes.

More information

Gene Mapping Techniques

Gene Mapping Techniques Gene Mapping Techniques OBJECTIVES By the end of this session the student should be able to: Define genetic linkage and recombinant frequency State how genetic distance may be estimated State how restriction

More information

Queen Management Notes New Hampshire Bee Meeting October 28, 2006

Queen Management Notes New Hampshire Bee Meeting October 28, 2006 Queen Management Notes New Hampshire Bee Meeting October 28, 2006 By Dan Conlon Warm Colors Apiary South Deerfield, Massachusetts warmcolors@verizon.net 413-665-4513 www.warmcolorsapiary.com Understanding

More information

Biology Behind the Crime Scene Week 4: Lab #4 Genetics Exercise (Meiosis) and RFLP Analysis of DNA

Biology Behind the Crime Scene Week 4: Lab #4 Genetics Exercise (Meiosis) and RFLP Analysis of DNA Page 1 of 5 Biology Behind the Crime Scene Week 4: Lab #4 Genetics Exercise (Meiosis) and RFLP Analysis of DNA Genetics Exercise: Understanding how meiosis affects genetic inheritance and DNA patterns

More information

A trait is a variation of a particular character (e.g. color, height). Traits are passed from parents to offspring through genes.

A trait is a variation of a particular character (e.g. color, height). Traits are passed from parents to offspring through genes. 1 Biology Chapter 10 Study Guide Trait A trait is a variation of a particular character (e.g. color, height). Traits are passed from parents to offspring through genes. Genes Genes are located on chromosomes

More information

Biology Final Exam Study Guide: Semester 2

Biology Final Exam Study Guide: Semester 2 Biology Final Exam Study Guide: Semester 2 Questions 1. Scientific method: What does each of these entail? Investigation and Experimentation Problem Hypothesis Methods Results/Data Discussion/Conclusion

More information

Mendelian and Non-Mendelian Heredity Grade Ten

Mendelian and Non-Mendelian Heredity Grade Ten Ohio Standards Connection: Life Sciences Benchmark C Explain the genetic mechanisms and molecular basis of inheritance. Indicator 6 Explain that a unit of hereditary information is called a gene, and genes

More information

The correct answer is c A. Answer a is incorrect. The white-eye gene must be recessive since heterozygous females have red eyes.

The correct answer is c A. Answer a is incorrect. The white-eye gene must be recessive since heterozygous females have red eyes. 1. Why is the white-eye phenotype always observed in males carrying the white-eye allele? a. Because the trait is dominant b. Because the trait is recessive c. Because the allele is located on the X chromosome

More information

2. True or False? The sequence of nucleotides in the human genome is 90.9% identical from one person to the next. False (it s 99.

2. True or False? The sequence of nucleotides in the human genome is 90.9% identical from one person to the next. False (it s 99. 1. True or False? A typical chromosome can contain several hundred to several thousand genes, arranged in linear order along the DNA molecule present in the chromosome. True 2. True or False? The sequence

More information

DNA and Forensic Science

DNA and Forensic Science DNA and Forensic Science Micah A. Luftig * Stephen Richey ** I. INTRODUCTION This paper represents a discussion of the fundamental principles of DNA technology as it applies to forensic testing. A brief

More information

INTERMEDIATE BEEKEEPING WEEK 1 CAROL COTTRILL

INTERMEDIATE BEEKEEPING WEEK 1 CAROL COTTRILL INTERMEDIATE BEEKEEPING WEEK 1 CAROL COTTRILL BEE BIOLOGY WHY IT MATTERS There is a spectrum there are different ways to try to do things. I once thought you could do it all by a cookbook, and now I think

More information

The Genetics of Drosophila melanogaster

The Genetics of Drosophila melanogaster The Genetics of Drosophila melanogaster Thomas Hunt Morgan, a geneticist who worked in the early part of the twentieth century, pioneered the use of the common fruit fly as a model organism for genetic

More information

Heredity. Sarah crosses a homozygous white flower and a homozygous purple flower. The cross results in all purple flowers.

Heredity. Sarah crosses a homozygous white flower and a homozygous purple flower. The cross results in all purple flowers. Heredity 1. Sarah is doing an experiment on pea plants. She is studying the color of the pea plants. Sarah has noticed that many pea plants have purple flowers and many have white flowers. Sarah crosses

More information

Cloake Board Method of Queen Rearing and Banking

Cloake Board Method of Queen Rearing and Banking Cloake Board Method of Queen Rearing and Banking Rearing your own queens is one of the more interesting and rewarding aspects of beekeeping. It provides a means to maintain young, vigorous queens in colonies

More information

Marker-Assisted Backcrossing. Marker-Assisted Selection. 1. Select donor alleles at markers flanking target gene. Losing the target allele

Marker-Assisted Backcrossing. Marker-Assisted Selection. 1. Select donor alleles at markers flanking target gene. Losing the target allele Marker-Assisted Backcrossing Marker-Assisted Selection CS74 009 Jim Holland Target gene = Recurrent parent allele = Donor parent allele. Select donor allele at markers linked to target gene.. Select recurrent

More information

Biology 1406 Exam 4 Notes Cell Division and Genetics Ch. 8, 9

Biology 1406 Exam 4 Notes Cell Division and Genetics Ch. 8, 9 Biology 1406 Exam 4 Notes Cell Division and Genetics Ch. 8, 9 Ch. 8 Cell Division Cells divide to produce new cells must pass genetic information to new cells - What process of DNA allows this? Two types

More information

Terms: The following terms are presented in this lesson (shown in bold italics and on PowerPoint Slides 2 and 3):

Terms: The following terms are presented in this lesson (shown in bold italics and on PowerPoint Slides 2 and 3): Unit B: Understanding Animal Reproduction Lesson 4: Understanding Genetics Student Learning Objectives: Instruction in this lesson should result in students achieving the following objectives: 1. Explain

More information

MCB41: Second Midterm Spring 2009

MCB41: Second Midterm Spring 2009 MCB41: Second Midterm Spring 2009 Before you start, print your name and student identification number (S.I.D) at the top of each page. There are 7 pages including this page. You will have 50 minutes for

More information

Simulation Model of Mating Behavior in Flies

Simulation Model of Mating Behavior in Flies Simulation Model of Mating Behavior in Flies MEHMET KAYIM & AYKUT Ecological and Evolutionary Genetics Lab. Department of Biology, Middle East Technical University International Workshop on Hybrid Systems

More information

I. Genes found on the same chromosome = linked genes

I. Genes found on the same chromosome = linked genes Genetic recombination in Eukaryotes: crossing over, part 1 I. Genes found on the same chromosome = linked genes II. III. Linkage and crossing over Crossing over & chromosome mapping I. Genes found on the

More information

Name: 4. A typical phenotypic ratio for a dihybrid cross is a) 9:1 b) 3:4 c) 9:3:3:1 d) 1:2:1:2:1 e) 6:3:3:6

Name: 4. A typical phenotypic ratio for a dihybrid cross is a) 9:1 b) 3:4 c) 9:3:3:1 d) 1:2:1:2:1 e) 6:3:3:6 Name: Multiple-choice section Choose the answer which best completes each of the following statements or answers the following questions and so make your tutor happy! 1. Which of the following conclusions

More information

LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Period Date LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Why do we study random chance and probability at the beginning of a unit on genetics? Genetics is the study of inheritance,

More information

Answer Key Problem Set 5

Answer Key Problem Set 5 7.03 Fall 2003 1 of 6 1. a) Genetic properties of gln2- and gln 3-: Answer Key Problem Set 5 Both are uninducible, as they give decreased glutamine synthetase (GS) activity. Both are recessive, as mating

More information

AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Ms. Foglia Date AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Why do we study random chance and probability at the beginning of a unit on genetics? Genetics is the study of inheritance,

More information

SEASONAL APLICATION OF JENTER S METHOD FOR A SUCCESSFUL QUEEN BEES REARING IN ALBANIA

SEASONAL APLICATION OF JENTER S METHOD FOR A SUCCESSFUL QUEEN BEES REARING IN ALBANIA SEASONAL APLICATION OF JENTER S METHOD FOR A SUCCESSFUL QUEEN BEES REARING IN ALBANIA L. Sena, S. Sena, F. Gjurgji and M. Nikolla Department of Animal Production, Faculty of Agriculture and Environment,

More information

Genetics Lecture Notes 7.03 2005. Lectures 1 2

Genetics Lecture Notes 7.03 2005. Lectures 1 2 Genetics Lecture Notes 7.03 2005 Lectures 1 2 Lecture 1 We will begin this course with the question: What is a gene? This question will take us four lectures to answer because there are actually several

More information

17. A testcross A.is used to determine if an organism that is displaying a recessive trait is heterozygous or homozygous for that trait. B.

17. A testcross A.is used to determine if an organism that is displaying a recessive trait is heterozygous or homozygous for that trait. B. ch04 Student: 1. Which of the following does not inactivate an X chromosome? A. Mammals B. Drosophila C. C. elegans D. Humans 2. Who originally identified a highly condensed structure in the interphase

More information

Second Grade Insects Assessment

Second Grade Insects Assessment Second Grade Insects Assessment 1a. The stiff shell that covers an insect s body is called an: a. outer shell b. exoskeleton 1b. The stiff shell that covers and insect s body is called an: a. outer shell

More information

Name: Class: Date: ID: A

Name: Class: Date: ID: A Name: Class: _ Date: _ Meiosis Quiz 1. (1 point) A kidney cell is an example of which type of cell? a. sex cell b. germ cell c. somatic cell d. haploid cell 2. (1 point) How many chromosomes are in a human

More information

GENOMIC SELECTION: THE FUTURE OF MARKER ASSISTED SELECTION AND ANIMAL BREEDING

GENOMIC SELECTION: THE FUTURE OF MARKER ASSISTED SELECTION AND ANIMAL BREEDING GENOMIC SELECTION: THE FUTURE OF MARKER ASSISTED SELECTION AND ANIMAL BREEDING Theo Meuwissen Institute for Animal Science and Aquaculture, Box 5025, 1432 Ås, Norway, theo.meuwissen@ihf.nlh.no Summary

More information

LAB : PAPER PET GENETICS. male (hat) female (hair bow) Skin color green or orange Eyes round or square Nose triangle or oval Teeth pointed or square

LAB : PAPER PET GENETICS. male (hat) female (hair bow) Skin color green or orange Eyes round or square Nose triangle or oval Teeth pointed or square Period Date LAB : PAPER PET GENETICS 1. Given the list of characteristics below, you will create an imaginary pet and then breed it to review the concepts of genetics. Your pet will have the following

More information

Heredity - Patterns of Inheritance

Heredity - Patterns of Inheritance Heredity - Patterns of Inheritance Genes and Alleles A. Genes 1. A sequence of nucleotides that codes for a special functional product a. Transfer RNA b. Enzyme c. Structural protein d. Pigments 2. Genes

More information

Genetics Module B, Anchor 3

Genetics Module B, Anchor 3 Genetics Module B, Anchor 3 Key Concepts: - An individual s characteristics are determines by factors that are passed from one parental generation to the next. - During gamete formation, the alleles for

More information

Mitochondrial DNA Analysis

Mitochondrial DNA Analysis Mitochondrial DNA Analysis Lineage Markers Lineage markers are passed down from generation to generation without changing Except for rare mutation events They can help determine the lineage (family tree)

More information

DNA Determines Your Appearance!

DNA Determines Your Appearance! DNA Determines Your Appearance! Summary DNA contains all the information needed to build your body. Did you know that your DNA determines things such as your eye color, hair color, height, and even the

More information

Chapter 8: Recombinant DNA 2002 by W. H. Freeman and Company Chapter 8: Recombinant DNA 2002 by W. H. Freeman and Company

Chapter 8: Recombinant DNA 2002 by W. H. Freeman and Company Chapter 8: Recombinant DNA 2002 by W. H. Freeman and Company Genetic engineering: humans Gene replacement therapy or gene therapy Many technical and ethical issues implications for gene pool for germ-line gene therapy what traits constitute disease rather than just

More information

PRACTICE PROBLEMS - PEDIGREES AND PROBABILITIES

PRACTICE PROBLEMS - PEDIGREES AND PROBABILITIES PRACTICE PROBLEMS - PEDIGREES AND PROBABILITIES 1. Margaret has just learned that she has adult polycystic kidney disease. Her mother also has the disease, as did her maternal grandfather and his younger

More information

What is a Honey Bee? What is an Africanized Honey Bee?

What is a Honey Bee? What is an Africanized Honey Bee? What is a Honey Bee? Honey bees are not native to the North America Honey bees currently pollinate about 90 agricultural crops (accounting for 80% of the pollination in the US): ~ $10 billion pollination

More information

MODEL BEEKEEPING ORDINANCE

MODEL BEEKEEPING ORDINANCE MODEL BEEKEEPING ORDINANCE For Florida (Local and Municipal) Governments Whereas, honey bees are beneficial to mankind and to Florida in particular, by providing agricultural fruit and vegetable pollination

More information

Lecture 6: Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and Restriction Fragment Length Polymorphisms (RFLPs)

Lecture 6: Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and Restriction Fragment Length Polymorphisms (RFLPs) Lecture 6: Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and Restriction Fragment Length Polymorphisms (RFLPs) Single nucleotide polymorphisms or SNPs (pronounced "snips") are DNA sequence variations that occur

More information

Bio EOC Topics for Cell Reproduction: Bio EOC Questions for Cell Reproduction:

Bio EOC Topics for Cell Reproduction: Bio EOC Questions for Cell Reproduction: Bio EOC Topics for Cell Reproduction: Asexual vs. sexual reproduction Mitosis steps, diagrams, purpose o Interphase, Prophase, Metaphase, Anaphase, Telophase, Cytokinesis Meiosis steps, diagrams, purpose

More information

Investigating the genetic basis for intelligence

Investigating the genetic basis for intelligence Investigating the genetic basis for intelligence Steve Hsu University of Oregon and BGI www.cog-genomics.org Outline: a multidisciplinary subject 1. What is intelligence? Psychometrics 2. g and GWAS: a

More information

AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic

AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic Background The Essential Knowledge statements provided in the AP Biology Curriculum Framework are scientific claims describing phenomenon occurring in

More information

Michel, GF. Right handedness: A consequence of infant supine head orientation preference? Science 1981; 212:685-687.

Michel, GF. Right handedness: A consequence of infant supine head orientation preference? Science 1981; 212:685-687. Right-Handedness: A Consequence of Infant Supine Head-Orientation Preference? By: George F. Michel Michel, GF. Right handedness: A consequence of infant supine head orientation preference? Science 1981;

More information

MOLECULAR MARKERS AND THEIR APPLICATIONS IN CEREALS BREEDING

MOLECULAR MARKERS AND THEIR APPLICATIONS IN CEREALS BREEDING MOLECULAR MARKERS AND THEIR APPLICATIONS IN CEREALS BREEDING Viktor Korzun Lochow-Petkus GmbH, Grimsehlstr.24, 37574 Einbeck, Germany korzun@lochow-petkus.de Summary The development of molecular techniques

More information

Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1

Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1 Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1 Chapter in Review 1. Heredity is the tendency of offspring to resemble their parents in various ways. Genes are units of heredity. They are functional strands of DNA grouped

More information

Genetics for the Novice

Genetics for the Novice Genetics for the Novice by Carol Barbee Wait! Don't leave yet. I know that for many breeders any article with the word genetics in the title causes an immediate negative reaction. Either they quickly turn

More information

CHAPTER 15 THE CHROMOSOMAL BASIS OF INHERITANCE. Section B: Sex Chromosomes

CHAPTER 15 THE CHROMOSOMAL BASIS OF INHERITANCE. Section B: Sex Chromosomes CHAPTER 15 THE CHROMOSOMAL BASIS OF INHERITANCE Section B: Sex Chromosomes 1. The chromosomal basis of sex varies with the organism 2. Sex-linked genes have unique patterns of inheritance 1. The chromosomal

More information

PRINCIPLES OF POPULATION GENETICS

PRINCIPLES OF POPULATION GENETICS PRINCIPLES OF POPULATION GENETICS FOURTH EDITION Daniel L. Hartl Harvard University Andrew G. Clark Cornell University UniversitSts- und Landesbibliothek Darmstadt Bibliothek Biologie Sinauer Associates,

More information

(1-p) 2. p(1-p) From the table, frequency of DpyUnc = ¼ (p^2) = #DpyUnc = p^2 = 0.0004 ¼(1-p)^2 + ½(1-p)p + ¼(p^2) #Dpy + #DpyUnc

(1-p) 2. p(1-p) From the table, frequency of DpyUnc = ¼ (p^2) = #DpyUnc = p^2 = 0.0004 ¼(1-p)^2 + ½(1-p)p + ¼(p^2) #Dpy + #DpyUnc Advanced genetics Kornfeld problem set_key 1A (5 points) Brenner employed 2-factor and 3-factor crosses with the mutants isolated from his screen, and visually assayed for recombination events between

More information

Influence of Sex on Genetics. Chapter Six

Influence of Sex on Genetics. Chapter Six Influence of Sex on Genetics Chapter Six Humans 23 Autosomes Chromosomal abnormalities very severe Often fatal All have at least one X Deletion of X chromosome is fatal Males = heterogametic sex XY Females

More information

Appendix J. Genetic Implications of Recent Biotechnologies. Appendix Contents. Introduction

Appendix J. Genetic Implications of Recent Biotechnologies. Appendix Contents. Introduction Genetic Improvement and Crossbreeding in Meat Goats Lessons in Animal Breeding for Goats Bred and Raised for Meat Will R. Getz Fort Valley State University Appendix J. Genetic Implications of Recent Biotechnologies

More information

Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010

Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010 Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010 Week 1 Week 2 14.0 Students organize and describe distributions of data by using a number of different

More information

5 GENETIC LINKAGE AND MAPPING

5 GENETIC LINKAGE AND MAPPING 5 GENETIC LINKAGE AND MAPPING 5.1 Genetic Linkage So far, we have considered traits that are affected by one or two genes, and if there are two genes, we have assumed that they assort independently. However,

More information

CCR Biology - Chapter 7 Practice Test - Summer 2012

CCR Biology - Chapter 7 Practice Test - Summer 2012 Name: Class: Date: CCR Biology - Chapter 7 Practice Test - Summer 2012 Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. A person who has a disorder caused

More information

Introduction. Swarm Intelligence - Thiemo Krink EVALife Group, Dept. of Computer Science, University of Aarhus

Introduction. Swarm Intelligence - Thiemo Krink EVALife Group, Dept. of Computer Science, University of Aarhus Swarm Intelligence - Thiemo Krink EVALife Group, Dept. of Computer Science, University of Aarhus Why do we need new computing techniques? The computer revolution changed human societies: communication

More information

TEXAS A&M PLANT BREEDING BULLETIN

TEXAS A&M PLANT BREEDING BULLETIN TEXAS A&M PLANT BREEDING BULLETIN October 2015 Our Mission: Educate and develop Plant Breeders worldwide Our Vision: Alleviate hunger and poverty through genetic improvement of plants A group of 54 graduate

More information

Chapter 13: Meiosis and Sexual Life Cycles

Chapter 13: Meiosis and Sexual Life Cycles Name Period Concept 13.1 Offspring acquire genes from parents by inheriting chromosomes 1. Let s begin with a review of several terms that you may already know. Define: gene locus gamete male gamete female

More information

Variability of hygienic behavior in bee

Variability of hygienic behavior in bee 674 Bulgarian Journal of Agricultural Science, 21 (No 3) 2015, 674-679 Agricultural Academy Variability of hygienic behavior in bee Apis mellifera macedonica C. XONIS 1, A. THRASYVOULOU 1 and H. F. El

More information

Evolution, Natural Selection, and Adaptation

Evolution, Natural Selection, and Adaptation Evolution, Natural Selection, and Adaptation Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. (Theodosius Dobzhansky) Charles Darwin (1809-1882) Voyage of HMS Beagle (1831-1836) Thinking

More information

The Concept of Inclusive Fitness 1 Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Spring 2008

The Concept of Inclusive Fitness 1 Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Spring 2008 The Concept of Inclusive Fitness 1 Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Spring 2008 I. The components of Fitness A. Direct fitness W d, darwinian fitness, W gained by increasing ones own reproduction relative

More information

2016 Bee College Tentative Short Course Descriptions

2016 Bee College Tentative Short Course Descriptions 2016 Bee College Tentative Short Course Descriptions A Beekeeper s Year For starting beekeepers, the first year could be the hardest since you are trying to figure out all the quirks of beekeeping. Come

More information

2 GENETIC DATA ANALYSIS

2 GENETIC DATA ANALYSIS 2.1 Strategies for learning genetics 2 GENETIC DATA ANALYSIS We will begin this lecture by discussing some strategies for learning genetics. Genetics is different from most other biology courses you have

More information

7A The Origin of Modern Genetics

7A The Origin of Modern Genetics Life Science Chapter 7 Genetics of Organisms 7A The Origin of Modern Genetics Genetics the study of inheritance (the study of how traits are inherited through the interactions of alleles) Heredity: the

More information

DNA MARKERS FOR ASEASONALITY AND MILK PRODUCTION IN SHEEP. R. G. Mateescu and M.L. Thonney

DNA MARKERS FOR ASEASONALITY AND MILK PRODUCTION IN SHEEP. R. G. Mateescu and M.L. Thonney DNA MARKERS FOR ASEASONALITY AND MILK PRODUCTION IN SHEEP Introduction R. G. Mateescu and M.L. Thonney Department of Animal Science Cornell University Ithaca, New York Knowledge about genetic markers linked

More information

Evolution (18%) 11 Items Sample Test Prep Questions

Evolution (18%) 11 Items Sample Test Prep Questions Evolution (18%) 11 Items Sample Test Prep Questions Grade 7 (Evolution) 3.a Students know both genetic variation and environmental factors are causes of evolution and diversity of organisms. (pg. 109 Science

More information

5. The cells of a multicellular organism, other than gametes and the germ cells from which it develops, are known as

5. The cells of a multicellular organism, other than gametes and the germ cells from which it develops, are known as 1. True or false? The chi square statistical test is used to determine how well the observed genetic data agree with the expectations derived from a hypothesis. True 2. True or false? Chromosomes in prokaryotic

More information

Chromosomes, Mapping, and the Meiosis Inheritance Connection

Chromosomes, Mapping, and the Meiosis Inheritance Connection Chromosomes, Mapping, and the Meiosis Inheritance Connection Carl Correns 1900 Chapter 13 First suggests central role for chromosomes Rediscovery of Mendel s work Walter Sutton 1902 Chromosomal theory

More information

LAB 11 Drosophila Genetics

LAB 11 Drosophila Genetics LAB 11 Drosophila Genetics Introduction: Drosophila melanogaster, the fruit fly, is an excellent organism for genetics studies because it has simple food requirements, occupies little space, is hardy,

More information

The Techniques of Molecular Biology: Forensic DNA Fingerprinting

The Techniques of Molecular Biology: Forensic DNA Fingerprinting Revised Fall 2011 The Techniques of Molecular Biology: Forensic DNA Fingerprinting The techniques of molecular biology are used to manipulate the structure and function of molecules such as DNA and proteins

More information

CURRICULUM GUIDE. When this Forensics course has been completed successfully, students should be able to:

CURRICULUM GUIDE. When this Forensics course has been completed successfully, students should be able to: CURRICULUM GUIDE NAME OF COURSE: FORENSICS COURSE NUMBER: SCI 40 WRITTEN / REVISED: SEPTEMBER, 2011 LEVEL OF COURSE: REPLACMENT NUMBER OF CREDITS: SIX (6) PREREQUISITES: BIOLOGY GRADE LEVELS OFFERED TO:

More information

Population Genetics and Multifactorial Inheritance 2002

Population Genetics and Multifactorial Inheritance 2002 Population Genetics and Multifactorial Inheritance 2002 Consanguinity Genetic drift Founder effect Selection Mutation rate Polymorphism Balanced polymorphism Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium

More information

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores Appendix D Basic Measurement And Statistics The following information was developed by Steven Rothke, PhD, Department of Psychology, Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago (RIC) and expanded by Mary F. Schmidt,

More information

A and B are not absolutely linked. They could be far enough apart on the chromosome that they assort independently.

A and B are not absolutely linked. They could be far enough apart on the chromosome that they assort independently. Name Section 7.014 Problem Set 5 Please print out this problem set and record your answers on the printed copy. Answers to this problem set are to be turned in to the box outside 68-120 by 5:00pm on Friday

More information

Tuesday 14 May 2013 Morning

Tuesday 14 May 2013 Morning THIS IS A NEW SPECIFICATION H Tuesday 14 May 2013 Morning GCSE TWENTY FIRST CENTURY SCIENCE BIOLOGY A A161/02 Modules B1 B2 B3 (Higher Tier) *A137150613* Candidates answer on the Question Paper. A calculator

More information

Paternity Testing. Chapter 23

Paternity Testing. Chapter 23 Paternity Testing Chapter 23 Kinship and Paternity DNA analysis can also be used for: Kinship testing determining whether individuals are related Paternity testing determining the father of a child Missing

More information

Summary. 16 1 Genes and Variation. 16 2 Evolution as Genetic Change. Name Class Date

Summary. 16 1 Genes and Variation. 16 2 Evolution as Genetic Change. Name Class Date Chapter 16 Summary Evolution of Populations 16 1 Genes and Variation Darwin s original ideas can now be understood in genetic terms. Beginning with variation, we now know that traits are controlled by

More information

1. Why is mitosis alone insufficient for the life cycle of sexually reproducing eukaryotes?

1. Why is mitosis alone insufficient for the life cycle of sexually reproducing eukaryotes? Chapter 13: Meiosis and Sexual Life Cycles 1. Why is mitosis alone insufficient for the life cycle of sexually reproducing eukaryotes? 2. Define: gamete zygote meiosis homologous chromosomes diploid haploid

More information

Basic Principles of Forensic Molecular Biology and Genetics. Population Genetics

Basic Principles of Forensic Molecular Biology and Genetics. Population Genetics Basic Principles of Forensic Molecular Biology and Genetics Population Genetics Significance of a Match What is the significance of: a fiber match? a hair match? a glass match? a DNA match? Meaning of

More information

Process 3.5. A Pour it down the sink. B Pour it back into its original container. C Dispose of it as directed by his teacher.

Process 3.5. A Pour it down the sink. B Pour it back into its original container. C Dispose of it as directed by his teacher. Process 3.5 Biology EOI sample test questions Objective numbers correspond to the State Priority Academic Student Skills (PASS) standards and objectives. This number is also referenced with the local objective

More information

TITRATION OF raav (VG) USING QUANTITATIVE REAL TIME PCR

TITRATION OF raav (VG) USING QUANTITATIVE REAL TIME PCR Page 1 of 5 Materials DNase digestion buffer [13 mm Tris-Cl, ph7,5 / 5 mm MgCl2 / 0,12 mm CaCl2] RSS plasmid ptr-uf11 SV40pA Forward primer (10µM) AGC AAT AGC ATC ACA AAT TTC ACA A SV40pA Reverse Primer

More information

Cystic Fibrosis Webquest Sarah Follenweider, The English High School 2009 Summer Research Internship Program

Cystic Fibrosis Webquest Sarah Follenweider, The English High School 2009 Summer Research Internship Program Cystic Fibrosis Webquest Sarah Follenweider, The English High School 2009 Summer Research Internship Program Introduction: Cystic fibrosis (CF) is an inherited chronic disease that affects the lungs and

More information

COMMENTARIES. Ecology, evolution and division of labour in social insects

COMMENTARIES. Ecology, evolution and division of labour in social insects Anim. Behav., 1997, 53, 209 213 COMMENTARIES Ecology, evolution and division of labour in social insects JAMES F. A. TRANIELLO & REBECA B. ROSENGAUS Department of Biology, Boston University (Received 27

More information

A Primer of Genome Science THIRD

A Primer of Genome Science THIRD A Primer of Genome Science THIRD EDITION GREG GIBSON-SPENCER V. MUSE North Carolina State University Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts USA Contents Preface xi 1 Genome Projects:

More information

Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium Problems

Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium Problems Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium Problems 1. The frequency of two alleles in a gene pool is 0.19 (A) and 0.81(a). Assume that the population is in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium. (a) Calculate the percentage of

More information

Y Chromosome Markers

Y Chromosome Markers Y Chromosome Markers Lineage Markers Autosomal chromosomes recombine with each meiosis Y and Mitochondrial DNA does not This means that the Y and mtdna remains constant from generation to generation Except

More information

Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs)

Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) Additional Markers 13 core STR loci Obtain further information from additional markers: Y STRs Separating male samples Mitochondrial DNA Working with extremely degraded

More information

Rearing queen bees. Introduction. Races of bees. The queen. Mark Johnstone. Project Officer, Honey Bees, Richmond

Rearing queen bees. Introduction. Races of bees. The queen. Mark Johnstone. Project Officer, Honey Bees, Richmond SEPTEMBER 2008 PRIMEFACT 828 Rearing queen bees Mark Johnstone Project Officer, Honey Bees, Richmond Introduction As a beekeeper you may have purchased queen bees from a commercial queen breeder and have

More information

Genetics Test Biology I

Genetics Test Biology I Genetics Test Biology I Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. Avery s experiments showed that bacteria are transformed by a. RNA. c. proteins.

More information

Bio 102 Practice Problems Mendelian Genetics and Extensions

Bio 102 Practice Problems Mendelian Genetics and Extensions Bio 102 Practice Problems Mendelian Genetics and Extensions Short answer (show your work or thinking to get partial credit): 1. In peas, tall is dominant over dwarf. If a plant homozygous for tall is crossed

More information

GENETIC CROSSES. Monohybrid Crosses

GENETIC CROSSES. Monohybrid Crosses GENETIC CROSSES Monohybrid Crosses Objectives Explain the difference between genotype and phenotype Explain the difference between homozygous and heterozygous Explain how probability is used to predict

More information

Genetics 1. Defective enzyme that does not make melanin. Very pale skin and hair color (albino)

Genetics 1. Defective enzyme that does not make melanin. Very pale skin and hair color (albino) Genetics 1 We all know that children tend to resemble their parents. Parents and their children tend to have similar appearance because children inherit genes from their parents and these genes influence

More information

Genomic Selection in. Applied Training Workshop, Sterling. Hans Daetwyler, The Roslin Institute and R(D)SVS

Genomic Selection in. Applied Training Workshop, Sterling. Hans Daetwyler, The Roslin Institute and R(D)SVS Genomic Selection in Dairy Cattle AQUAGENOME Applied Training Workshop, Sterling Hans Daetwyler, The Roslin Institute and R(D)SVS Dairy introduction Overview Traditional breeding Genomic selection Advantages

More information

The H-Map: An Alternative to the Venn Diagram

The H-Map: An Alternative to the Venn Diagram The H-Map: An Alternative to the Venn Diagram H Fossil Fuels 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Both? 1. 2. 3. Solar Power 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Venn Diagram Fossil Fuels Solar Energy Source: Holt Elements of Literature (2005) Word

More information

DNA Sample preparation and Submission Guidelines

DNA Sample preparation and Submission Guidelines DNA Sample preparation and Submission Guidelines Requirements: Please submit samples in 1.5ml microcentrifuge tubes. Fill all the required information in the Eurofins DNA sequencing order form and send

More information