Satiety related to 24 h diet-induced thermogenesis during high protein=carbohydrate vs high fat diets measured in a respiration chamber



Similar documents
Carbohydrate Counting for Patients with Diabetes. Lauren Dorman, MS RD CDE Registered Dietitian & Certified Diabetes Educator

WEIGHT GAINER S NUTRITION GUIDE

My Diabetic Meal Plan during Pregnancy

Try pancakes, waffles, french toast, bagels, cereal, English muffins, fruit or juice. These foods are all high in carbohydrates.

Carbohydrate Counting for Patients With Diabetes. Review Date 4/08 D-0503

Presentation Prepared By: Jessica Rivers, BASc., PTS

UIW ATHLETICS WEIGHT GAIN NUTRITION GUIDE

BETTER PRACTICE IN FOOD AND NUTRITION SERVICES - MENU PLANNING AND MENU DESIGN TO DELIVER OPTIMAL NUTRITION TO RESIDENTIAL AGED CARE

FITTEAM 5. Overview. Keys to Success

MEAL PLANNING FOR MECHANICAL SOFT DIET

Meal Planning for a Mushy Soft Diet After Nissen Fundoplication

Level 3. Applying the Principles of Nutrition to a Physical Activity Programme Level 3

Carbohydrate Counting for Pediatric Patients With Type 1 Diabetes. Review Date 4/08 K-0591

Sedentarity and Exercise in the Canadian Population. Angelo Tremblay Division of kinesiology

High-protein diets in weight management; short and long term effects; significance of protein quality

Meal Planning for a Mushy Soft Diet After Laparoscopic Myotomy

Calories. 23 calories from fat + 48 calories from carbohydrates + 32 calories from protein = 103 Calories in 1 cup of 1% milk

Melt Your Body Fat for Good!

EMBRACE Your Journey Nutrition During Treatment

Healthy Menu Planning

NAME: The measurement of BMR must be performed under very stringent laboratory conditions. For example:

School Nutrition Policy Background

Nutri Lean Lifestyle 30

Material AICLE. 5º de Primaria.: Food and nutrition (Solucionario)

Maintaining Nutrition as We Age

Unit 3L.4: Body Parts and Functions

Canada s Food Guide Jeopardy

Snacking and Gestational Diabetes

Oxford Bariatric Service Pre bariatric surgery diet Information for patients

An introduction to carbohydrate counting

Learning Objectives. ADA Diet vs. Medical Nutrition Therapy. In Diabetes, Food IS Medicine: Current Trends In Diabetes Nutrition Management

Nutrition Education Competencies Aligned with the California Health Education Content Standards

Dietary Composition for Weight Loss and Weight Loss Maintenance

Nutrition and Chronic Kidney Disease

BEST & WORST FOODS FOR BELLY FAT

Food Intake and Social Inequalities

Diet for Oral Surgery/Wired Jaw

1) High Fructose corn syrup 2) Lack of Sleep 3) Over-Eating in Evening / End of day Vegetation (sumo wrestler diet)

Days 1 and 2: Bariatric Clear Liquids

Fertile Food Can you eat your way to pregnancy? Tracy Cherry, RD, CDN University of Rochester Women s Lifestyle Center

Eat More, Weigh Less?

UW MEDICINE PATIENT EDUCATION. PCOS Nutrition. Eat a Balanced Diet

100 Gram Fat Diet for 72 hour Fecal Fat Collection

Gaining Weight for Athletes

Nutrition Consultation Report

Healthy Eating During Pregnancy

Low Fat Diet after Cardiac Surgery With or Without Chyle Leak

Lesson 3 Assessing My Eating Habits

Eating Well with Diabetes. Cassie Vanderwall UW Health Nutrition Registered Dietitian Certified Personal Trainer Certified Diabetes Educator

CORPORATE HEALTH LOWERING YOUR CHOLESTEROL & BLOOD PRESSURE

MINTO PREVENTION & REHABILITATION CENTRE CENTRE DE PREVENTION ET DE READAPTATION MINTO. Counting Fat Grams. About This Kit

Carbohydrate s Role in Fat Loss by Cameron L. Martz, ACSM H/FI

Blood Sugar & Glycaemic Index

gestational diabetes my pregnancy, my baby, and me

Do children with diabetes need a special diet?

DIABETES & HEALTHY EATING

# Starch # Fat # Fruit # Free Foods. # Other Carbohydrates # Fast Foods # Vegetable. # Meat and Meat Substitutes

Nutrients: Carbohydrates, Proteins, and Fats. Chapter 5 Lesson 2

Dietary advice for people starting treatment for Hepatitis C. Information for patients Sheffield Dietetics

Nutritional Guidelines for Roux-en-Y, Sleeve Gastrectomy and Duodenal Switch. Gastric Restrictive Procedures. Phase III Regular Consistency

UTI CAT FOOD COMPARISON CHART

Healthy Eating for Diabetes

Healthy Eating for Diabetes

Bariatric Surgery: Step III Diet

I The THREE types of LIPIDS

Calcium , The Patient Education Institute, Inc. nuf40101 Last reviewed: 02/19/2013 1

Resources for Carbohydrate Counting

Carbohydrate Counting (Quiz Number: Manatee )

(1) Move the glucose from the blood into cells that need the glucose for its source of energy to survive; and,

Selection and Preparation of Foods Management of the Food Budget*

Sinclair Community College, Division of Allied Health Technologies

Calcium and Vitamin D: Important at Every Age

Let s Talk Oils and Fats!

Everything You Need to Know About Sucralose

Nonalcoholic Fatty Liver Disease. Dietary and Lifestyle Guidelines

Guidelines for Offering Healthy Foods at Meetings, Seminars and Catered Events

Registered Trade Mark

Detailed Course Descriptions for the Human Nutrition Program

NIT 5: Food I like. Debbie: Do you want some chocolate, Leo?

Homework Help Heart Disease & Stroke

Water It s Crucial Role in Health. By: James L. Holly, MD

Diabetes Nutrition. Roseville & Sacramento Medical Centers. Health Promotion Department Nutritional Services

February Best Foods for Athletes

Nutrition and Chronic Kidney Disease

Dr. Paul Naughton, Teagasc Dr. Sinéad McCarthy, Teagasc Dr. Mary McCarthy, UCC

week 2 itinerary: CALORIES $25 Scheels Gift Card This week s raffle prize is: Why would you ever want to track calories? Here are some of the benefits

Appendix A Oxymax Calculations

Screening Tests for Gestational Diabetes

Endocrine System: Practice Questions #1

Women and Children s Directorate

Energy in the New Dairy NRC. Maurice L. Eastridge 1 Department of Animal Sciences The Ohio State University

Kidney Stones and Diet

VEGETABLES AND FRUIT Help your child to like them. A guide for parents of young children

Presentation Notes Maintaining a Healthy Digestive System

Welcome to the webinar!

Sports Nutrition for the Youth & High School Athlete

NUTRITION AND HEMODIALYSIS

Chapter 5 DASH Your Way to Weight Loss

The Polyphenol Study. Effect of Dietary Polyphenols on Intestinal Microbiota, Intestinal Inflammation and Metabolic Syndrome

How you can achieve normal blood sugars with diet and insulin. Dr Katharine Morrison IDDT October 2007

Transcription:

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (1999) 53, 495±502 ß 1999 Stockton Press. All rights reserved 0954±3007/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/ejcn during high protein=carbohydrate vs high fat diets measured in a respiration chamber MS Westerterp-Plantenga 1 *, V Rolland 2, SAJ Wilson 1 and KR Westerterp 1 1 Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands; and 2 Institut EuropeÂen des Sciences du GouÃt et des Comportements Alimentaires, DyÈon, France Objective: Assessment of a possible relationship between perception of satiety and diet-induced thermogenesis, with different macronutrient compositions, in a controlled situation over 24 h. Design: Two diets with different macronutrient compositions were offered to all subjects in randomized order. Setting: The study was executed in the respiration chambers at the department of Human Biology, Maastricht University. Subjects: Subjects were eight females, ages 23 ± 33 y, BMI 23 3kg=m 2, recruited from University staff and students. Interventions: Subjects were fed in energy balance, with protein=carbohydrate=fat: 29=61=10 and 9=30=61 percentage of energy, with xed meal sizes and meal intervals, and a xed activity protocol, during 36 h experiments in a respiration chamber. The appetite pro le was assessed by questionnaires during the day and during meals. Diet induced thermogenesis was determined as part of the energy expenditure. Results: Energy balance was almost complete, with non-signi cant deviations. Diet-Induced-Thermogenesis (DIT) was 14.6 2.9%, on the high protein=carbohydrate diet, and 10.5 3.8% on the high fat diet (P < 0.01). With the high protein=high carbohydrate diet, satiety was higher during meals (P < 0.001; P < 0.05), as well as over 24 h (P < 0.001), than with the high fat diet. Within one diet, 24 h DIT and satiety were correlated (r ˆ 0.6; P < 0.05). The difference in DIT between the diets correlated with the differences in satiety (r ˆ 0.8; P < 0.01). Conclusion: In lean women, satiety and DIT were synchronously higher with a high protein=high carbohydrate diet than with a high fat diet. Differences (due to the different macronutrient compositions) in DIT correlated with differences in satiety over 24 h. Descriptors: diet-induced thermogenesis; satiety; appetite pro le; macronutrient composition; respiration chamber; energy balance Introduction Humans are continuous nutrient metabolizers but ingest food discontinuously. This discrepancy requires a ne tuning of energy intake to energy expenditure in order to maintain energy balance. Normally, energy balance is stable over on average a week (Edholm et al, 1955), which is mirrored by a relatively constant body weight over time. Tuning of energy intake to energy expenditure is realized by interaction of a variety of factors that control the actual food intake and meal pattern. Hunger, satiety and sensory signals are the main regulatory factors of meal size, meal frequency, and food selection. Different mediating processes which maintain inhibition of hunger during the early and late phases of satiety have been identi ed, that is, sensory, cognitive, post-ingestive, and post-absorptive (for example, Blundell and Halford, 1994). Sensory effects are generated through the smell, taste, temperature and texture of food and it is likely that these factors contribute to inhibition of eating while nishing a course or a meal Correspondence Dr. M. S. Westerterp-Plantenga, Department of Human Biology, Maastricht University, PO Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands. Received 21 September 1998; revised 5 January 1999; accepted 7 January 1999 (Rolls et al, 1982). Cognitive effects represent the beliefs held about the properties of foods, and these factors may also inhibit hunger postprandially. Post-ingestive processes include gastric distention, the rate of gastric emptying, the release of hormones and the stimulation of gastrointestinal receptors (Mei, 1985). The post-absorptive phase of satiety represents the effects which result from the metabolism of absorbed nutrients, and which may affect the metabolic rate and, for instance, the membrane potential of liver cells (Langhans, 1996). Alternatively, absorbed nutrients may act directly on the brain after crossing the blood-brain barrier, or may in uence the brain indirectly via neural inputs, following the stimulation of peripheral chemoreceptors (Blundell and Halford, 1994). The most important suppression and subsequent control of appetite seems to be brought about by post-ingestive and post-absorptive mediating processes (Blundell and Halford, 1994). The digestive products, which accurately re ect the food that has been consumed, may be metabolised in the peripheral tissues or organs or may enter the brain directly; as such, they constitute a class of metabolic satiety signals. With respect to the monitoring of energy intake we observed an association between the magnitude of dietinduced thermogenesis, expressed as an absolute increase

496 in metabolic rate during and after the meal, and satiety scores, with meals that differed in carbohydrate=fat proportions (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1997a). Considering different satiating ef cacies of the macronutrients protein, carbohydrate and fat, a priority has been reported with protein as most satiating and fat as least satiating (de Castro, 1987; de Graaf et al, 1992, Stubbs, 1995; Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1996). At the same time, a priority is shown with respect to the magnitude of the rate at which these macronutrients are metabolised (Stubbs, 1995; Abbot et al, 1988; JeÂquier, 1992). Because of this association it may be that the priority in satiating effects of these macronutrients is related to a main metabolic component. Taking the results of the studies mentioned together, we hypothesize that the magnitude of metabolic rate is positively related to the perception of satiety over 24 h. The theoretical basis for it might be that increased energy expenditure at rest implies increased oxygen consumption and an increase in body temperature which may coincide with increased satiety (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1990). In this study, we assessed the possible relationship between perception of satiety and metabolic rate at the same time, with different macronutrient compositions, in a controlled situation over 24 h. Therefore, perception of satiety during the waking period of time (16 h), and metabolic rates over 24 h, were measured during 36 h experiments in the respiration chamber. Aspects of satiety perception were only described by questionnaires, and not in terms of meal size and meal interval, since under conditions where ad libitum feeding responses are allowed, changes in satiety may translate into changes in feeding behaviour, which might cancel changes in subjective satiety. Moreover, this would not be in line with establishing energy balance. Therefore in order to maximise the ability to detect changes in subjective satiety, meal size and time were xed and mandatory. Therefore the subjects were fed to energy balance, and were provided an activity protocol, which was the same on each day. As such we compared possible relationships over 24 h between perception of satiety and diet-induced thermogenesis as a part of total metabolic rate, between a high fat menu and a high protein=high carbohydrate menu, provided in three meals and two snacks during a day each. Methods Subjects Subjects, eight healthy dietary unrestrained females, aged 23 ± 33 y, were recruited from the University staff. The subjects came from different cultures (French, Finnish, American, Caribbean, Dutch), but they had all lived in The Netherlands for at least 6 months. Degree of dietary restraint was determined by the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkard and Messick, 1985), that is, subjects completed the English version next to the French or the Dutch version, which did not yield a difference in any of the scores on the questions. Dietary restraint scores on the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire were: F1 (cognitive restraint): 7 2, F2 (disinhibition or emotional eating): 6 3, F3 (hunger): 5 3. Anthropometric measurements took place in the fasted state. Body weight was measured using a digital scale accurate to 0.01 kg (Sauter, type E1200, Germany), and height was measured to the nearest 0.001 m. Body mass index (BMI, kg=m 2 ) was calculated as body weight (kg) divided by height (m) squared. On average body mass was 67 1.2 (sd standard deviation) kg; body mass index was 23 3 (sd) kg=m 2. Body weight was checked each time the subjects came to the department for a measurement, and deviations remained within the sd. Body composition was estimated by using hydrodensitometry and isotope dilution (Westerterp et al, 1995). Body composition was calculated using the combined equation of Siri (Siri, 1961). Percentage body fat was 29 6%; fat free mass was 47 4 kg. Procedure Subjects came to the department three times, with two weeks in between the rst and second, and in between the second and third time, to stay in the respiration chamber for 36 hours. The rst time energy balance was determined, and the second and third time the different diets were offered, in random order. The subjects came in xed pairs. To exclude a possible change of season effect, the members of a pair had different diets at the same time. Respiration chamber Subjects were asked to stay in a respiration chamber, where oxygen consumption and carbon dioxide production was measured, three times for 36 h each, that is, each time two nights and a day. The respiration chamber is a 14 m 3 room, furnished with a bed, chair, computer, television, radio-cassette player, telephone, intercom, sink and toilet, and is ventilated with fresh air at a rate of 70 ± 80 l=min. Ventilation rate was measured with a dry gas meter (Schlumberger, type G4, The Netherlands). The concentration of oxygen and carbon dioxide was measured using paramagnetic O 2 analyzers (Hartmann & Braun, type Magnos 6G, Germany; Servomex, type OA 184A, UK) and infrared CO 2 analyzers (Hartmann & Braun, type Uras 3G, Germany). During each 15 min period six samples of outgoing air for each chamber, and one sample each of fresh air, zero, and calibration gas were measured. The gas samples to be measured were selected by a computer that also stored and processed the data (Schoffelen et al, 1997). Physical activity protocol During daytime subjects followed a light exercise protocol of six exercise periods of either walking or low intensity bench stepping, each with a duration of 10 min. The exercise protocol was implemented to extend the range of movement and avoid single extreme values of energy expenditure that could affect the calculation of diet-induced thermogenesis. Apart from the exercise periods, subjects were not restricted in their activities, only sleeping and streneous physical activity was not allowed during the day. Physical activity was continuously monitored by means of a radar system working on the Doppler principle. Energy intake protocol Energy intake (EI), adapted to reach energy balance, was determined during the rst 36 h stay in the respiration chamber. EI was calculated from sleeping metabolic rate (SMR), measured during this rst time, and multiplied by an activity index of 1.6 (Schrauwen et al, 1997). Before entering the chamber, at 19.00 h, subjects were given a dinner of similar macronutrient composition to the diet of the following day (protein=carbohydrate=fat:17=58=25) (Schrauwen et al, 1997). This macronutrient composition

was based upon the subjects' indications of their normal food intake, and it was chosen as being intermediate between the other two macronutrients that were going to be tested. The second and third time, the experimental interventions took place. Then, during the day when they stayed in the respiration chamber, the subjects received a diet divided over three meals and two snacks, which consisted of a xed menu of xed energy content and macronutrient composition. Also during the previous evening of staying in the chamber, a dinner with the same macronutrient composition was provided. This was executed to start adaptation of respiratory quotient (RQ) to food quotient (FQ) already before the experiment, in order to avoid the `party effect', ie the effect of the change instead of the effect after the change (Schrauwen et al, 1997). 24 h Energy intake was subject speci cally pre-determined (from the rst 36 h stay) to be able to feed the subjects to energy-balance. During the experimental interventions, two different macronutrient compositions were offered in random order (protein=carbohydrate=fat: 29=61=10 percentage of energy; protein=carbohydrate=fat: 9=30=61 percentage of energy). All subjects were familiar with the products that were offered, and they liked the food (hedonic values from 100 mm VAS scales 82 9), therefore the dependent variables would not interfere with familiarity or low hedonic values (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1992a and b). The diets were consumed as breakfast: 8.30 h: 23% of 24 h energy intake; lunch: 12.30 h: 31% of 24 h energy intake; dinner: 18.30 h; 32% of 24 h energy intake, and two snacks: 15.00 h: 8% of 24 h energy intake; and 20.30 h: 6% of 24 h energy intake. The diets were iso-energetic, that is, on average: 8.9 0.7 MJ=d, and iso-volumetric (since volume has been shown to affect satiety Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1997a), and were matched as closely as possible for organoleptic properties (taste, smell, appearance), (see Table 1). The macronutrient composition of the diets was calculated using the Dutch food composition table (Stichting Nederlands Voedingsstoffenbestand, 1996) and food quotients (FQ) were calculated using the equations of Brouwer (Brouwer, 1957). Satiety To characterize satiety perception throughout the day, a questionnaire with seven different questions was completed ten times at the same time each day, that is, before and after breakfast, mid-morning, before and after lunch, in the afternoon, before and after dinner, and twice in the evening. The questions were the following: How hungry are you; how full are you; how satiated are you; how thirsty are you; (these questions were anchored with: not at all ± very), how much do you estimate you could eat (anchored: not much ± very much); how is your desire to eat; how is your appetite; (both anchored: very weak ± very strong). The ratings had to be given on 100 mm Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). To characterize the development of satiation during a meal for breakfast, lunch and dinner, a questionnaire with the following questions was completed every 3 min only during the main course of the meal, using Visual Analogue Scales. These questions were: how hungry are you; how satiated are you; how pleasant is the taste of the food now in your mouth; (all anchored: not at all ± very). Data analysis Energy expenditure The total energy expenditure data analysis has been executed with a protocol described elsewhere (Westerterp et al, 1999). A brief summary follows. 24 h energy 497 Table 1 Diets of approx. 8900 kj in the following macronutrient compositions Protein=Carbohydrate= Fat: 29=61=10 Protein=Carbohydrate=Fat: 9=30=61 Breakfast: white bread with low fat croissant with butter, low energy jam, margarine 3%, jam, curd cream cheese-boursin. herb cheese. fat free fruit yogurt drink (water, tea or coffee) drink (water, tea or coffee) Lunch: mushroom soup with mushroom soup with fat-free milk powder double cream mixed salad (iceberg lettuce, mixed salad (iceberg lettuce, tomato, cucumber) tomato, cucumber) cottage cheese tuna sh in brine smoked mackerel mayonaise (5% fat) mayonaise (40% fat) fresh orange juice no energy orange drink `fantomalt' (a carbohydrate powder) drink (water, tea or coffee) Snack: peaches in syrup with peaches in syrup with fat-free yogurt and whipped double cream quark (arti cial sweetener) drink (water, tea or coffee) Dinner: pasta and tomato sauce; pasta and tomato sauce; with: chicken llet with: quorn (a vegetarian meat substitute) and vegetable oil cucumber-raw cucumber-raw drink (water, tea, or coffee) drink (water, tea, or coffee) Snack: vanilla pudding (fat-free) vanilla pudding with: double cream drink (water, tea, or coffee) drink (water, tea, or coffee) Total weight 3088.g 3076.g Energy density: 2.9 kj=g 2.9 kj=g Total weight of solid food 1288.g 1011.g Total weight of liquid food 1800.g 2065.g

498 expenditure (24 h EE) consists of sleeping metabolic rate (SMR), diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT), and activity induced energy expenditure (AEE). 24 h values for energy expenditure were calculated from 07.00 h-07.00 h, from oxygen consumption and CO 2 production, using the Weir formula (Weir, 1949). Sleeping metabolic rate (SMR) was measured on both nights of each experiment and was de ned as the lowest mean energy expenditure measured over three consecutive hours, between 0.00 and 07.00 h. The average SMR of the two nights was used in further calculations. Diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT) was calculated by plotting energy expenditure against radar output, both averaged over 30 min periods. The intercept of the regression line at lowest radar output represents energy expenditure in the inactive state (resting metabolic rate or RMR), consisting of SMR and DIT (Westerterp et al, 1999). Energy spent on activity (AEE) was calculated by subtracting RMR from 24 h EE. Diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT) equals 24 hee-smr- AEE. Calculation of DIT over 24 h yields the same gure as DIT over 21 h, since by de nition DIT is zero, when the only energy expenditure is represented by SMR. This was on average the case between 03.00 h and 06.00 h in the morning. During the two experiments (second and third time) in the respiration chamber, 24 h urine was collected from the second voiding on the day of the experiment until the rst voiding the following day. Samples were collected in containers that contained 10 ml H 2 SO 4 to prevent nitrogen loss through evaporation. Volume and nitrogen concentration were measured, the latter using a nitrogen analyser (Carlo Erba, EA 1108CHN, Rodano, Italy). Substrate oxidation Substrate oxidation was calculated using O 2 consumption, CO 2 production, and urinary nitrogen excretion, using the formula of Brouwer (1957). Theoretical estimates of DIT were calculated from measured substrate utilization using median values for the thermogenic effect of separate nutrients (Tappy, 1996) (namely, carbohydrate 10% EI, fat 1.5% EI, protein 25% EI). Satiety The scores on the questions during the main meals were compared between breakfasts, lunches and dinners, respectively, that is, between similar meals with the different macronutrient compositions (ANOVA repeated measures) (Figures 1, 2, 3). From the ratings on the seven questions throughout the day (24 h), the area under the curve (AUC), minus the rectangle that represents the basic area in which no scores were given was calculated for each subject (Figure 4). VAS ratings were interpolated from the latest measurement at night until the rst measurement in the morning, in order to complete the AUC over 24 h. The values were averaged over the subjects, separately for each question. These average AUC gures were compared between the situation with the high fat menu and the situation with the high protein=carbohydrate menu, using ANOVA repeated measures. Statistical analysis All results are expressed as mean sd. Data from the two experimental situations, that is, the high-fat diet and the high protein=high carbohydrate diet were compared with each other (ANOVA repeated measures; Statview SE Graphics TM ). P values were considered signi cant when < 0.05. A regression analysis was performed for each diet between 24 h satiety and DIT, and for the difference in satiety (AUC) and in DIT between both situations. Results Subjects were almost in energy balance (Westerterp et al, 1999), see Table 2. There was no statistically signi cant difference in energy balance between the two experimental Figure 1 Hunger, satiety and pleasantness of taste ratings (mean SD) during breakfast with a high protein=carbohydrate diet or with a high fat diet (n ˆ 8). Hunger was higher (42.3 2.8 vs 36.8 2.3) and satiety was lower (45.8 2.6 vs 57.5 2.4) on the high fat diet than on the high protein=high carbohydrate diet (P < 0.001).

499 Figure 2 Hunger, satiety and pleasantness of taste ratings (mean SD) during lunch with a high protein=carbohydrate diet or with a high fat diet (n ˆ 8). Hunger was higher (37.9 2.2 vs 36.0 2.3) and satiety was lower (58.0 2.5 vs 59.7 2.1) on the high fat diet than on the high protein=high carbohydrate diet (P < 0.05). Figure 3 Hunger, satiety and pleasantness of taste ratings (mean sd) during dinner with a high protein=carbohydrate diet or with a high fat diet (n ˆ 8). Hunger was higher (51.7 2.8 vs 42.6 1.9) and satiety was lower (49.5 3.4 vs 60.5 2.6) on the high fat diet than on the high protein=high carbohydrate diet (P < 0.001). days (Westerterp et al, 1999). Also, there was no statistically signi cant difference in SMR and AEE between these days. However, 24 h EE was higher during the high protein=high carbohydrate diet, compared to the high fat diet (Westerterp et al, 1999), although the difference did not reach statistical signi cance (Table 2). DIT was consistently higher in all subjects while on the high protein=carbohydrate diet, compared to while on the high fat diet (Westerterp et al, 1999), (Table 2). The theoretically calculated DIT for each diet, was very close to the measured DIT. On a group level, the difference in energy balance between the two experimental days was approximately equivalent to the difference in DIT between the diets (Table 2). The difference between each diet's FQ and RQ was statistically signi cant. Also, the RQ's between the diets differed signi cantly (Table 2). During the meals of the high protein=high carbohydrate diet, satiety was higher and hunger was lower than on the high fat diet (P < 0.05; P < 0.001, respectively; Figures 1, 2, 3). The changes in hunger (2.6 5mm=min ±

500 Figure 4 Satiety over 24 h during a high protein=carbohydrate diet and a high fat diet (n ˆ 8). AUC (mm.hrs) is 886.4 24.1 during the high protein=carbohydrate diet, vs 788.8 23.8 during the high fat diet (P < 0.001). Table 2 Deviation from energy balance (mean SD), magnitude of dietinduced thermogenesis (mean SD), and RQ value (mean SD), with a high protein=high carbohydrate diet and with a high fat diet; FQ's of the diets are given (n ˆ 8) High protein= high carbohydrate diet High fat diet P value Deviations from energy balance 7 351 593 kj=d 28 601 kj=d P > 0.05 DIT 1295 240 kj=d 931 315 kj=d 14.6 2.9% 10.5 3.8%; P < 0.01 Theoretical DIT 1338 122 kj=d 987 158 kj=d FQ 0.91 0.00 0.80 0.00 RQ 0.92 0.01 0.86 0.01 P < 0.001 RQ compared to FQ P < 0.01 P < 0.0001 DIT: diet-induced thermogenesis. FQ: food quotient. RQ: respiratory quotient. 3.3 4mm=min) and satiety (2.9 5mm=min ± 3.9 5mm=min) over the meal did not differ signi cantly between the two diets (P < 0.08). Sensory signals during the main courses, expressed as pleasantness of taste of the food in the mouth, showed small decreases from the start towards the end of the meals (1.2 3mm=min ± 1.7 2mm=min). The ratings on the questions throughout the day differed statistically signi cantly between the high protein=high carbohydrate and the high fat situation (Figure 4; Table 3). In the high protein=high carbohydrate situation, satiety and fullness expressed as area under the curve (AUC) were Table 3 AUC (mm.hrs) (mean SD) from the following 100 mm VAS ratings, over 24 h (before and after breakfast, lunch, dinner, and in the morning, afternoon, evening and late evening, and interpolated between late evening and before breakfast), during a day with a high protein= high carbohydrate diet and during a day with a high fat diet (n ˆ 8) High protein= high carbohydrate diet High fat diet P value Hunger 781.2 22.8 841.6 23.4 P < 0.01 Desire to eat 718.8 22.5 767.6 23.7 P < 0.01 Appetite 720.4 19.4 766.1 19.2 P < 0.01 Estimation of how much 606.4 23.9 655.6 24.4 P < 0.01 Satiety 886.4 24.1 788.8 23.8 P < 0.001 Fullness 847.8 19.3 779.2 20.2 P < 0.001 Thirst 623.6 24.3 575.2 20.7 P < 0.01 higher, whereas hunger, desire to eat, appetite and the estimation of how much one could eat (also expressed as AUC), were lower. Moreover at certain time points these scores differed signi cantly in the same directions (P < 0.05), that is, for hunger, desire to eat, appetite and estimation of how much one could eat at 7 h; at 13 h; at 19 h; at 21 h; and at 23 h. For satiety and fullness, signi cant (P < 0.05) higher scores with the high protein=high carbohydrate diet were found at the following time points: 7 h; 19 h. Thirst was only higher at 7 h, with the high carbohydrate high protein menu. The differences between satiety (AUC) with both macronutrient compositions or hunger (AUC) were correlated to the differences in DIT between both macronutrient

compositions (r ˆ 0.8; P < 0.01). Within one diet, 24 h DIT and satiety were correlated (r ˆ 0.6; P < 0.05). The order in which the second and third diet was offered, had not affected the results with respect to the appetite pro le data and the metabolic data. The average data of the 8 subjects produced during their second time did not differ signi cantly from the average of the data produced during their third time. Discussion On the two experimental days, the subjects ingested predetermined, identical amounts of energy and volume from almost equivalent foods (with respect to organoleptic characteristics), at identical times, in a fully controlled situation. The most obvious difference between the two diets consisted of the relatively extreme macronutrient composition, that is, a high protein=high carbohydrate diet (protein=carbohydrate=fat: 29=61=10 percentage of energy) and a high fat diet (protein=carbohydrate=fat: 9=30=61 percentage of energy). Throughout the day, in between meals, satiety and fullness was signi cantly higher on the high protein=high carbohydrate diet, than on the high fat diet, while hunger, appetite, desire to eat, and estimated quantity to eat, were signi cantly lower. Therefore, in this well controlled condition, that is, with subjects being fed in energy balance over 24 h, it is shown that a mixed diet with a relatively higher proportion of protein and carbohydrate, is more satiating than a diet with a relatively higher proportion of fat. This has been found before, though in different experimental designs, that is, when the extent to which subjects were in energy balance was not included (de Castro, 1987; de Graaf et al, 1992; Stubbs, 1995, Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1996). In this study, the consequences of eating given meals, at given time points, by subjects who were in energy balance, were observed by their appetite pro le ratings only. The order in which the two diets were given had not affected the results. Therefore, these ratings under controlled conditions, con rm behavioural changes that showed differences in energy intake due to different macronutrient compositions, observed in previous studies (de Castro, 1987; de Graaf et al, 1992; Stubbs, 1995, Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1996). Satiety was not only higher in the post-prandial state during the high protein=high carbohydrate diet, it was also higher during the high protein=high carbohydrate meals, and hunger was lower, compared to the high fat meal. Hunger and satiety ratings showed fast changes during the meals, without a signi cant difference between the two diets. Sensory signals, expressed as pleasantness of taste of the food (main course) in the mouth, showed small decreases from the start towards the end of the meals. Since the energy density and the volume of both diets were the same, the solid=liquid food proportion differed slightly (Table 1), which might have had some effect on satiety (Himaya et al, 1998). This effect would be mainly due to differences in gastric emptying (Himaya et al, 1998), which, over 24 h as in these experiments will not play a role anymore. The difference in satiety was present already before breakfast, probably because of the previous evening meal being of the same macronutrient composition as the menu throughout the day. Also thirst, being different before breakfast, might have been due to the previous evening menus. The main result from the metabolic part of this study (which is elaborately discussed by Westerterp et al, 1999), is the signi cant difference in diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT) over 24 h, that is, a higher DIT with a high protein=high carbohydrate diet, compared to the DIT with a high fat diet. Moreover, the difference that created the nonsigni cant energy imbalance, appeared to be mainly the difference in DIT (Westerterp et al, 1999), since activity induced energy expenditure and sleeping metabolic rate did not differ between both situations with different diets (Westerterp et al, 1999). It could be argued, that a higher DIT would be attributable to relative unfamiliarity with the diets, which we have reported before (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1992b). However, since the products that the different diets consisted of, were at least for some time familiar to the subjects, it is unlikely that this would have played a role (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1992a). From the RQ measurements we may assume that the high=carbohydrate=high protein diet was relatively closer to the subjects' normal diet, than the high fat diet. Usually the RQ values represent the macronutrient composition of the diet during the previous week (Schrauwen et al, 1997). The subjects had not complained about the mandatory activity pattern, which, yielding a physical activity index of 1.6, was, as usual during a respiration chamber stay (Schrauwen et al, 1997), somewhat lower than their usual physical activity index, which was estimated to be between 1.7 and 1.8. Combining the 24 h satiety and DIT results, we see that even when energy balance tended to be slightly negative, due to the higher DIT, satiety is higher than when energy balance tended to be slightly positive, with a lower 24 h DIT. It would be of interest to have a closer look at the time pattern of DIT and satiety. However, since the synchronization of DIT and satiety over 24 h is not completely parallel over time, it is more accurate to include the complete DIT from one postabsorptive state until the next postabsorptive state, as well as the complete appetite pro le over 24 h. In this respect measuring DIT using a respiration chamber is more realistic compared to measuring DIT using a ventilated hood system. In the respiration chamber subjects do not have to lay down during DIT measurements, which is the case with the ventilated hood system (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1997a). Moreover, a ventilated hood only allows DIT measurements over a limited period of time, which yields different results due to the pattern of increased EE over time caused by DIT. From the respiration chamber measurements it appears that DIT never is completely extinguished in between meals, which makes it clear that DIT measurements from ventilated hood experiments usually include an arbitrarily limited period of time. The result with respect to synchronous increases in satiety and DIT relates to a previous observation where we found that increased energy expenditure at rest, implying an increased oxygen consumption and an increase in body temperature, was related to increased satiety (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1990). Moreover, relatively higher satiety scores have been observed under limited oxygen availability conditions, for instance at high altitude (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1999), after exercise (Westerterp- Plantenga et al, 1997b) and in COPD patients (Schols, 1997). We suggest that when metabolic rate increases at rest, oxygen availability becomes limiting, which seems to be perceived by the subjects as a reduction in the possibility to eat and therefore as an increase in satiety, during and in between meals. 501

502 Conclusion From this study we conclude that when subjects, lean women, ingest identical amounts of energy and volume, in identical meal patterns and almost equivalent meal compositions, the difference in satiety over 24 h is correlated with the difference in the 24 h DIT component of energy expenditure. The relatively higher satiety on a protein rich or carbohydrate rich diet compared to a fat rich diet has been shown before in different experimental designs (de Castro, 1987; de Graaf et al, 1992; Stubbs, 1995, Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1996). Also, the relatively higher DIT on a protein rich or carbohydrate rich diet compared to a fat rich diet (Stubbs, 1995; Abbot et al, 1988; JeÂquier, 1992) has been reported before. However, those measurements were not carried out simultaneously or over 24 h. We previously observed an association between the magnitude of diet-induced thermogenesis, expressed as an absolute increase in metabolic rate during and 3 h after one meal, and satiety scores, with meals that differed in carbohydrate=fat proportions (Westerterp-Plantenga et al, 1997a), which we have con rmed now using 24 h measurements. The interpretation of these results in relation to the daily life situation may be that, within the range given in this study and controlled for all other factors, the higher the fat content of the diet, the lower satiety and diet-induced thermogenesis will be. In daily life, satiety signals usually affect meal size and intermeal interval (Blundell and Halford, 1994). Therefore, a diet with a higher fat content may lead to a positive energy balance, due to changes in energy intake as well as in energy expenditure. If these results, which apply to normal weight young women, are more generally applicable, the vulnerability to a positive energy balance while consuming a high fat diet, has implications for the development and maintenance of obesity. Over 24 h there is no ne tuning of energy intake to energy expenditure by an interaction of hunger, satiety and sensory signals. These factors may control the actual food intake and meal pattern within a day to a certain extent, that is, more or less in the range of the energy requirement, and a longer period of time is necessary to maintain energy balance accurately. References Abbot WGH, Howard BV, Christin L, Freymond D, Lilioja S, Boyce VL, Anderson TE, Bogardus C & Ravussin E (1988): Short term energy balance: relationships with protein, carbohydrate and fat balances. Am. J. Physiol. 255, 332 ± 337. Blundell JE & Halford JCG (1994): Regulation of nutrient supply: the brain and appetite control. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 53, 407 ± 418. Brouwer E (1957): On simple formulae for calculating the heat expenditure and the quantities of carbohydrate and fat oxidised in metabolism of men and animals, from gaseous exchange (oxygen intake and carbonic acid output) and urine-n. Acta Physiol. Pharmacol. Neerlandica. 6, 795 ± 802. Castro JM de (1987): Macronutrient relationships with meal patterns and mood in the spontaneous feeding behaviour of humans. Physiol. Behav. 39, 561 ± 569. Edholm OG, Fletcher JG, Widdowson EM & McCance RA (1955): The energy expenditure and food intake of individual men. Br. J. Nutr. 9, 286 ± 300. Graaf C de, Hulshof T, Weststrate JA & Jas P (1992): Short-term effects of different amounts of protein, fats and carbohydrates on satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 55, 33 ± 38. Himaya A & Louis-Sylvestre J (1998): The effect of soup on satiation. Appetite 30, 199 ± 210. JeÂquier E (1992): Calorie balance vs nutrient balance. In: Energy Metabolism: Tissue Determinants and Cellular Corollarie. JM Kinney (ed) Raven Press, New York 123 ± 138. Langhans W (1996): Role of the liver in the metabolic control of eating: what we know-and what we do not know. Neurosc. and Biobehavioral Rev. 20, 145 ± 153. Mei N (1985): Intestinal Chemosensitivity. Physiol. Review 65, 211 ± 237. Rolls BJ, Rowe EA, Rolls ET (1982): How sensory properties of foods affect human feeding behavior. Physiol. Behav. 29, 409 ± 417. Schoffelen PFM, Westerterp KR, Saris WHM & Ten Hoor F (1997): A dual respiration chamber with automated calibration. J. Appl. Physiol. 83, 2064 ± 2072. Schols A (1997): Nutrition and outcome in chronic respiratory disease. Nutr. 13: 161 ± 163. Schrauwen P, van Marken Lichtenbelt WD, Saris WHM & Westerterp KR (1997): Changes in nutrient oxidation in response to a high-fat diet. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 66, 276 ± 282. Siri WE (1961). Body composition from uid spaces and density: analysis of methods. In: Techniques for measuring body composition. Brozek J, Henschel A (eds) National Academy of Science, Washington DC 223 ± 244. Stichting Nederlands Voedingsstoffenbestand (1996). NEVO Tabel. Voorlichtingsbureau voor de Voeding, Den Haag. Stubbs RJ (1995): Macronutrient effects on appetite. Int. J. Obes. 19, (Suppl) S11 ± S19. Stunkard AJ & Messick S (1985): The Three Factor Eating Questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Psych. Res. 29, 71 ± 83. Tappy L (1996): Thermic effect of food and sympathetic nervous system activity in humans. Reprod Nutr Dev 36, 391 ± 397. Weir JBDV (1949): New methods for calculating metabolic rate with special reference to protein metabolism. J. Physiol. 109, 1±9. Westerterp KR, Wilson SAJ & Rolland V (1999): Diet-induced thermogenesis measured over 24 h in a respiration chamber: effect of diet composition. Int. J. Obes. 23, in press. Westerterp KR, Wouters L & van Marken Lichtenbelt WD (1995): The Maastricht protocol for the measurement of body composition and energy expenditure with labeled water. Obes. Res. 3, (Suppl 1) 49 ± 57. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Wouters L & Ten Hoor F (1990): Deceleration in cumulative food intake curves, changes in body temperature and dietinduced thermogenesis. Physiol. Behav. 48, 831 ± 836. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Van den Heuvel E, Wouters L & Ten Hoor F (1992a): Accuracy of estimates of forthcoming ingestion as a function of menu familiarity and dietary restraint. Appetite 18, 101 ± 109. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Van den Heuvel E, Wouters L & Ten Hoor F (1992b): Diet-induced thermogenesis and cumulative food intake curves as a function of familiarity with food and dietary restraint in humans. Physiol. Behav. 51, 457 ± 465. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Ijedema MJW & Wijckmans-Duijsens NEG (1996): The role of macronutrient selection in determining patterns of food intake in obese and non-obese women. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 50, 580 ± 591. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Wijckmans-Duijsens NEG, Verboeket-van de Venne WPHG, Graaf C de, Weststrate JA & Hof KH van het (1997a): Diet-induced thermogenesis and satiety in humans, after full fat and reduced fat meals. Physiol. Behav. 61, 343 ± 349. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Verwegen CRT, Ijdema MJW, Wijckmans NEG & Saris WHM (1997b): Acute effects of exercise or sauna on appetite in obese and nonobese men. Physiol. Behav. 62, 1345 ± 1354. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Westerterp KR, Rubbens M, Verwegen CRT, Richalet JP & Gardette B (1999): Appetite at `high altitude', operation Everest III (Comex-'97): a simulated ascent of the Mt. Everest. JAP. In press.