Which category replaces an omitted clitic? The case of European Portuguese Introduction.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Which category replaces an omitted clitic? The case of European Portuguese Introduction."

Transcription

1 Costa, João, Maria Lobo e Carolina Silva. No prelo. Which category replaces an omitted clitic? The case of European Portuguese. In P. Larranaga e P. Guijarro-Fuentes (eds.). Pronouns and Clitics in Early Language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter Which category replaces an omitted clitic? The case of European Portuguese João Costa, Maria Lobo & Carolina Silva CLUNL/FCSH/Universidade Nova de Lisboa 1. Introduction. A well known fact about child language is that pronominal clitics are omitted in children s early productions in many languages (cf. Jakubowicz et al. 1998, Wexler, Gavarró & Torrens, 2003; Tsakali & Wexler, 2003; Babyonyshev & Marin, 2005, among others). There is less consensus regarding the universality of clitic omission. For instance, in Spanish, there is an ongoing debate regarding the existence of omission and its duration (cf. e.g. Castilla et al. 2008). In recent work, we contend that there is evidence to claim that clitic omission is not a uniform phenomenon (Costa, Lobo & Silva 2009, Costa & Lobo 2010). Another area in which there is little consensus and not much discussion is on the nature of the (empty) category replacing the omitted clitic in languages with omission. The goal of this paper is twofold: first, we review previous work on clitic production by children acquiring European Portuguese, showing that there is omission in this language. Second, we contribute to the debate regarding the nature of the empty category, by arguing that children acquiring European Portuguese produce a null object, as in the target adult grammar, when they fail to produce a clitic. Based on this discussion, we try to find evidence for or against continuity in the representation of the null object in child language, by inspecting whether it is a pro, a variable or whether children s omissions of complements might be target deviant instances of VP-ellipsis. This paper is organized as follows: in section 2, some background information on clitics and null objects in European Portuguese is provided. Section 3 presents the results of previous studies on the production of different types of clitics by children acquiring European Portuguese. Section 4 discusses a set of comprehension experiments confirming the idea that European Portuguese speaking children know that the language has null objects. Finally, in section 5, we present some preliminary results of a study aiming at determining whether European Portuguese children know the nature and properties of the empty category involved in the null object construction.

2 2. Clitics and null objects in European Portuguese. Like other Romance languages, European Portuguese has pronominal clitics. Clitics can be accusative, as in (1a), but also dative (1b), reflexive (1c), or non-argumental (1d): (1) a. O Pedro viu-o. the Pedro saw it/him acc Pedro saw it/him. b. O Pedro deu-lhe um livro. the Pedro gave him/her dat a book Pedro gave him/her a book. c. O Pedro lavou-se. the Pedro washed-se refl Pedro washed himself. d. O Pedro porta-se mal. the Pedro behaves-se inherent bad Pedro behaves badly. Some clitics vary in gender, person and number. This is shown for the accusative clitic in (2) and (3): (2) Number and gender variation a. O Pedro viu-o. Masculine singular the Pedro saw him acc/masc Pedro saw him. b. O Pedro viu-a. Feminine singular the Pedro saw her acc/fem c. O Pedro viu-os. Masculine plural the Pedro saw them acc/masc

3 Pedro saw them. d. O Pedro viu-as. Feminine plural the Pedro saw them acc/fem (3) Person variation a. O Pedro viu-me. 1st person singular the Pedro saw me acc b. O Pedro viu-te. 2nd person singular the Pedro saw you acc c. O Pedro viu-o. 3rd person singular the Pedro saw him acc d. O Pedro viu-nos. 1st person plural the Pedro saw us acc e. O Pedro viu-vos. 2nd person plural the Pedro saw you acc f. O Pedro viu-os. 3rd person plural the Pedro saw them acc Unlike what happens in the majority of Romance languages, clitics are not always proclitic. Depending on the syntactic environment, clitics can be proclitic, enclitic or mesoclitic. As stated in Duarte & Matos (2000), there is a limited set of proclisis triggers, and enclisis functions as an elsewhere environment. Mesoclisis is restricted to future and conditional inflection, and occurs in enclisis contexts. In (4), we list the set of proclisis triggers (cf. Duarte & Matos 2000). In (5), an example of proclisis, enclisis and mesoclisis is given. (4) Proclisis triggering environments: Negation Wh-questions Embedded sentences with a lexical C Certain adverbs in preverbal position Preverbal quantified subjects Affective operators

4 (5) a. Proclisis Eu não te vi. I not you saw I didn t see you. b. Enclisis Eu vi-te. I saw you I saw you. c. Mesoclisis Eu ver-te-ei. I see-you-fut I will see you. Besides clitics, European Portuguese also has null objects (Raposo 1986). A null object construction can be used in the exact same pragmatic environment in which a clitic is legitimate: whenever the referent is given information: (6) A: E o Pedro? What about Pedro? B: a. Não o vi. Not him saw I haven t seen him. b. Não vi. Not saw I haven t seen him. As described in Raposo (1986) and Costa & Duarte (2003), null objects do not always freely alternate with clitics. In reflexive contexts, null objects are ruled out (7): (7) A: E o Pedro? What about Pedro? B: a. Não se lavou.

5 Not SE washed He hasn t washed himself b. *Não lavou. Not washed The same holds for 1 st and 2 nd person clitics. (8) illustrates this for 1 st person: (8) A Maria viu*(-me). the Maria saw me Maria saw me. The availability of the null object construction is also constrained by certain syntactic environments. As argued in Raposo (1986), null objects are ruled out in strong island environments: (9) A: E o Pedro? What about Pedro B: a. *Fiquei aborrecido quando abracei. Got annoyed when hugged b. Fiquei aborrecido quando o abracei. Got annoyed when him hugged I got annoyed when I hugged him. The observation that null objects are ruled out in strong island contexts motivates Raposo s (1986) proposal that null objects are variables. As such, they cannot be bound by a topic operator in the matrix sentence across a strong island boundary. Pronominal forms, instead, can establish coreference relations across islands. We will return to the different properties of pronouns and variables below. For the time being, it is enough for us that the reader keeps in mind the idea that the null object is a variable, which explains its limited distribution and its ungrammaticality in contexts like islands a context in which pronouns cannot be bound. The main aspects to be retained from this sketchy description of the behavior of clitics and null objects in European Portuguese are:

6 a) European Portuguese has pronominal clitics, like other Romance languages; b) European Portuguese clitics vary in syntactic function, gender, number and person; c) European Portuguese is a null object language; d) In certain environments, null objects freely alternate with clitics; e) In specific environments, null objects are ruled out; f) The analysis of null objects as variables explains their sensitivity to strong island environments. Given this description, it should be clear now that, for the learning child, acquiring clitics in this language involves, at least, detecting its form and functions, finding out that the clitic can alternate with a null object, and discovering the correct distribution of null objects, partly by determining the nature of the empty category involved in null object constructions. 3. Clitic production in the acquisition of European Portuguese. In a series of studies by Ken Wexler and associates (Wexler, Gavarró & Torrens, 2003; Tsakali & Wexler, 2003; Babyonyshev & Marin, 2005), it is argued that children omit clitics in early stages of acquisition, but omission is not universal. These authors argue that it correlates with the availability of past participle agreement in the language. For instance, French is a language with past participle agreement (10), and as such it is expected to have clitic omission: (10) a. Les fenêtres Je les ai repeintes. The windows I them ACC-FEM-PL have repainted- FEM-PL The windows. I repainted them. Spanish, on the other hand, does not have past participle agreement (11), and is not expected to have clitic omission: (11) a. Las ventanas las he repintado The windows I them ACC-FEM-PL have repainted- MASC-SG

7 The windows. I repainted them. These authors proposal predicts that there should be no clitic omission in European Portuguese, since this language patterns like Spanish in what concerns past participle agreement. In a series of studies (Costa & Lobo 2006, Silva 2008, Carmona & Silva 2007, Costa, Lobo, Silva & Carmona 2008), we tested this prediction and found no evidence to support it, because children acquiring European Portuguese do omit clitics. However, two problems arise: first, the rates of omission were not comparable to those found for other languages. It was found that children acquiring European Portuguese omit more than children acquiring other languages with omission, and that omission lasts longer than in other languages, since 5 year olds still display a certain rate of omission, whereas in other languages, omission rates drop much earlier. In Varlokosta et al. (in preparation), a comparative work on 16 languages, it becomes obvious that omission rates are higher in Portuguese than in other languages. In this study, it is shown that children acquiring European Portuguese, at the age of 5, produce 20% of their sentences with clitics, whereas Italian and Catalan children, at the same age, and in the same experimental setting, produce 92,9% and 98% of clitics, respectively. The second problem is that the data do not receive a self-evident interpretation. Recall that European Portuguese has null objects. As such, when a child utters a sentence without a clitic, one does not know whether the child is omitting a clitic as in other languages, or producing a target null object construction. In order to find the source of the problem, we tested a wider range of clitics. In particular, we decided to test the production of clitics in contexts in which null objects are ruled out in order to determine whether the children omit in accordance with a target null object grammar. If so, their rates of omission should be nearly null in those contexts in which the clitic is compulsory. In the remainder of this section, we summarize the results obtained for clitic production of different types. The results presented come from Silva (2008, 2010). Following the elicitation procedure first used in Schaeffer (1997), and first adapted for European Portuguese in Costa & Lobo (2006, 2007), Silva (2008, 2010) tested 73 children aged between 3 and 6 years and 6 months old. The tests of clitic

8 production were also applied to a control group composed by 15 adults. The following table gives details on the children and adults tested: Children Mean age Female Male Total [3,0; 4,0[ 3 years and 7 months [4,0; 5,0[ 4 years and 5 months [5,0; 6,0[ 5 years and 6 months [6,0; 6,5[ 6 years and 3 months Total Adults Mean age Women Men Total [24,0; 27,0[ 25 years and 2 months Table 1: Participants information As just mentioned, the aim of Silva s study was to test different types of clitics, in order to assess correlations with the null object construction. As such, the following types of clitics were tested:

9 Tests per type of clitic Accusative Dative Reflexive Non-argumental Enclisis Proclisis Islands Enclisis Proclisis Islands Enclisis Proclisis Enclisis Proclisis 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Sing 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 1 st Plur 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Sing 2 nd Plur 2 nd Plur 2 nd Plur 2 nd Plur 2 nd Plur 2 nd Plur 3 rd Sing, fem 3 rd Sing, masc 3 rd Sing, fem 3 rd Sing, masc 3 rd Sing, fem 3 rd Sing, masc 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Sing 3 rd Plur, fem 3 rd Plur, masc 3 rd Plur, fem 3 rd Plur, masc 3 rd Plur, fem 3 rd Plur, masc 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur 3 rd Plur Table 2: Conditions tested in the experiment. As can be seen in the table, the conditions include those contexts in which clitics and null objects alternate, and those in which only clitics are a valid option. Two elicitation procedures were used. For 1 st and 2 nd person clitics, a puppet interacted with the child and with some toys. For 3 rd person clitics, the clitics were induced through the presentation of pictures. Both procedures had in common the relevant fact that the referent for the pronoun was made highly accessible. Below, we present an example for each of the procedures taken from Silva (2010):

10 Experiencer: What will Grandma do?! Grandma: I can smell something But I don t know what it is Are you both wearing perfume? Let me smell you [Grandma smells the child and the Puppet] Hmmm! You smell so good! Puppet: Estava distraído! Não reparei no que a Avó fez. Como é que ela sabe que nós cheiramos bem?! Diz lá o que a Avó fez? I was distracted! I didn t pay attention to what Grandma did. How does she know we smell good?! Tell me what did she do? EXPECTED RESPONSE: Cheirou-nos. She smelled us. Figure 1: Example of test item for eliciting 1 st person clitics. Experiencer (1 st Image): These 3 boys are dirty because they played football in the mud. But look! Each one of them has a towel in his hand. Experiencer (2 nd Image): [Pointing to the drawing] Este menino usou esta toalha e está agora limpo. Mas estes 2 continuam sujos. O que é que eles não fizeram? This boy used this towel and now he s clean. But these 2 boys are still dirty. What didn t they do? EXPECTED RESPONSE: Não se limparam. They didn t clean themselves. Figure 2: Example of test item for eliciting 3 rd person clitics.

11 For a detailed presentation of the results for each and every condition, we refer the reader to Silva (2008, 2010). For the purposes of this paper, it is sufficient that we focus on the results obtained in the 3 rd person accusative condition the clearest case in which clitics and null objects are in free variation, comparing them with a summary of results in which clitics should be obligatorily produced. Let us start by considering the results for the 3 rd person (singular and plural) accusative condition in non-compulsory contexts (enclisis and proclisis) summarized in table 3: 3 rd PERSON ACCUSATIVE CLITICS Clitic [3,0; 4,0[ [4,0; 5,0[ [5,0; 6,0[ [6,0; 6,5[ 23/240 99/ /336 74/160 9,58% 22,92% 43,45% 46,25% Control group 229/240 95,42% Null form 210/ / /336 84/160 3/240 87,50% 68,98% 44,35% 52,50% 1,25% DP 6/ /432 41/336 2/160 8/240 2,50% 7,64% 12,20% 1,25% 3,33% Strong pronoun 1/240 0,42% 2/432 0,46% 0/336 0% 0/160 0% 0/240 0% Table 3: Results for 3 rd person accusative clitics in non-compulsory contexts. Two major conclusions emerge from these results. First, children s production of clitics is different from what adults do (chi-square tests: p < for all comparisons between each children s group and the control group). There is an evident development effect from 3 to 5 years old (chi-square tests: p < 0.001). From 5 to 6, the development is much slower (chi-square test: p = 0,897). However, at age 6 the rate of production is still far from what is observed for adults 1 (as mentioned above, chi-square test: p < 0.001). Second, as was referred before, it is not the case that clitic omission ceases 1 The adults were, possibly, sensitive to the experiment since a higher production of null objects was expected in contexts where they freely vary with clitics.

12 at around age 3, as argued for other languages. Recall, however, that these data do not allow us a clear interpretation of the facts as clitic omission, since the null form row may be read as a case for assuming that children produce target null objects. More revealing data comes in the following table, containing a synthesis of the results for those contexts in which clitics are obligatory and null objects are ruled out. 3 rd PERSON ACCUSATIVE CLITICS IN STRONG ISLANDS Clitic [3,0; 4,0[ [4,0; 5,0[ [5,0; 6,0[ [6,0; 6,5[ 4/120 33/216 49/168 32/80 3,33% 15,28% 29,17% 40% Control group 78/120 65% Null form 77/120 64,17% 76/216 35,18% 27/168 16,07% 20/80 25% 0/120 0% DP 38/ /216 92/168 28/80 42/120 31,67% 49,54% 54,76% 35% 35% Strong pronoun 1/120 0,83% 0/216 0% 0/168 0% 0/80 0% 0/120 0% Table 4: Results for 3 rd person accusative clitics in strong islands (compulsory contexts). The results of these tables are interesting. First, they reveal that clitics are also omitted in contexts in which null objects are ruled out. However, they show that the rate of omission is lower in compulsory contexts than in non compulsory contexts: [3,0; 4,0[ - 87,50% vs. 64,17%; [4,0; 5,0[ - 68,98% vs. 35,18%; [5,0; 6,0[ - 44,35% vs. 16,07%; [6,0; 6,5[ - 52,50% vs. 25 %. In Costa & Lobo (2006), the fact that children omit clitics even in contexts in which null objects are ruled out was taken as an indication that there is clitic omission different from the target null object construction in children s productions. However, this account failed to provide an explanation for why there were different rates of omission, and did not make any commitment as to the nature and category of the empty element in the contexts in which clitics were not produced.

13 In fact, in the absence of a clitic, several outcomes are possible: either the child produces a kind of truncated structure and no empty category is involved, or the child is filling in the object position with some kind of empty category. The first hypothesis predicts that children treat structures without a complement as intransitive, whereas the second one predicts that children know that the relevant structures are transitive. In other words, under the second hypothesis, children know how to interpret null object constructions. If the second alternative is on the right track, one still has to know what type of null object is involved: a pronominal or a variable. Summarizing, the results obtained in the studies on children s production of clitics in European Portuguese open up space for a certain degree of analytical indeterminacy. The options are: a) Children omit clitics in compulsory contexts, because they have clitic omission of the type found in other languages. b) Children omit clitics in compulsory contexts, because they know that European Portuguese is a null object language, but they overuse null objects. In order to decide between these two options, some comprehension tests were run, in order to understand whether children acquiring European Portuguese display knowledge on the existence of the null object construction. If they do not, the second hypothesis can be immediately discarded. In the next section, we briefly present the results of the experiments carried out in Costa and Lobo (2008, 2010), which try to provide answers to this issue. 4. Null objects in comprehension. French children are known to omit clitics. Grüter (2006) raised a question for French quite similar to the one we raised for European Portuguese: can it be that French children are producing a kind of null object construction? If so, as conjectured by Grüter (2006), this means that their grammar includes the possibility of generating transitive structures without an explicit element. As such, they are supposed to interpret a verb

14 without a complement transitively. In order to test this prediction, Grüter (2006) developed a test in which children were asked to judge the truth value of verbs without a complement in transitive contexts. Children acquiring French massively rejected transitive interpretations for verbs that did not appear with any type of complement. This result can be interpreted as a consequence of the absence of a null object construction in their grammar. Costa and Lobo (2008, 2009) adapted Grüter s test for European Portuguese, and assessed whether children could assign transitive interpretations to a verb without a complement in the following environments: a) Simple clauses; b) Strong islands; c) Reflexive contexts. The purpose of testing environment a) is twofold: first, it allows for a clear comparison with the results obtained for French; second, it allows us to determine whether children acquiring European Portuguese have null objects. Environments b) and c) are those in which null objects are illegitimate in the adult grammar (out of these, only environment b) was tested for French, and no differences were found with respect to context a)). Testing the comprehension of verbs without a complement is interesting, since one can assess consistency between production and comprehension, and add a further argument to the idea that omission in European Portuguese is a case of overuse of the null object construction. In order to test the comprehension of null objects, Grüter s test was adapted to European Portuguese. In this test, a selection of verbs that can be either intransitive or transitive are used. A transitive situation is presented with a sentence describing it (uttered by a puppet) containing only subject and verb. If the child s grammar has no null objects, the child must judge such description false. If, on the other hand, the child s grammar includes null objects, she will accept a sentence with no explicit internal argument, considering it true. Let us consider two examples of test items: a potentially ambiguous sentence is presented to the child in a context making one of the interpretations true or false. After

15 the presentation of an image, the child is asked to judge whether a sentence uttered by the puppet is true or false. In the test items, the following transitive/intransitive alternation verbs were used: mergulhar dive ('x mergulha' or 'x mergulha y'), adormecer fall asleep / put to sleep ('x adormece' or 'x adormece y'), acordar wake up ('x acorda' or 'x acorda y'), baloiçar swing ('x baloiça' ou 'x baloiça y'). As shown in the following examples, both uses are possible for all these verbs: (12) a. O Rui mergulhou. the Rui dove Rui dove. b. O Rui mergulhou o irmão na piscina. the Rui dove the brother in the pool Rui put his brother in the pool (13) a. O Rui adormeceu. The Rui fell asleep Rui fell asleep. b. O Rui adormeceu o bebé. The Rui fell asleep the baby Rui put the baby to sleep. (14) a. O Rui acordou. The Rui woke up Rui woke up b. O Rui acordou o bebé. The Rui woke up the baby Rui woke up the baby. (15) a. O Rui baloiçou na cadeira. The Rui swang in the chair Rui swang in the chair. b. O Rui baloiçou o bebé na cadeira.

16 The Rui rocked the baby in the chair Rui rocked the baby in the chair. The test was preceded by a familiarization period with the task, with the puppet, with the images and with the verbs used in both variants (transitive and intransitive). In the period of adaptation to the experimental setup, the images were presented to the child and the four verbs were used in the transitive and in the intransitive constructions in order to ensure that the verbs, in their different constructions, were known to the child, and in order to make the interpretation of the drawings easier. In the following set of images, one can see the type of material used in the elicitation of judgements on null objects in reflexive environments: Figure 3: Example of pictures used in the comprehension of null objects For these images, the experimenter first introduces the characters, and then he describes the situation in the first drawing making the characters highly available in the discourse scale. Then, the puppet would utter a sentence like He is washing (without the reflexive clitic), which is not a legitimate description for the rightmost image. The results of this experiment, run on 19 children aged between 3;6 and 5;9, are summarized in the following graph, in which the rates of adult-like responses are given 2 : 2 Pron (V): Condition for pronouns with pronominal interpretation (True); Pron=Refl (F): Condition for pronouns with reflexive interpretation (False);Refl (V): Condition for reflexives with reflexive interpretation (True); Refl=Pron (F): Condition for reflexives with pronominal interpretation (False); ON

17 3;6-5;9 (n=19) 100% 80% 60% 40% 20% 0% Pron (V) Pron=Refl (F) Refl (V) Refl=Pron (F) ON (V) ReflN (F) DP (V) TransRefl (F) Pron (V) Pron=Refl (F) Refl (V) Refl=Pron (F) ON (V) ReflN (F) DP (V) TransRefl (F) Graph 1: Rates of correct comprehension of pronouns, reflexives and null arguments The most important conditions to focus on in the graph are ON(V), in which we tested the comprehension of null objects in simple sentences, and REFLN(F), the condition in which null objects should be ruled out. As can be read off the graph, children are successful in the mastery of null objects in simple transitive clauses. This is obvious, given the high success rate in the interpretation of transitive verbs without a complement as transitive. This contrasts with the behavior of children acquiring French described in Grüter (2006). The comparison between ON(V) and REFLN(F) shows that children fail to understand that reflexives are not good candidates for null objects. Note that no clear individual differences were found, and, as such, the difference between these two conditions is quite strong. These results are consistent with the results obtained in production tasks. Recall that children fail to reject the ungrammatical structures that they produce. From these observations, several interesting conclusions can be drawn: a) First, it becomes obvious that omission is not a uniform phenomenon, since different speakers display different behaviors towards omission. In particular, in French, no acceptance was found in these contexts, very much (V): Condition with accusative null objects (True); ReflN (F): Condition with null reflexive (False); DP (V): Control condition with full DP (True); TransRefl (F): Control condition with full DP and reflexive interpretation (False).

18 differing from European Portuguese. As such, it is legitimate to conjecture that the high rate of omission in this language is due to the overuse of null objects, whereas in French the inability to judge the same kind of stimulus transitively reflects that no null object grammar is at play. b) Second, the hypothesis that the high rates of omission found in European Portuguese are due to an overuse of the null object construction is confirmed, since the contexts of illegitimate production and comprehension overlap. c) Third, the rate of illegitimate acceptance of null objects in reflexive contexts is very similar to the rate of illegitimate acceptance of null objects in strong islands, found in Costa & Lobo (2008). This consistency in results again parallels what has been found in production. If the analysis we are proposing is on the right track, the combination of the results from production and comprehension converge in showing that children know that European Portuguese is a null object language, and overuse the null object option. If that is so, the differential behavior in clitic production, when this language is compared to others, can be easily explained: the rate of clitic omission and the ages at which children converge with the control group are different from what has been found for other languages, because clitic omission in this language is of a different nature. Unlike in other languages, clitic omission in European Portuguese is a kind of null object construction. Note that this is an interesting result, since it confirms the idea that much of the syntactic knowledge is acquired very early (Wexler 1998), and gives support to the claim there is great continuity between child and adult grammar (Guasti 2002). However, note that the evidence referred to so far does not yet provide a crystal-clear answer regarding the nature of the empty category involved. One possibility raised in Silva (2010) is that the null category assumed by children is a pro, and not a variable. This would explain why there are more contexts in which the null object is allowed for the child than for the adult. What is at stake, then, is to determine the degree of continuity involved in the mastery of the null object construction. Do children posit the same empty category as adults? For children, is the null object a pro or a variable? If the null object is pro, the

19 degree of continuity is smaller than one might think. Still another option, available in the target grammar is that children use a VP-ellipsis construction (as in example 16a). VP-ellipsis differs from null object in that it is allowed in islands (16b), with reflexives (16c), and it requires verbal parallelism (16d) (Matos 1992) 3 : (16) a. O Pedro tinha escrito o artigo e eu também tinha. The Pedro had written the article and I also had Pedro had written the article, and I had too. b. O Pedro escreveu o artigo, porque eu também escrevi. The Pedro wrote the article because I also wrote Pedro wrote the article, because I also did. c. O Pedro lavou-se e eu também lavei. Pedro washed SE and I also washed Pedro washed himself, and I did too. d. *O Pedro comprou o livro, porque eu roubei. The Pedro bought the book, because I stole The pilot study presented in the next section tries to shed some light on the nature of the null category involved in children s performances. 5. Null object: pro, variable or ellipsis? As discussed in the previous section, there is good evidence for saying that clitic omission in child Portuguese is an overuse of the null object option, independently available in the target grammar. If this analysis is on the right track, there is continuity between children s knowledge and the properties of adult grammar. However, in order to determine the degree to which child language and adult grammar are alike, it is 3 Santos (2006), unlike Matos (1992), rejects the idea that VP-ellipsis requires verbal identity. The fact that sentences like (16d) are ungrammatical is a standard argument to distinguish VP-ellipsis and null objects, since there is no verbal identity, and the ellipsis is illegitimate, because of the island context. If, on the contrary, a relaxation of the identity requirement were possible, this sentence should be fine under a VP-ellipsis reading, since VP-ellipsis is allowed in island contexts. Since, in the experiment, we use cases similar to these ones, we will follow Matos in accepting the idea that VP-ellipsis requires identity.

20 important to find out whether the representation assumed by the child for null objects matches the one of adults. In order to do so, Costa & Lobo (2010a) developed a test enabling to establish whether children ascribe to null objects the interpretation expected if it is a variable. In this section, we present the test and explore some pilot results. This test is highly inspired in the findings of Miyagawa (2010), who described that pro and variables behave differently in anaphoric retrieval contexts. In particular, a null subject in an embedded environment cannot have a sloppy reading. This can be seen in the following example: (11) O Pedro disse que os pais iam a cavalo e o Rui disse que iam a pé. The Pedro said that the parents went on horse and the Rui said that went on foot Pedro said that his parents went on horse, and Rui said that his (parents) went by foot. Possible reading: Rui said that Pedro s parents went walking. Impossible reading: Rui said that his parents went walking. The only reading available for this sentence is the strict one, in which the null category retrieves the reference of the matrix antecedent. This contrasts with null objects: (12) O Pedro abraça os pais e o Afonso beija. The Pedro hugs his parents and Afonso kisses Sentence (12) is ambiguous, since the object can be co-referential with an antecedent from the first coordinate (in the case Afonso kisses Pedro s parents), or with Afonso s parents (in the reading in which Pedro hugs his parents, and Afonso kisses his parents). The first reading is generally known as strict, whereas the second one is known as sloppy.

21 Recall from the previous section that VP-ellipsis is another option to be considered in the interpretation of children s performances. As such, it is relevant to know how this construction behaves with respect to the strict/sloppy readings. As shown in (13), VP-ellipsis allows both strict and sloppy readings: (13) O Pedro abraça os pais e o Afonso também abraça. The Pedro hugs his parents and Afonso also hugs This sentence is ambiguous, since it can mean that Afonso hugs Pedro s parents (strict reading) or that he hugs his own parents (sloppy reading). Given this, it becomes obvious that the sole difference between null objects in simple sentences (as in 12) and VP-ellipsis in the same context is the requirement of verbal parallelism. Since we know that pro only allows for strict readings (Miyagawa 2010) (recall the description of the sentence in (11)), if a child consistently rejects sloppy readings for null object, there will be good reasons to assume that she does not know that the null object is a variable. If, on the other hand, the child knows that the null subject can only have strict readings, rejecting sloppy readings for it, and accepting them for null objects, we will have good evidence to postulate that children know the category of the null arguments they use. If the child gives non-adult answers for null subjects, accepting sloppy readings, however, we will have to assume that the interpretation of empty categories in child grammar is generally subject to development. In order to test this, Costa & Lobo (2010) developed a truth value judgment task testing the following conditions: a) Strict readings for null subjects (True); b) Sloppy readings for null subjects (False); c) Strict readings for null objects (True); d) Sloppy readings for null objects (True); e) Strict readings for VP ellipsis (True); f) Sloppy readings for VP ellipsis (True).

22 The test was designed in the following way: pictures of situations involving two children and their parents were presented to the participants. A puppet uttered a statement about the picture, which had to be judged true or false. The sentences were of the type illustrated in (11), (12) and (13). Consider, for instance, the following test item for condition a). The picture below was shown: Figure 4: Example of picture used in the strict/sloppy readings comprehension task The test sentence for this picture was: O Moreno disse que os pais estavam sentados e o Loiro disse que tinham chapéu. The dark hair kid said that the parents were sitting and the blond kid said that had hat The dark-haired kid said that his parents were sitting, and the blond kid said that they had a hat. This sentence is true, because only the strict reading is available for subject pro in the adult grammar. Let us now see an example of a test item for the null object condition under the sloppy reading. First, a picture like the following is shown:

23 Figure 5: Example of picture used in the strict/sloppy readings comprehension task The test sentence for this picture was: O Louro abraça os pais e o Moreno beija. The blond kid hugs the parents and the dark haired kid kisses The blond kind hugs his parents and the dark-haired kid kisses them. This sentence is true, because the sloppy reading is available for null object in adult grammar. In what follows, we present the results of a pilot of this test. In the pilot, 20 5 year old typically developing monolingual children acquiring European Portuguese, aged between 5;0 and 5;10 participated (8 boys and 12 girls). The mean age was 5;5. Children were tested individually in a quiet room. No response-contingent stimulus was given, and children were only rewarded after completion of the whole task. Data were coded and transcribed by two experimenters. Additionally, the test was ran on a control group consisting of 15 adults aged between 23 and 46 years old. The test contained 5 items for each of the conditions listed above (30 items for the sum of the whole conditions) and 17 fillers.

24 As can be seen in the following table, the adult control group performed as expected, associating sloppy readings with VP-ellipsis and null object only, and rejecting such sloppy reading in null subject contexts: %target responses Sloppy reading for null subject (False) 96% (72/75) Strict reading for null subject (True) 100% (75/75) Strict reading for null object (True) 100% (75/75) Sloppy reading for null object (True) 100% (75/75) Strict reading for VP ellipsis (True) 98,6% (74/75) Sloppy reading for VP ellipsis (True) 98,6% (74/75) Table 5: Results of the control group in the strict/sloppy readings comprehension test In the following table, we present the general results per condition for children: %target responses Sloppy reading for null subject (False) 51% (51/100) Strict reading for null subject (True) 64% (64/100) Strict reading for null object (True) 71% (71/100) Sloppy reading for null object (True) 71% (71/100) Strict reading for VP ellipsis (True) 69% (69/100) Sloppy reading for VP ellipsis (True) 86% (86/100) Table 6: Results for children in the strict/sloppy readings comprehension test These preliminary results from the pilot study are very interesting. As is clear from the results in the table, there is no clear difference between conditions. In fact, children have a poor performance in all conditions, and do not reach ceiling performances in any of them (unlike we found for adults, cf. Costa & Lobo 2010a). Crossing these results with those of previous experiments and the results of Santos (2006) for VP-ellipsis can help us understand this performance. First, one cannot argue that the bad results are a consequence of a lack of knowledge on null object, null subject or VP-ellipsis. Independent research reveals that children acquiring European Portuguese know null objects (cf. the studies mentioned in the previous sections). The same holds for null subjects, since they produce sentences with null subjects from early

25 on (Gonçalves 2005). Finally, Santos (2006) convincingly shows that children know VP-ellipsis, since they produce it in verbal answers to yes-no questions, and are able to retrieve the antecedent of an elided VP in simple contexts. In short, children s difficulties cannot be attributed to the inexistence of each of these constructions in their grammar. We contend that these data show that children have problems interpreting the null category involved in these constructions. In other words, they have null subjects, null objects, and VP-ellipsis, but fail to assign them an adult interpretation, and are not able to distinguish between strict and sloppy readings. If we go back to our original question - what is the nature of the category involved in children s object omissions? we are now closer to an answer. Three possibilities emerge: i) children use pro in object position, but do not know its interpretation; ii) iii) children use a variable in object position, but do not know its interpretation; children use a VP-ellipsis, and disrespect the requirement of verbal parallelism. Option iii) can be easily discarded on the basis of Santos (2006) results, since she showed that children s production of VP-ellipsis in verbal answers to yes-no questions provide positive evidence for compliance with the parallelism requirement. However, as shown by these results, their interpretation of VP-ellipsis is not target, since there are obvious differences between children s performance and the ceiling results of adults. Options i) and ii) can, actually, be merged, since, on the basis of the evidence we have, they cancel each other out. pro and variables can be distinguished on the basis of their distribution and interpretation. The data from previous experiments show that children annul the differences with respect to distribution, since they license an alleged variable in islands. The preliminary data from this pilot shows that they also annul the difference with respect to interpretation, since they assign pro readings typical of variables (the sloppy one), and they sometimes reject strict reading for null objects. Given this and on the basis of the data available so far, it is legitimate to state that the difference between pro and variables is irrelevant at this stage of language development.

26 Considering all this, one can hypothesize that for children to reach an adult knowledge of the types of null categories involved in subject and object position, they have to acquire the difference in interpretation that distinguish them, which arguably involves acquiring further properties distinguishing operator-variable relations from pronominal binding, a matter we leave for further research. 6. Conclusions. In this paper, we aimed at presenting a summary of previous results obtained in the study of clitic production by European Portuguese children. It was shown that the results of the production tasks indicated that clitic omission in European Portuguese is different from what occurs for other languages. As mentioned in previous studies, the results obtained for European Portuguese show that clitic omission is not a uniform phenomenon crosslinguistically, and, because of this, no clear conclusions may be drawn from the European Portuguese data in order to account for or against views of clitic omission like Wexler, Gavarró & Torrens (2003). We also showed that the results from comprehension studies provide good evidence to say that null objects are available early, and known to the children. This implicit knowledge of null objects provides further evidence for a different treatment of clitic omission, and is a good argument in favor of continuity in language acquisition. In order to determine the degree of continuity, we tested whether children master the difference between pro, variable and VP-ellipsis. Preliminary results from a pilot experiment indicate that children do not yet know the interpretive option associated with each one of these null constructions. As a consequence, we did not yet obtain crystal-clear evidence for determining the nature of the category involved in clitic omission contexts, but we are now able to say that the difference between pro and variable is not a relevant distinctive factor for children. This is promising as an explanation for the overuse of null object. By hypothesis, and comparing to the results obtained for VP-ellipsis (for which there is independent evidence for the early mastery of its syntax), we can suppose that children know the syntax of null subject and null object, and the only problem they have is in the assignment of the correct interpretation.

27 Children s task for null objects will be to narrow down the range of possible interpretations, excluding, for instance, reflexive and 1 st and 2 nd person options. References Babyonyshev, Maria & Stefania Marin (2005) The Acquisition of Object Clitic Constructions in Romanian. In Gess, Randall S. & Edward J. Rubin (eds.), Theoretical and Experimental Approaches to Romance Linguistics; Carmona, Jaqueline & Carolina Silva (2007) A aquisição de clíticos dativos em PE: teste piloto. In A. Coutinho & M. Lobo (eds.) Textos Seleccionados. XXII Encontro Nacional da APL. APL/Colibri, Lisboa; Castilla, A., A. T. Pérez-Leroux & A. Eriks-Brophy (2008) Omission in Early Spanish Clitics. In A. Gavarrò e M. J. Freitas, eds. Language Acquisition and Development. Proceedings of GALA2007. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing; Costa, João & Inês Duarte (2003) Objectos nulos em debate. In I. Castro e I. Duarte (orgs.) Razões e emoção: miscelânea de estudos em homenagem a Maria Helena Mateus, Vol. I, Lisboa: Imprensa Nacional Casa da Moeda; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2006) A aquisição de clíticos em PE: omissão de clíticos ou objecto nulo?. In XXI Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Textos seleccionados. Lisboa: APL; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2007a) Clitic omission, null objects or both in the acquisition of European Portuguese?. In S. Baauw, F. Drijkonongen & M. Pinto, eds. Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory Amsterdam: John Benjamins; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2007b) Complexidade e omissão de clíticos: o caso dos reflexos. In A. Coutinho & M. Lobo (eds.) XXII Encontro Nacional da APL. Lisboa: APL/Colibri; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2008) Omissão de clíticos na aquisição do português europeu: dados da compreensão. In Textos Seleccionados, XXIII Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, APL/Colibri, Lisboa; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2009) Clitic Omission in the Acquisition of European Portuguese: Data from comprehension. In A. Pires & J. Rothman eds. Minimalist

28 Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition: Case Studies across Portuguese, Berlin/ New York: Mouton de Gruyter; Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2010a) Objecto nulo na aquisição do PE: pro ou variável? Paper presented at 26º Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Costa, João & Maria Lobo (2010b) Clitic omission is null object: evidence from comprehension. In J. Costa, A. Castro, M. Lobo & F. Pratas, orgs. Proceedings of GALA2009. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing. Costa, João, Maria Lobo, Jaqueline Carmona & Carolina Silva (2008) Clitic omission in European Portuguese: correlation with null objects?. In A. Gavarró & M. J. Freitas (orgs). Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2007, Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing; Costa, João, Maria Lobo & Carolina Silva (2009) Null objects and early pragmatics in the acquisition of European Portuguese. Probus 21; Duarte, Inês & Gabriela Matos (2000) Romance Clitics and the Minimalist Program. In Costa, J. (org.) Portuguese Syntax. New Comparative Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press; Gonçalves, Fernanda (2005) Riqueza morfológica e aquisição da sintaxe em português europeu e brasileiro, PhD dissertation, Univ. Évora. Grüter, T. (2006) Object clitics and null objects in the acquisition of French. PhD Dissertation, McGill University, Montreal. Guasti, Maria-Teresa (2002) Language Acquisition. The growth of grammar. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Jakubowicz, C. et al. (1998) Determiners and Clitic pronouns in French speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition 7.2; Matos, Gabriela (1992) Construções de elipse do predicado em português SV nulo e despojamento. PhD dissertation, Universidade de Lisboa Miyagawa, Shigeru (2010) Primacy of Person Agreement: Revisiting Jaeggli and Safir s Morphological Uniformity for Pro-drop. Talk given at MIT. Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Mihaela Pirvulescu & Yves Roberge (2006) Early Object Omission in Child French and English. In Montreuil, Jean-Pierre and Nishida, Chiyo (eds.) Selected papers from LSRL New Perspectives in Romance Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

29 Raposo, E. P. (1986) On the Null Object Construction in European Portuguese. In Studies in Romance Linguistics. orgs. Jaeggli e Silva-Corvalán. Dordrecht: Foris; Santos, Ana Lúcia (2006) Minimal Answers. Ellipsis, syntax and discourse in the acquisition of European Portuguese. PhD dissertation. Univ. de Lisboa. Schaeffer, J. (1997) Direct object scrambling in Dutch and Italian child language, UCLA Dissertations in Linguistics, 17. Silva, Carolina (2008) Assimetrias na aquisição de clíticos diferenciados em português europeu. MA dissertation. Faculdade de Ciências Sociais e Humanas, Universidade Nova de Lisboa. Silva, Carolina (2010) Asymmetries in the acquisition of different types of clitics in European Portuguese. In V. Torrens, L. Escobar, A. Gavarró, J. Gutiérrez (eds.) Movement and Clitics: Adult and Child Grammar. Newcastle, Cambridge Scholars Publishing; Tsakali, Vina & Kenneth Wexler (2003) Why children omit clitics in some languages but not in others: new evidence from Greek. Paper presented at Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2003, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands. Varlokosta, Spyridoula et al. (in preparation) A Cross-Linguistic Study of the Acquisition of Clitic and Pronoun Production. Wexler, Kenneth (1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. In Lingua 106; Wexler, Kenneth, Anna Gavarró & Vicent Torrens (2003) Feature checking and object clitic omission in child Catalan and Spanish. In R. Bok-Bennema, B. Hollebrandse, B. Kampers-Manhe e P. Sleeman (eds.) Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory. Selected Papers from Going Romance Amsterdam, John Benjamins.

Gapping in European Portuguese. Gonçalo Silva

Gapping in European Portuguese. Gonçalo Silva Gapping in European Portuguese Gapping has been classically characterized as an ellipsis. However, recent literature (Johnson 2009) has regarded it as Across-the-Board movement in the English language.

More information

Minimal Projections: evidence from defective constructions in European Portuguese

Minimal Projections: evidence from defective constructions in European Portuguese CatWPL 7, 1999 59-69 Minimal Projections: evidence from defective constructions in European Portuguese João Costa Universidade Nova de Lisboa Av. Berna, 26-C. 1050 Lisboa. Portugal a.j.costa@mail.telepac.pt

More information

Pronouns: A case of production-before-comprehension

Pronouns: A case of production-before-comprehension Faculty of Arts University of Groningen Pronouns: A case of production-before-comprehension A study on the the comprehension and production of pronouns and reflexives in Dutch children Petra Hendriks Jennifer

More information

Semantics and Generative Grammar. Quantificational DPs, Part 3: Covert Movement vs. Type Shifting 1

Semantics and Generative Grammar. Quantificational DPs, Part 3: Covert Movement vs. Type Shifting 1 Quantificational DPs, Part 3: Covert Movement vs. Type Shifting 1 1. Introduction Thus far, we ve considered two competing analyses of sentences like those in (1). (1) Sentences Where a Quantificational

More information

Phases and autonomous features: a case of mixed agreement in European Portuguese

Phases and autonomous features: a case of mixed agreement in European Portuguese Phases and autonomous features: a case of mixed agreement in European Portuguese João Costa & Sandra Pereira Universidade Nova de Lisboa & Centro de Linguística da Universidade de Lisboa This paper studies

More information

VP ellipsis in European and Brazilian Portuguese a comparative analysis

VP ellipsis in European and Brazilian Portuguese a comparative analysis Published in: Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 1(2):177-195, 2002. VP ellipsis in European and Brazilian Portuguese a comparative analysis SONIA M. L. CYRINO (Universidade Estadual de Londrina, Brasil/CNPq)

More information

19. Morphosyntax in L2A

19. Morphosyntax in L2A Spring 2012, April 5 Missing morphology Variability in acquisition Morphology and functional structure Morphosyntax in acquisition In L1A, we observe that kids don t always provide all the morphology that

More information

Clitic Climbing in L2 Portuguese

Clitic Climbing in L2 Portuguese Clitic Climbing in L2 Portuguese Alexandra Fiéis, Ana Madeira, and Maria Francisca Xavier 1. Introduction Clitic climbing (henceforth, CC), or the raising of a clitic pronoun to a position adjacent to

More information

ADVERBS AND THE SYNTAX-SEMANTICS INTERPLAY

ADVERBS AND THE SYNTAX-SEMANTICS INTERPLAY ADVERBS AND THE SYNTAX-SEMANTICS INTERPLAY JOÃO COSTA (Universidade Nova de Lisboa) ABSTRACT: Recent literature on the placement and interpretation of adverbs has tried to explain crosslinguistic fixed

More information

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006 Overview of topics What is Syntax? Word Classes What to remember and understand: Ling 201 Syntax 1 Jirka Hana April 10, 2006 Syntax, difference between syntax and semantics, open/closed class words, all

More information

Movement and Binding

Movement and Binding Movement and Binding Gereon Müller Institut für Linguistik Universität Leipzig SoSe 2008 www.uni-leipzig.de/ muellerg Gereon Müller (Institut für Linguistik) Constraints in Syntax 4 SoSe 2008 1 / 35 Principles

More information

Non-nominal Which-Relatives

Non-nominal Which-Relatives Non-nominal Which-Relatives Doug Arnold, Robert D. Borsley University of Essex The properties of non-restrictive relatives All non-restrictive relative clauses include a wh-word. There are no that or zero

More information

The Syntax of Ellipsis Resolution: Eye-tracking Evidence from φ-feature Mismatches

The Syntax of Ellipsis Resolution: Eye-tracking Evidence from φ-feature Mismatches The Syntax of Ellipsis Resolution: Eye-tracking Evidence from φ-feature Mismatches Helena Aparicio, Katie Franich, Ming Xiang University of Chicago NELS 45 October 30, 2014 1 / 71 Ellipsis: The Phenomenon

More information

The Root Infinitive Stage in Afrikaans: Evidence for Phonologically Neutralized Rich

The Root Infinitive Stage in Afrikaans: Evidence for Phonologically Neutralized Rich Title: The Root Infinitive Stage in Afrikaans: Evidence for Phonologically Neutralized Rich Agreement Running Head: The RI-Stage in Afrikaans: Evidence for Null AGR 1 Abstract: Corpus data available on

More information

Constraints in Phrase Structure Grammar

Constraints in Phrase Structure Grammar Constraints in Phrase Structure Grammar Phrase Structure Grammar no movement, no transformations, context-free rules X/Y = X is a category which dominates a missing category Y Let G be the set of basic

More information

Negation and (finite) verb placement in a Polish-German bilingual child. Christine Dimroth, MPI, Nijmegen

Negation and (finite) verb placement in a Polish-German bilingual child. Christine Dimroth, MPI, Nijmegen Negation and (finite) verb placement in a Polish-German bilingual child Christine Dimroth, MPI, Nijmegen Consensus: Bilingual L1A bilingual children develop two separate linguistic systems from early on

More information

The binomial distribution and proportions. Erik-Jan Smits & Eleonora Rossi

The binomial distribution and proportions. Erik-Jan Smits & Eleonora Rossi The binomial distribution and proportions Erik-Jan Smits & Eleonora Rossi Seminar in statistics and methodology 2005 Sampling distributions Moore and McCabe (2003:367) : Nature of sampling distribution

More information

COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH. FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr

COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH. FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr It has become common to analyse comparatives by using degrees, so that John is happier than Mary would

More information

CAS LX 500 A1 Language Acquisition

CAS LX 500 A1 Language Acquisition CAS LX 500 A1 Language Acquisition Week 4a. Root infinitives, null subjects and the UCC Root infinitives vs. time Here are those Danish graphs again. Ooo. Consistent. Syntax at age two Root infinitives

More information

Assessing the discourse referential properties of weak definites

Assessing the discourse referential properties of weak definites Assessing the discourse referential properties of weak definites TIN-dag 2010 Jolien Scholten & Ana Aguilar Guevara February 6, 2010 NWO project Weak referentiality: bare nominals at the lexicon-syntax-semantics

More information

Being a Female Engineering Student: Lessons from the Industrial Engineering

Being a Female Engineering Student: Lessons from the Industrial Engineering Being a Female Engineering Student: Lessons from the Industrial Engineering Celina P. Leão 1, Carina Pimentel 2, Cristina S. Rodrigues 3 Abstract - The increasing of women engineers participation in a

More information

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES Zhen Qin, Allard Jongman Department of Linguistics, University of Kansas, United States qinzhenquentin2@ku.edu, ajongman@ku.edu

More information

The syntactic positions of adverbs and the Second Language Acquisition

The syntactic positions of adverbs and the Second Language Acquisition September 2010, Volume 7, No.9 (Serial No.81) Sino-US English Teaching, ISSN 1539-8072, USA The syntactic positions of adverbs and the Second Language Acquisition ZHANG Zi-hong (Department of Foreign Language

More information

Comparing because to want; How connectives affect the processing of causal relations.

Comparing because to want; How connectives affect the processing of causal relations. MAD 2010, Moissac, 17 20 march 2010 Comparing because to want; How connectives affect the processing of causal relations. Anneloes Canestrelli (1), Ted Sanders (2), Pim Mak (3) (1)Uil- OTS, Utrecht University

More information

IP PATTERNS OF MOVEMENTS IN VSO TYPOLOGY: THE CASE OF ARABIC

IP PATTERNS OF MOVEMENTS IN VSO TYPOLOGY: THE CASE OF ARABIC The Buckingham Journal of Language and Linguistics 2013 Volume 6 pp 15-25 ABSTRACT IP PATTERNS OF MOVEMENTS IN VSO TYPOLOGY: THE CASE OF ARABIC C. Belkacemi Manchester Metropolitan University The aim of

More information

PortuguesePod101.com Learn Portuguese with FREE Podcasts

PortuguesePod101.com Learn Portuguese with FREE Podcasts Introduction First Impressions: Wow Your Portuguese Friends! Portuguese English Vocabulary Phrase Usage 3 Grammar Points 3 Cultural Insight 4 Portuguese Oi. Meu nome é. Oi. Meu nome é. Prazer em conhecê-la.

More information

bound Pronouns

bound Pronouns Bound and referential pronouns *with thanks to Birgit Bärnreuther, Christina Bergmann, Dominique Goltz, Stefan Hinterwimmer, MaikeKleemeyer, Peter König, Florian Krause, Marlene Meyer Peter Bosch Institute

More information

Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science +Business Media New York. This e-offprint is for

Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science +Business Media New York. This e-offprint is for 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science +Business Media New York. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be selfarchived in electronic

More information

EVIDENCE FOR A CUE-BASED THEORY OF LANGUAGE CHANGE AND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION THE NULL OBJECT IN BRAZILIAN PORTUGUESE 1

EVIDENCE FOR A CUE-BASED THEORY OF LANGUAGE CHANGE AND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION THE NULL OBJECT IN BRAZILIAN PORTUGUESE 1 versão final publicada em: Lopes, R. & Cyrino, S. Evidence for a cue-based theory of language change and language acquisition -the null object in Brazilian Portuguese Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory,

More information

Language Meaning and Use

Language Meaning and Use Language Meaning and Use Raymond Hickey, English Linguistics Website: www.uni-due.de/ele Types of meaning There are four recognisable types of meaning: lexical meaning, grammatical meaning, sentence meaning

More information

Syntax-driven bindings of Spanish clitic pronouns

Syntax-driven bindings of Spanish clitic pronouns Procesamiento del Lenguaje Natural, núm. 35 (2005), pp. 253-257 recibido 30-04-2005; aceptado 0-06-2005 Syntax-driven bindings of Spanish clitic pronouns Ivan V. Meza Ruiz ICCS University of Edinburgh

More information

The compositional semantics of same

The compositional semantics of same The compositional semantics of same Mike Solomon Amherst College Abstract Barker (2007) proposes the first strictly compositional semantic analysis of internal same. I show that Barker s analysis fails

More information

Appendix to Chapter 3 Clitics

Appendix to Chapter 3 Clitics Appendix to Chapter 3 Clitics 1 Clitics and the EPP The analysis of LOC as a clitic has two advantages: it makes it natural to assume that LOC bears a D-feature (clitics are Ds), and it provides an independent

More information

CARLA CONTEMORI Curriculum Vitae January 2010

CARLA CONTEMORI Curriculum Vitae January 2010 PERSONAL INFORMATION DoB 03/11/1982 Address: Viale Lombardia 12 53042 Chianciano Terme (SI) Italy E-mail: contemori7@unisi.it CARLA CONTEMORI Curriculum Vitae January 2010 RESEARCH INTERESTS - Typical

More information

The role of prosody in toddlers interpretation of verbs argument structure

The role of prosody in toddlers interpretation of verbs argument structure Workshop Early Language Acquisition - July 5, 2012 The role of prosody in toddlers interpretation of verbs argument structure Isabelle Dautriche*, Alexjandrina Cristia*, Perrine Brusini*, Sylvia Yuan #,

More information

Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada

Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada ADVERB ORIENTATION: SEMANTICS AND PRAGMATICS José María García Núñez Universidad de Cádiz Orientation is a well known property of some adverbs in English. Early

More information

Nominative-Dative Inversion and the Decline of Dutch

Nominative-Dative Inversion and the Decline of Dutch Nominative-Dative Inversion and the Decline of Dutch Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of Amsterdam (E-mail: weerman@uva.nl) 1. Introduction Recently, linguistics was in the centre

More information

The Problem of Fragment Answers 1

The Problem of Fragment Answers 1 The Problem of Fragment Answers 1 Ana Lúcia Santos als@fl.ul.pt Universidade de Lisboa Abstract: In this paper, I discuss the status of fragment answers to yes-no questions, based on facts from European

More information

Cross-linguistic differences in the interpretation of sentences with more than one QP: German (Frey 1993) and Hungarian (É Kiss 1991)

Cross-linguistic differences in the interpretation of sentences with more than one QP: German (Frey 1993) and Hungarian (É Kiss 1991) Cross-linguistic differences in the interpretation of sentences with more than one QP: German (Frey 1993) and Hungarian (É Kiss 1991) 1. Quantifier Scope in English (May 1977, 1985) Predictions of May

More information

Module: Security Research Methods. Professor Patrick McDaniel Fall 2011. CSE543 - Introduction to Computer and Network Security

Module: Security Research Methods. Professor Patrick McDaniel Fall 2011. CSE543 - Introduction to Computer and Network Security CSE543 - Introduction to Computer and Network Security Module: Security Research Methods Professor Patrick McDaniel Fall 2011 1 Reading papers What is the purpose of reading papers? How do you read papers?

More information

Paraphrasing controlled English texts

Paraphrasing controlled English texts Paraphrasing controlled English texts Kaarel Kaljurand Institute of Computational Linguistics, University of Zurich kaljurand@gmail.com Abstract. We discuss paraphrasing controlled English texts, by defining

More information

UTILIZATION OF INTERPRETERS

UTILIZATION OF INTERPRETERS 1. INTRODUCTION UTILIZATION OF INTERPRETERS People with limited English proficiency (LEP) and people who are deaf or hard of hearing face many barriers to health and mental health care. When communication

More information

Young Learners English

Young Learners English University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations Young Learners English Flyers Information for Candidates Information for candidates YLE Flyers Dear Parent Thank you for encouraging your child to learn English

More information

Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar

Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar Contents A Cheat Sheet on the Tenses in English... 2 Conditionals... 3 Past Participles... 5 Present Perfect Tense... 6 Present Perfect Continuous... 6 Past

More information

English Descriptive Grammar

English Descriptive Grammar English Descriptive Grammar 2015/2016 Code: 103410 ECTS Credits: 6 Degree Type Year Semester 2500245 English Studies FB 1 1 2501902 English and Catalan FB 1 1 2501907 English and Classics FB 1 1 2501910

More information

Syntactic and Semantic Differences between Nominal Relative Clauses and Dependent wh-interrogative Clauses

Syntactic and Semantic Differences between Nominal Relative Clauses and Dependent wh-interrogative Clauses Theory and Practice in English Studies 3 (2005): Proceedings from the Eighth Conference of British, American and Canadian Studies. Brno: Masarykova univerzita Syntactic and Semantic Differences between

More information

Constituency. The basic units of sentence structure

Constituency. The basic units of sentence structure Constituency The basic units of sentence structure Meaning of a sentence is more than the sum of its words. Meaning of a sentence is more than the sum of its words. a. The puppy hit the rock Meaning of

More information

CINTIL-PropBank. CINTIL-PropBank Sub-corpus id Sentences Tokens Domain Sentences for regression atsts 779 5,654 Test

CINTIL-PropBank. CINTIL-PropBank Sub-corpus id Sentences Tokens Domain Sentences for regression atsts 779 5,654 Test CINTIL-PropBank I. Basic Information 1.1. Corpus information The CINTIL-PropBank (Branco et al., 2012) is a set of sentences annotated with their constituency structure and semantic role tags, composed

More information

Read this newspaper article and complete the tasks below.

Read this newspaper article and complete the tasks below. CHAPTER 6 The Present Perfect FORM 1 Examining Form Read this newspaper article and complete the tasks below. county: a geographical and political division within a state in the U.S. 1. There are five

More information

Extended Projections of Adjectives and Comparative Deletion

Extended Projections of Adjectives and Comparative Deletion Julia Bacskai-Atkari 25th Scandinavian Conference University of Potsdam (SFB-632) in Linguistics (SCL-25) julia.bacskai-atkari@uni-potsdam.de Reykjavík, 13 15 May 2013 0. Introduction Extended Projections

More information

Free reflexives: Reflexives without

Free reflexives: Reflexives without Nordic Atlas of Language Structures (NALS) Journal, Vol. 1, 522 526 C opyright Björn Lundquist 2014 Licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License Free reflexives: Reflexives without a sentence

More information

Book Review. Byrd, Steven. Calunga and the legacy of an African language in Brazil. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 2012. Pp. 278.

Book Review. Byrd, Steven. Calunga and the legacy of an African language in Brazil. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 2012. Pp. 278. Book Review Byrd, Steven. Calunga and the legacy of an African language in Brazil. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 2012. Pp. 278. Reviewed by TJERK HAGEMEIJER Description Calunga is a currently

More information

CS4025: Pragmatics. Resolving referring Expressions Interpreting intention in dialogue Conversational Implicature

CS4025: Pragmatics. Resolving referring Expressions Interpreting intention in dialogue Conversational Implicature CS4025: Pragmatics Resolving referring Expressions Interpreting intention in dialogue Conversational Implicature For more info: J&M, chap 18,19 in 1 st ed; 21,24 in 2 nd Computing Science, University of

More information

MODERN WRITTEN ARABIC. Volume I. Hosted for free on livelingua.com

MODERN WRITTEN ARABIC. Volume I. Hosted for free on livelingua.com MODERN WRITTEN ARABIC Volume I Hosted for free on livelingua.com TABLE OF CcmmTs PREFACE. Page iii INTRODUCTICN vi Lesson 1 1 2.6 3 14 4 5 6 22 30.45 7 55 8 61 9 69 10 11 12 13 96 107 118 14 134 15 16

More information

Agreement, PRO and Imperatives

Agreement, PRO and Imperatives Agreement, PRO and Imperatives Hans Bennis 1. Introduction: the pro drop parameter 1 In the literature the occurrence of an empty subject in finite clauses is generally related to a parametric choice in

More information

The impact of embedded pronouns on children's relative clause comprehension in Italian

The impact of embedded pronouns on children's relative clause comprehension in Italian The impact of embedded pronouns on children's relative clause comprehension in Italian Yair Haendler & Flavia Adani University of Potsdam Going Romance 2014 Centro de Linguística da Universidade da Lisboa

More information

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT Pete Bibby Dept of Psychology 1 About Laboratory Reports The writing of laboratory reports is an essential part of the practical course One function of this course is to

More information

Using eye-tracking to study anaphoric relations processing in European Portuguese

Using eye-tracking to study anaphoric relations processing in European Portuguese Using eye-tracking to study anaphoric relations processing in European Portuguese 1 A Costa, G Matos, P Luegi Faculdade de Letras da Universidade de Lisboa CLUL, PORTUGAL 1 armandacosta@fl.ul.pt ABSTRACT

More information

Light verbs features in European Portuguese

Light verbs features in European Portuguese Light verbs features in European Portuguese Inês Duarte * Anabela Gonçalves * Matilde Miguel * Amália Mendes * Iris Hendrickx * Fátima Oliveira α Luís Filipe Cunha α Fátima Silva α Purificação Silvano

More information

Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language

Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language Tone Spanish: In both English and Spanish there are four tone levels, but Spanish speaker use only the three lower pitch tones, except when

More information

CHILDREN S METALINGUISTIC ACTIVITY IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF LINGUISTIC EXISTENCE *

CHILDREN S METALINGUISTIC ACTIVITY IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF LINGUISTIC EXISTENCE * Psychology of Language and Communication 1999, Vol. 3. No. 2 OTÍLIA DA COSTA E SOUSA Higher School of Education, Lisbon New University of Lisbon CHILDREN S METALINGUISTIC ACTIVITY IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF

More information

Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent

Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent The theses of the Dissertation Nominal and Verbal Plurality in Sumerian: A Morphosemantic

More information

How do absolute beginners learn inflectional morphology? (French learners of Polish)

How do absolute beginners learn inflectional morphology? (French learners of Polish) How do absolute beginners learn inflectional morphology? (French learners of Polish) Rebekah Rast (American University of Paris & UMR-SFL, CNRS) Marzena Watorek (University Paris 8 & UMR-SFL, CNRS) Agnieszka

More information

ENGLISH GRAMMAR Elementary

ENGLISH GRAMMAR Elementary ENGLISH GRAMMAR Elementary A cura di NADIA TOSI Centro IDA ITE Enrico Tosi Busto Arsizio CONTENTS DEFINITE ARTICLES 3 INDEFINITE ARTICLES 3 PERSONAL PRONOUNS SUBJECTS 4 PLURALS. 5 POSSESSIVE ADJECTIVES

More information

GRS LX 865 Topics in Linguistics

GRS LX 865 Topics in Linguistics GRS LX 865 Topics in Linguistics Week 3. Experimental design and the responsible use of numbers (I) Why we do experiments In this context, we re generally interested in the state and developmental course

More information

Data Coding and Entry Lessons Learned

Data Coding and Entry Lessons Learned Chapter 7 Data Coding and Entry Lessons Learned Pércsich Richárd Introduction In this chapter we give an overview of the process of coding and entry of the 1999 pilot test data for the English examination

More information

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1 Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida The critical reader of a research report expects the writer to

More information

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation The grammar of our first language is learnt naturally and implicitly through interactions with other speakers and from reading. Explicit knowledge

More information

CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE. CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4. CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7. CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages 8-10. CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14

CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE. CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4. CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7. CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages 8-10. CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14 CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4 CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7 CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages 8-10 CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14 CHARTE NIVEAU C1 Pages 15-17 MAJ, le 11 juin 2014 A1 Skills-based

More information

Estudios de Asia y Africa Idiomas Modernas I What you should have learnt from Face2Face

Estudios de Asia y Africa Idiomas Modernas I What you should have learnt from Face2Face Estudios de Asia y Africa Idiomas Modernas I What you should have learnt from Face2Face 1A Question Forms 1.1 Yes-No Questions 1. If the first verb is an auxiliary verb, just move it in front of the Subject:

More information

Pronouns. Their different types and roles. Devised by Jo Killmister, Skills Enhancement Program, Newcastle Business School

Pronouns. Their different types and roles. Devised by Jo Killmister, Skills Enhancement Program, Newcastle Business School Pronouns Their different types and roles Definition and role of pronouns Definition of a pronoun: a pronoun is a word that replaces a noun or noun phrase. If we only used nouns to refer to people, animals

More information

UNIVERSIDAD TECNOLOGICA DE PEREIRA LICENCIATURA EN LENGUA INGLESA SEMINARIO DE GRADO RECASTS AND THEIR USE IN SECOND LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

UNIVERSIDAD TECNOLOGICA DE PEREIRA LICENCIATURA EN LENGUA INGLESA SEMINARIO DE GRADO RECASTS AND THEIR USE IN SECOND LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT 1 UNIVERSIDAD TECNOLOGICA DE PEREIRA LICENCIATURA EN LENGUA INGLESA SEMINARIO DE GRADO RECASTS AND THEIR USE IN SECOND LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT ANGELA MARIA PEREZ SANTACOLOMA JAVIER CORDOBA SOTO PEREIRA 2013

More information

Spanish Reflexive Pronouns: a Null Preposition Hypothesis *

Spanish Reflexive Pronouns: a Null Preposition Hypothesis * Spanish Reflexive Pronouns: a Null Preposition Hypothesis * Jonathan E. MacDonald Stony Brook University 1. Introduction This paper discusses two types of reflexive pronouns in Spanish. The first is the

More information

The Phenomenon of Private Tutoring: Implications for Public Education

The Phenomenon of Private Tutoring: Implications for Public Education Global Education Review, ISSN 2220-1599 September 2013, Vol. 1, No.5, 70-74 The Phenomenon of Private Tutoring: Implications for Public Education António V. Bento University of Madeira (Portugal) Maria

More information

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF UBC FACULTY SALARIES: INVESTIGATION OF

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF UBC FACULTY SALARIES: INVESTIGATION OF STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF UBC FACULTY SALARIES: INVESTIGATION OF DIFFERENCES DUE TO SEX OR VISIBLE MINORITY STATUS. Oxana Marmer and Walter Sudmant, UBC Planning and Institutional Research SUMMARY This paper

More information

Measurement in ediscovery

Measurement in ediscovery Measurement in ediscovery A Technical White Paper Herbert Roitblat, Ph.D. CTO, Chief Scientist Measurement in ediscovery From an information-science perspective, ediscovery is about separating the responsive

More information

CALICO Journal, Volume 9 Number 1 9

CALICO Journal, Volume 9 Number 1 9 PARSING, ERROR DIAGNOSTICS AND INSTRUCTION IN A FRENCH TUTOR GILLES LABRIE AND L.P.S. SINGH Abstract: This paper describes the strategy used in Miniprof, a program designed to provide "intelligent' instruction

More information

Scandinavian Dialect Syntax Transnational collaboration, data collection, and resource development

Scandinavian Dialect Syntax Transnational collaboration, data collection, and resource development Scandinavian Dialect Syntax Transnational collaboration, data collection, and resource development Janne Bondi Johannessen, Signe Laake, Kristin Hagen, Øystein Alexander Vangsnes, Tor Anders Åfarli, Arne

More information

Collateral Feature Discharge

Collateral Feature Discharge Collateral Feature Discharge Daniela Henze & Eva Zimmermann ConSOLE XIX, Groningen January 6, 2011 Henze & Zimmermann (ConSOLE XIX) Collateral Feature Discharge January 6, 2011 1 / 40 Introduction Blocking

More information

Evaluation of a Segmental Durations Model for TTS

Evaluation of a Segmental Durations Model for TTS Speech NLP Session Evaluation of a Segmental Durations Model for TTS João Paulo Teixeira, Diamantino Freitas* Instituto Politécnico de Bragança *Faculdade de Engenharia da Universidade do Porto Overview

More information

Is there repair by ellipsis?

Is there repair by ellipsis? Is there repair by ellipsis? Craig Sailor University of Groningen cwsailor@gmail.com Carson T. Schütze UCLA cschutze@ucla.edu Draft: December, 2014 Written for The book of syntactic questions 100 ideas

More information

Date of birth: 4th May 1977 Place of birth: Rome Citizenship: Italian

Date of birth: 4th May 1977 Place of birth: Rome Citizenship: Italian V VINCENZO MOSCATI PERSONAL INFORMATION Date of birth: 4th May 1977 Place of birth: Rome Citizenship: Italian EDUCATION July 2006: June 2002: Ph.D. in Cognitive Science (European Doctorate) - Linguistics

More information

Right Node Raising and the LCA

Right Node Raising and the LCA 1 Right Node Raising and the LCA CHRIS WILDER 1 Constituent sharing in coordination In (1), a typical right node raising (RNR) sentence, the object the book is a constituent shared by the verbs of both

More information

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 15

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 15 Mind on Statistics Chapter 15 Section 15.1 1. A student survey was done to study the relationship between class standing (freshman, sophomore, junior, or senior) and major subject (English, Biology, French,

More information

Superiority: Syntax or Semantics? Düsseldorf Jul02. Jill devilliers, Tom Roeper, Jürgen Weissenborn Smith,Umass,Potsdam

Superiority: Syntax or Semantics? Düsseldorf Jul02. Jill devilliers, Tom Roeper, Jürgen Weissenborn Smith,Umass,Potsdam Superiority: Syntax or Semantics? Düsseldorf Jul02 Jill devilliers, Tom Roeper, Jürgen Weissenborn Smith,Umass,Potsdam Introduction I. Question: When does a child know the grammaticality difference between

More information

Writing the Empirical Social Science Research Paper: A Guide for the Perplexed. Josh Pasek. University of Michigan.

Writing the Empirical Social Science Research Paper: A Guide for the Perplexed. Josh Pasek. University of Michigan. Writing the Empirical Social Science Research Paper: A Guide for the Perplexed Josh Pasek University of Michigan January 24, 2012 Correspondence about this manuscript should be addressed to Josh Pasek,

More information

Imposters: An Online Survey of Grammaticality Judgments Chris Collins, Stephanie N. Guitard and Jim Wood

Imposters: An Online Survey of Grammaticality Judgments Chris Collins, Stephanie N. Guitard and Jim Wood Imposters: An Online Survey of Grammaticality Judgments Chris Collins, Stephanie N. Guitard and Jim Wood 1 Introduction In this paper we develop a simple online survey technique for collecting grammaticality

More information

A Brief Introduction to Mathematical Writing

A Brief Introduction to Mathematical Writing A Brief Introduction to Mathematical Writing William J. Turner August 26, 2009 1 Introduction Mathematics is not just about computation. It is about ideas, knowledge, learning, understanding, and perception.

More information

UNBOUND ANAPHORIC PRONOUNS: E-TYPE, DYNAMIC, AND STRUCTURED-PROPOSITIONS APPROACHES

UNBOUND ANAPHORIC PRONOUNS: E-TYPE, DYNAMIC, AND STRUCTURED-PROPOSITIONS APPROACHES FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN UNBOUND ANAPHORIC PRONOUNS: E-TYPE, DYNAMIC, AND STRUCTURED-PROPOSITIONS APPROACHES ABSTRACT. Unbound anaphoric pronouns or E-type pronouns have presented notorious problems for semantic

More information

An Experiment on the Effect of Design Recording on Impact Analysis

An Experiment on the Effect of Design Recording on Impact Analysis An Experiment on the Effect of Design Recording on Impact Analysis F. Abbattista, F. Lanubile, G. Mastelloni, and G. Visaggio Dipartimento di Informatica University of Bari, Italy Abstract An experimental

More information

Young Learners English

Young Learners English University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations Young Learners English Movers Information for Candidates Information for candidates YLE Movers Dear Parent Thank you for encouraging your child to learn English

More information

136 CHAPTER 4. INDUCTION, GRAPHS AND TREES

136 CHAPTER 4. INDUCTION, GRAPHS AND TREES 136 TER 4. INDUCTION, GRHS ND TREES 4.3 Graphs In this chapter we introduce a fundamental structural idea of discrete mathematics, that of a graph. Many situations in the applications of discrete mathematics

More information

Para-educator/Parent Training Package on Toilet Training (Short Term)

Para-educator/Parent Training Package on Toilet Training (Short Term) Para-educator/Parent Training Package on Toilet Training (Short Term) CONTENTS page INTRODUCTION 1 BEGINNING TOILET TRAINING 2 TOILET TRAINING 7 Step 1 Tell/Help 7 2 Dry Pants Check 10 3 Tell/Touch 11

More information

PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR

PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR DOKTORI DISSZERTÁCIÓ HALM TAMÁS THE GRAMMAR OF FREE-CHOICE ITEMS IN HUNGARIAN THESIS BOOKLET NYELVTUDOMÁNYI DOKTORI ISKOLA ELMÉLETI NYELVÉSZET MŰHELY

More information

Grammar Unit: Pronouns

Grammar Unit: Pronouns Name: Miss Phillips Period: Grammar Unit: Pronouns Unit Objectives: 1. Students will identify personal, indefinite, and possessive pronouns and recognize antecedents of pronouns. 2. Students will demonstrate

More information

of knowledge that is characteristic of the profession and is taught at professional schools. An important author in establishing this notion was

of knowledge that is characteristic of the profession and is taught at professional schools. An important author in establishing this notion was Mathematics teacher education and professional development João Pedro da Ponte jponte@fc.ul.pt Grupo de Investigação DIFMAT Centro de Investigação em Educação e Departamento de Educação Faculdade de Ciências

More information

Study Plan for Master of Arts in Applied Linguistics

Study Plan for Master of Arts in Applied Linguistics Study Plan for Master of Arts in Applied Linguistics Master of Arts in Applied Linguistics is awarded by the Faculty of Graduate Studies at Jordan University of Science and Technology (JUST) upon the fulfillment

More information

Publications Tanja Kupisch. 1 Monographs and Textbooks

Publications Tanja Kupisch. 1 Monographs and Textbooks 1 Monographs and Textbooks 1. Kupisch, T. 2006. The acquisition of determiners in bilingual German-Italian and German-French children. München: Lincom Europa. ISBN 9783895869969. 2. Müller, N., T. Kupisch,

More information

Recognizing then as an Adverb: An Experiment with Brazilian Learners of English

Recognizing then as an Adverb: An Experiment with Brazilian Learners of English Recognizing then as an Adverb: An Experiment with Brazilian Learners of English Marina Augusto State University of Campinas/Fapesp 1. Introduction This study focuses on the acquisition of foreign language

More information

Writing a Course Paper. Capella University 225 South 6th Street, 9th Floor Minneapolis, MN 55402 1-888-CAPELLA (227-3552)

Writing a Course Paper. Capella University 225 South 6th Street, 9th Floor Minneapolis, MN 55402 1-888-CAPELLA (227-3552) Writing a Course Paper Capella University 225 South 6th Street, 9th Floor Minneapolis, MN 55402 1-888-CAPELLA (227-3552) Table of Contents Creating Major Sections... 3 Writing Fundamentals... 7 Expressing

More information