THE MEANING OF RANDOMNESS FOR SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "THE MEANING OF RANDOMNESS FOR SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS"

Transcription

1 THE MEANING OF RANDOMNESS FOR SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS Carmen Batanero and Luis Serrano University of Granada, Spain Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(5), , 1999 (Extended version) SUMMARY Subjective perception of randomness has been researched by psychologists using a variety of production and judgement tasks, resulting in a number of different descriptions for the biases that characterise people's performances. These research findings, especially those concerned with children s and adolescents' understanding of randomness, are highly pertinent to didactic practices, as new mathematics curricula for compulsory teaching levels are being proposed that incorporate increased study of random phenomena. In this article, we first analyse the complexity of the meaning of randomness from the mathematical point of view and outline philosophical controversies associated with this. Secondly, we complement previous research by comparing the meaning of randomness for 277 secondary students in two age groups (14 and 17 year-olds), through the identification of the mathematical properties they associate to random and deterministic sequences and to two-dimensional distributions. Some implications for teaching and future research are then suggested. New mathematics curricula are being proposed in different countries that place more emphasis on the study of randomness and probability and that suggest the performing of simulation experiments, based on random sequences. For example, in the Spanish curriculum for compulsory secondary-level teaching (MEC, 1992), we find the following topic: "Random phenomena and the associated terminology". Reference is made to "using the vocabulary adapted to describe and quantify random situations" and to building frequency tables to represent the random phenomena s behavior". Also, within the list of algorithms and skills to develop, "obtaining random numbers with various devices" and the "detection of common mistakes in the interpretation of chance" are suggested. In the general strategies we find "recognizing random phenomena in everyday life and in scientific knowledge", "formulating and testing conjectures on the behavior of simple random phenomena" and "planning and performing simple experiments to study the behavior of random phenomena". This curriculum is not an exception, since similar terms or expressions are found in recent curricula adopted in other countries, for example those in France, England and Wales, and the United States. However, epistemological analysis of the concept, as well as psychological research results have shown that the meaning of randomness is not easy for statistically naive people. There is an apparent contradiction in people s understanding of random processes and sequences, which is related to the psychological problems associated with the concept, namely that randomness implies that anything possible might occur. Subjectively, however, many people believe that only the outcomes without visible patterns are permissible examples of randomness (Hawkins et al, 1991, p.104). Despite the relevance of the topic in probability and statistics, little research has been

2 The meaning of randomness... 1 carried out within mathematics education and most psychological research has merely concentrated on the accuracy of people s judgement of randomness. Before introducing a new topic in the curriculum, a fundamental issue is to determine students preconceptions, because they inevitably construct their own knowledge by combining their present experiences with their existing conceptions. Shaughnessy (1996) also suggest the need of documenting the changes (or lack of change) of students thinking over time and after instruction to identify the problematic teaching areas in which we need to revise our curriculum or instructional practices (Shaughnessy, 1997). The experimental study reported here was intended to examine possible differences in secondary students conceptions of randomness, before and after instruction in probability, which occurs in the Spanish curriculum between the ages of 14 and 17. At the same time we wanted to explore the similarities between our student s conceptions and various historical conceptions of randomness. To achieve these aims, the written responses of 277 secondary students in two different groups ( 14 and 17 year-olds) to some test items taken from Green (1989, 1991) concerning the perception of randomness in sequences and two dimensional distributions are analyzed from different points of view. Our results show that students subjective meaning of randomness is close to some interpretations that randomness has received throughout history. Because of the fact that Green s studies were carried out with years old students, we also provide a complement to this research by extending its results to 17 year-old students. In the following sections all these aspects will be described, starting off with a brief analysis of the mathematical meaning of randomness and some philosophical controversies relating thereto, as well as a summary of earlier research on which our study is based. MATHEMATICAL MEANING OF RANDOMNESS Randomness has received various interpretations at different periods in history. Even today, there are still serious difficulties involved in defining randomness (Fine, 1973; Zabell, 1992; Toohey, 1995). Randomness and causality An early meaning of randomness was that it was the opposite of something that had some known causes. Chance was then assumed to be the cause of random phenomena. This meaning was given to randomness throughout the period extending from antiquity to the beginning of the Middle Ages: Nothing happens by chance, but rather everything occurs for a reason and a necessity (Leucippus, Vth Century BC, quoted by Bennet, 1993). If we adopt this meaning, randomness is due to our ignorance and has a subjective nature. Poincaré (1936), however, found this meaning to be unsatisfactory because, if we accept it, certain phenomena with unknown laws (such as death) would be considered to be deterministic. Among the phenomena for which the laws are unknown, Poincaré chose to differentiate between random phenomena, for which probability calculus would give us some information, and those non-random phenomena, for which there is no possibility of prediction until we discover their laws. As Ayer (1974) stated, a

3 The meaning of randomness... 2 phenomenon is only considered random if it behaves in accordance with probability calculus, and this definition will still hold even when we have found the rules for the phenomenon. Randomness and probability With the first theoretical developments of probability, randomness was related to equi-probability (for example in the Liber of Ludo Aleae by Cardano), because this development was closely linked to games of chance, where the number of possibilities is finite and the principle of equal probabilities for the elementary events of the sample space in a simple experiment is reasonable. Nowadays, we sometimes find randomness explained in terms of probability, although such an explanation would depend on the underlying understanding of probability. If we adopt a classic interpretation, we say that an object (or an event) is a random member of a given class, if there is the same probability for selecting this than for any other member of its class. This definition of randomness may be sufficient for random games based on dice, coins, etc., but Kyburg (1974) suggests that it imposes severe, non-natural restrictions on its applications. We can, for example, only consider that an object is a random member of a class if this class is finite. If the class is infinite then the probability associated to each member is always null, and therefore still identical, even when the selection method is biased. Furthermore, this particular explanation precludes any consideration of randomness applied to elementary events that are not equi-probable. When we transfer the applications of probability to the physical or natural world, for example studying the blood type of a new-born baby or any other hereditary characteristic, we cannot rely on the equi-probability principle. Here, we may consider an object as a random member of a class if we can select it using a method providing a given a priori relative frequency to each member of this class in the long run. Thus, we use the frequentist basis of probability, which is most appropriate when we have data from enough cases. However, we are left with the theoretical problem of deciding how many experiments it is necessary to consider in order to be sure that we have sufficiently proven the random nature of the object. Within either of these two frameworks, randomness is an objective property assigned to the event or element of a class. Kyburg (1974) criticizes this view and proposes an interpretation of randomness composed of the following four terms: - the object that is supposed to be a random member of a class; - the set of which the object is a random member (population or collective); - the property with respect to which the object is a random member of the given class; - the knowledge of the person giving the judgement of randomness. Whether an object is considered to be a random member of a class or not, depends, under this interpretation, on our knowledge. This view, supported by the subjective conception of probability, is more appropriate when we have some information affecting our judgement about the randomness of an event. Formalization of randomness At the end of the Ith Century, Edgeworth, Galton, Pearson, Fisher and their

4 The meaning of randomness... 3 collaborators started to base statistical analysis in applied research on random samples of data. Before this period, rather than being oriented towards inference, statistics was often confined to the descriptive study of complete populations. When samples were used, they were not selected at random, because there was not an awareness of the need for this if valid and precise inferences about the population were to be achieved. Once theoretical developments began to show the importance of random sampling, then interest in finding models for processes providing long sequences of random digits was born. With the advent of tables of pseudo-random numbers, concern about how to ensure the quality of those numbers began to emerge. The possibility of obtaining pseudorandom digits with deterministic algorithms also suggested the need for examining the sequence produced, regardless of the process by which it had been generated. It was discussions about such things that led to the formalization of the concept of randomness (Fine, 1973). Von Mises based his study of this topic on the intuitive idea that a sequence is considered to be random if we are convinced of the impossibility of finding a method that lets us win in a game of chance where winning depends on forecasting that sequence. He defined the randomness of a sequence by proposing the invariance of the relative frequency for each possible event in all the possible sub-sequences of the given sequence. This definition of randomness is the basis for statistical tests that are used for checking random number tables before presenting them to the scientific community. However, since in all statistical tests there is the possibility of error, we can never be totally certain that a given sequence, in spite of having passed all the tests, does not have some unnoticed pattern within it. Thus, we cannot be absolutely sure about the randomness of a particular finite sequence. We only take a decision about its randomness with reference to the outcomes of test techniques and instruments. This explains why a computer-generated random sequence (which is not random in an absolute sense) can still be random in a relative sense (Harten & Steinbring, 1983). Another attempt to define the randomness of a sequence was based on its computational complexity. Kolmogorov s interpretation of randomness reflected the difficulty of describing it (or storing it in a computer) using a code that allows us to reconstruct it afterwards. In this approach, a sequence would be random if it cannot be codified in a more parsimonious way, and the absence of patterns is its essential characteristic. The minimum number of signs necessary to code a particular sequence provides a scale for measuring its complexity, so this definition allows for a hierarchy in the degrees of randomness for different sequences. It is important to remark that in both theoretical approaches perfect randomness would only apply to sequences of infinite outcomes and therefore, randomness would only be a theoretical concept. SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION OF RANDOMNESS There has been a considerable amount of research into adults subjective perception of randomness (e.g., Wagenaar, 1972; Falk, 1981; Bar-Hillel and Wagenaar 1991). These psychologists have used a variety of stimulus tasks, which have been classified in a recent review by Falk and Konold (1977) into two main types. In the first type (generation tasks), subjects generate random sequences under standard instructions to

5 The meaning of randomness... 4 simulate a series of outcomes from a typical random process, such as tossing a coin. The second type of research approach uses recognition tasks. People are asked to select the most random of several sequences of results that might have been produced by a random device or to decide whether some given sequences were produced by a random mechanism. Similar types of research have also been performed using two-dimensional random distributions, which essentially consist of random distributions of points on a squared grill. In these investigations, systematic biases have consistently been found. One such bias is known as the gambler's fallacy, by which people believe that, after a long run of a same result in a random process, the probability of that event occurring in the following trial is lower. Related to this is the tendency of people in sequence generation tasks to include too many alternations of different results (such as heads and tails in the flipping of a coin) in comparison to what would theoretically be expected in a random process. Similarly, in perception tasks people tend to reject sequences with long runs of the same result (such as a long sequence of heads) and consider sequences with an excess of alternation of different results to be random. Comparable results are found in people s performance with two-dimensional tasks, in which clusters of points seem to prevent a distribution from being perceived as random. In addition to various psychological mechanisms such as local representativeness (Kahneman et al, 1982) that may be in operation and may explain these biases, some authors (e.g. Falk, 1981; and Falk and Konold, in press) believe that individual consistency in people's performance with diverse tasks suggests underlying misconceptions about randomness. Konold et al (1993) used a different type of task consisting of having people judge the randomness of different natural phenomena. The results suggested that subjects conceptions about random experiments could be classified into the following categories that we believe parallel the historical interpretations of randomness outlined earlier: - Subjects for whom an experiment is random, only if the possible results are equally probable. If the probabilities of the events involved are very different, they would not be considered to be random - such as raining on a day for which the possibility of it raining has been forecast as 80%. - Randomness as opposed to causality, or as a special type of cause. - Randomness as uncertainty; existence of multiple possibilities in the same conditions. - Randomness as a model to represent some phenomenon, dependent upon our information about it. Research into children's conception of randomness From a didactic point of view, a crucial question is whether these biases and misconceptions are spontaneously acquired or whether they are a consequence of poor instruction in probability. Below, we outline a number of key research studies looking at children s and adolescents conceptions of randomness, and their performance when faced with tasks requiring the generation or recognition of sequences of random results. According to Piaget and Inhelder, chance is due to the interference of a series of independent causes, and the non presence of all the possible outcomes when there are only a few repetitions of an experiment. Each isolated case is indeterminate or unpredictable, but the set of possibilities may be found using combinatorial reasoning,

6 The meaning of randomness... 5 thereby making the outcome predictable. This notion of probability is based on the ratio between the number of possible ways for a particular case to occur and the number of all possible outcomes. This would suggest that chance and probability cannot be totally understood until combinatorial and proportional reasoning are developed and, for Piaget, this does not happen until a child reaches the formal operations stage (12-14 years). Piaget and Inhelder (1951) investigated children s understanding of patterns in twodimensional random distributions. They designed a piece of apparatus to simulate rain drops falling on paving stones. The desire for regularity appeared to dominate the young children s predictions. When they were asked where the following rain drop would fall, children at stage 1 (6 to 9 years) allocated the rain drops in approximately equal numbers on each pavement square, thereby producing a uniform distribution. With older children, proportional reasoning begins to develop, and Piaget and Inhelder report that such children tolerate more irregularity in the distribution. They believed that children understood the law of the large numbers, which explains the global regularity and the particular variability of each experiment simultaneously. Green s (1983) findings, however, contradict those of Piaget and Inhelder. His investigations with 2930 children aged 11-16, using paper and pencil versions of piagetian tasks, showed that the percentage of children recognizing random or semirandom distributions actually decreased with age. A second study with 1600 pupils aged 7 to 11 and 225 pupils aged 13 to 14 is described in Green (1989, 1991). Using a slightly different task, Green found that general reasoning ability was a significant factor influencing children s responses, and concluded that more detailed investigations of children s concepts of randomness were needed. In his second study, Green gave the children generation and recognition tasks related to a random sequence of heads and tails representing the results of flipping a fair coin. The study demonstrated that children were able to describe what was meant by equiprobable. However, they did not appear to understand the independence of the trials, and tended to produce series in which runs of the same result were too short compared to those that we would expect in a random process. In both studies, Green found that the perception of random sequences does not improve with age. Children did base their decisions on the following properties of the sequences: results pattern, number of runs of the same result, frequencies of results, and unpredictability of random events. However, these properties were not always correctly associated to randomness or determinism. Toohey (1995) repeated some of Green's studies with year-old students and concluded that some children have only a local perspective of randomness, while that of other children is entirely global. The local perspective of randomness emphasized the spatial arrangement of the outcomes within each square, while the global perspective concentrates on the frequency distribution of outcomes. Other authors, such as Fischbein and Gazit (1984) and Fischbein et al (1991), have also documented children's difficulties in differentiating random and deterministic aspects, and their beliefs in the possibility of controlling random experiments. In contrast to the Piagetian view, these authors have inclined towards stating that even very young children display important intuitions and precursor concepts of randomness. They argue that it is not didactically sound to delay exploiting and building on these subjective intuitions until the formal operations stage is reached.

7 The meaning of randomness... 6 AIMS AND METHODOLOGY OF THE EPERIMENTAL RESEARCH In the following we present the results from a wider study (Serrano, 1996) in which 277 secondary school students were asked to answer a written class questionnaire including generation and recognition tasks, which were taken from Green (1991). About half of the students (n=147) were in their third year of secondary school (14 years-old) and had not studied probability. The rest of the students (n=130) were in their last year of secondary education (17-18 years-old, pre-university level). This second group had studied probability with a formal, mathematical approach for about a month when they were 15 years old and for another month during their previous school year. Five different schools in the town of Melilla (Spain) were used. The test took between 45 and 60 minutes to complete. Our research was intended to compare secondary students conception of randomness before and after instruction in probability, which occurs in the Spanish curriculum between the ages of 14 and 17. As Shaughnessy (1996) points out, there is still a need to document the growth of students understanding of probability concepts over time. We also wanted to refine Green s research methodology, in particular to permit further analysis of children s reasoning when judging the randomness of twodimensional distributions, and to compare students conceptions with some of the interpretations that randomness has received along history. In this paper we present the responses to recognition rather than generation tasks. In Konold and Falk's opinion(in press), these reflect subjective conceptions of randomness more directly, because people may be able to perceive randomness, even if they are not able to generate an example of it. A total of eight items will be analyzed, four of which refer to random sequences and the reaming to random two-dimensional distributions. For each type of item (random sequences and two-dimensional distributions) we first compare the percentages of students considering the situation to be random, using the Chi-squared test of independence between the response and group of students. Then, the students arguments supporting or rejecting randomness are classified and compared by items, age group and type of judgement (randomness/ determinism), using again the Chisquared test. Synthesis of the general tendency in these subjective meanings of randomness is finally achieved through correspondence analysis. This is a multivariate data analysis technique, which us applicable to qualitative data and serves to identify factors in a multiway contingency table. In our case, the 2216 responses (277 students x8 items) were classified by item, type of response (random or fake) and argument provided (7 different categories). The type of item (random sequence or two dimensional distribution) and group of student were used as supplementary variables, which does not intervene in the analysis, but are plotted on the graphs to help in improving the interpretations of factors. Recognition of properties of random sequences Firstly, we analyze the responses to items 1 to 4, in which we studied the students capacity for discriminating random models in Bernoulli sequences. Two different variables were changed in the different sequences: a) The proportion of heads: In items 1 to 3 this proportion is very close to the

8 The meaning of randomness... 7 theoretical probability (P(H)=.53 in item 1, P(H)=.48 in items 2 and 3), while in item 4 the proportion of heads in only.30. b)the length of runs, and, consequently the proportion of alternations (change in the type of outcome, from head to tail or from tail to head). This proportion is the quotient between the number of alternations and the sequence length and is very close to the theoretical value.5 in items 1 ( P(A)=.54), 2 and 3 (P(A)=.51). On the contrary P(A)=.74 in item 2 that have too many alternations (too short runs) to be considered to be random. Therefore, from a normative point of view, we would consider that the correct response to items 1 and 3 is that the child was playing correctly and for items 2 and 4 that the child was cheating. However, as in a test of randomness there is always a small probability that the sequence is not random, in spite of having passed the randomness tests, we are not just interested in whether the students responses coincide or not with the normative answer, but in the properties of sequences that associate to randomness. Items 1 to 4. Some children were each told to toss a coin 40 times. Some did it properly. Others just made it up. They put H for Heads and T for tails. MarÌa: T T T H T H H T T T H T H H H H H T H T H T T H T H H T T T T H H H T H H T H H Daniel: H T H T T H H T H T H H T T H T T H H T T H T H H T T H T H T H T H T H T T H T MartÌn: H T T T H T T H H H T H T T T T T H T H T H H T H T T H H H H T T T H T T H H H Diana: H T T T H T T H T H T T T H T T T T H H T T T H T T H T T H T T T T H T T T H T Item 1: Did MarÌa make it up? How can you tell? Item 2: Did Daniel make it up? How can you tell? Item 3: Did MartÌn make it up? How can you tell? Item 4: Did Diana make it up? How can you tell? The students' answers to these four items are presented in Table 1. Most students considered all the sequences to be random, except that in item 4, in which the frequency of heads (12) is quite different from the theoretical frequency expected in a random sequence. Fifty-four 14 year-old students and thirty 17 year-old students gave a positive response to item 2, but most of the students considered the sequence to be random, in spite that the number of alternations (and consequently the length of runs) has been biased. This result confirms Green s findings (1991) that suggested that the students have more difficulty in recognizing runs properties than frequency properties. The consistent percentages of students considering the sequences to be random in items 1, 2 and 3 indicates that the similarity between observed and expected frequencies may be more important than the length of the runs in deciding whether a sequence is random. In all items, the Chi-squared test of independence between response and students group yielded a significant result (p-values were equal to.0011,.0045,.0028, and.0192 in items 1 to 4). A smaller proportion of 17-year-old students considered that the sequences were made up, and a higher proportion did not reach a conclusion. This also coincided with Green s findings, and suggest that the older students are more cautious in their judgment, because they better appreciate the variability of random phenomena. Table 1: Percentages of students' responses to items 1-4 and levels of significance in the

9 The meaning of randomness... 8 Chi-squared test Sequence Response Age=14 Age=17 Maria: P(H)=.53 Real P(A)=.54 Fake No decision 6 15 Daniel: P(H)=.48 Real P(A)=.74 Fake No decision 5 14 Martin: P(H)=.48 Real P(A)=.51 Fake No decision 5 15 Diana: P(H)=.30 Real P(A)=.51 Fake No decision 8 15 We asked the students to justify their answers. The reasons that they gave were classified according to the scheme followed by Green (1991), which is described below. In some cases students might be reasoning according to the representativeness heuristic described by Kahneman et al (1982), where people tend to estimate the likelihood for an event based on how well it represent some aspects of the parent population. a) There is a regular pattern in the sequence: This reasoning refers to the order in which heads and tails appear in the sequence, and to the regularity of this pattern: "He did not cheat, because there is a very regular sequence of heads and tails, which is most probable to happen", "He might have made it up because the results are very uniform; heads and tails almost alternate". b) There is an irregular pattern in the sequence: For example, "He did not make it up, because the sequence does not follow any order". In the last example or argument (b) the student associates the lack of pattern to randomness, which agree with the complexity approach to randomness described before, where the absence of patterns is an essential characteristic. c) The frequencies of the different results are quite similar: For example, "Because the numbers of heads and tails are very balanced", "The proportion of heads and tails is very similar", "The results are about 50-50". d) The frequencies of the different results are quite different: This is the opposite of the previous reasoning. These two reasons, (c) and (d), are both based on some sort of comparison between the observed frequencies and the theoretical probability distribution. Here, the frequentist approach to probability and randomness, where an object is considered as a random member of a class if we can select it using a method providing a given a priori relative frequency in the long run, might underlay. e) There are long runs: Here students show misconceptions concerning the idea of independence between successive trials: "Because there are many consecutive tails". Again, the students might be relying on the representativeness heuristic here. f) There are no runs: This is the opposite to the previous reason, for example, "There are too few sequences of heads".

10 The meaning of randomness... 9 g) The probability of different events must be the same: This reason is related to (d), but here there is an allusion to probability. A few subjects responses do fall into this category, stating for example, "There must be equal probability for heads and tails". In this case students relate randomness to equiprobability, using a classical approach to this concept, where an object is a random member of a class if there is the same probability for this object and for any other member of its class. h) It is unpredictable; it is random: Students giving this response might say, for example, "That is luck", "Though correctly flipped it can give those results, because it is a game", "When flipping a coin, there is no certain result, here randomness plays a very important role". Here the outcome approach described by Konold, where people interpret questions about probability in a non probabilistic way and rely on the unpredictability of random events to reject taking a decision. Belief in an underlying causal mechanism, which parallel the earlier meaning of randomness, might also be implicit in some of these categories of student explanations. This point is difficult to decide with our data, and would require further research using interviews and a more varied types of tasks. As shown in Table 2, the highest frequencies appear in responses based on unpredictability or luck, somewhat in contrast to Green s findings (1991). We are also able to detect variations in the more common responses to the different sequences: regular pattern in item 1; irregular pattern in items 2 and 4; difference of frequencies in item 2, 3 and 4; long runs in items 2 and 3. This variation suggests that the students were able to discriminate between the features of the different sequences. Green (1991) did not look at variation by items in this way. Table 2: Percentages of students arguments in Items 1-4 according age MarÌa MartÌn Daniel Diana Argument Regular pattern Irregular pattern Similar frequencies Different frequencies Runs Impredictibility No argument In each item we carried out the chi squared test of independence between the argument and group of students (p-values were equal to.001,.002,.0001 and.02). Younger students made more reference to the pattern of a sequence, whereas 17 yearold students used frequency arguments more often. As it is shown in Table 3, the responses that they gave also differed depending on whether the sequence was considered to be random or not (p. values in the Chi-squared test of independence between argument and type of response were equal to.0001 in the four items). The main argument given to support randomness was unpredictability. Irregular pattern, long runs, and non-coincidence of the outcome frequencies were associated with lack of randomness. Table 3: Percentage of students arguments in items 1 to 4 according to whether the sequence is considered to be random or not

11 The meaning of randomness MarÌa MartÌn Daniel Diana Argument Real Fake Real Fake Real Fake Real Fake Regular pattern Irregular pattern Similar frequencies Different frequencies Runs Impredictibility No argument RECOGNITION OF PROPERTIES IN TWO-DIMENSIONAL ARRAYS We included four further items in which students were requested to indicate their perception about whether the points in a two-dimensional array were distributed randomly or not. Our aim was to analyze the properties attributed by students to random two-dimensional distributions and the differences between the two age-groups. In order to make the stochastic model clear for the students the following introductory activity was set: Paul plays a game using 16 counters numbered 1, 2, 3, 4, Paul puts all the counters in a tin. He shakes the tin a lot. Rachel shuts her eyes and picks out a counter. It is number 7. Paul puts a cross in box 7. The 7 is put back in the tin and someone else picks out a counter After reading out this text, children were told to play this game until they understood the rules. They were then given two items exploring the children's perception of what might happen in the experiment if continued over 16 and 30 selection (generation tasks). The last four questions (the recognition tasks) were as follows: Items 5 to 8: Some children were told to play the counters game by themselves using 16 real counters. Did some cheat and make it up? x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x Jaime Jes s 5. Did Jaime cheat? How can you tell? 6. Did Jes s cheat? How can you tell? x xx x x x x x x x x x x x xx x x MarÌa LuÌs 7. Did MarÌa cheat? How can you tell? 8. Did LuÌs cheat? H Table 4: Percentages of students' responses to items 5-8 Array Response Age=14 Age=17 Jaime: χ 2 =1 Real Fake No decision 1 3 Jes s: χ 2 =11 Real 57 58

12 The meaning of randomness Fake No decision 1 4 MarÌa: χ 2 =2.3 Real (Diagonal) Fake No decision 1 5 LuÌs: χ 2 =24.3 Real Fake No decision 1 3 In a theoretical random distribution of the number of counters, about 6 empty squares, 6 squares with only one counter, 3 with two counters and 1 with 3 o more counters will be expected. The chi-squared test of goodness of fit between the theoretical and the observed distribution in each of the items yielded a value of 1 for item 5 (Jaime), 11 for item 6 (Jes s), 2.3 for item 7 (MarÌa), and 24.3 for item 8 (Luis). In addition, Maria s pattern has too many adjacent empty squares, all of them on the main diagonal. Consequently, only Jaime s distribution would be considered random from a normative point of view. There was a greater spread in the number of students considering the distributions to be random now (Table 4). The percentage of students ranged from 85.1% and 85.4% in item 5, to 25.2% and 22.3 % in item 8, where exactly one point is distributed in each square. There were no significant differences depending on the age-group in the proportions considering the distributions to be random in the Chi-squared test. This is not surprising as, in Spain, teaching does not include this type of activity. The percentages were also similar to those obtained by Green (1991) in year old students. Toohey s study (1995) did not include such comparison data. In neither Green (1989, 1981) nor Toohey (1995) were the students asked for reasons to justify their answers, as they were in the present study. We have grouped the students responses into categories that can be compared with those used in items 1 to 4: a) There is a regular pattern in the distribution of points: Similarly to the previous items, the subjects noted the presence of some regularity in the spatial arrangement of results presented and argued that the points distribution followed a regular pattern. For example, "It is all too correct, all in its place, well ordered", "It is too lucky to have all the squares in the top right and bottom left corners with dots and nothing in the others". b)the sequence follows an irregular pattern: This is the opposite reason to the previous one. When the student associates the lack of pattern to randomness, the complexity approach to randomness might be implicit. c) The frequencies of the different results are similar: This characterizes the reasoning of students who find the frequencies of different results to be too similar. It was the favorite argument to justify item 5, referring to Luis. The following statements are variations on the same theme. "It is very difficult not to have some square repeated", "It is very difficult that all the squares should appear the same number of times". d) The frequencies of the different results are different: In contrast, subjects responding in this category gave more importance to the differences between the results presented: "The results are quite different, from an to 4 and therefore reasonable", "I believe that we should get more even results, not so different the number of times that squares are repeated".

13 The meaning of randomness We can observe the frequentist approach to probability implicit in these arguments c) and d). e) There is a cell with too many points: This argument is frequently used to justify items 6 and 8: "Some squares have too many crosses". This would be equivalent to an argument that the runs are too long in items 1-4, because it implies similar misconceptions about the idea of independence. The following arguments were hardly used; although we have included them in order to maintain consistency with the classification of arguments in items 1 to 4. f) There are no cells with several points. This is the reversal of the previous reason. g) It must be equal possibilities in the number of points by square. h) Unpredictability of the results in random experiments. Students giving this type of response seems to reason by the outcome approach or by belief in an underlying causal mechanism. For example, "Such is each person's luck, we cannot guess the result". Table 5:Percentages of students arguments in Items 5-8 and level of significance in the Chi-squared test Jaime Jes s MarÌa LuÌs Argument Regular pattern Irregular pattern Similar frequencies Different frequencies Clusters Unpredictability No argument The students responses are presented in Table 5. The more frequent reasons were: too regular pattern, irregular pattern, similar frequencies, different frequencies, cells with too many points, and unpredictability. The most frequent response did change according to the item, which suggests that the students are able to distinguish the characteristics presented in the distributions. Regular pattern is more frequently associated with items 7 and 8, irregular pattern with items 5 and 6, equality of frequencies with item 8, and cells with too many points with items 5 and 6. We found significant p-values in the Chi squared test of independence between student group and arguments, except in item 6 (p-values were equal to.004,.001, and.001 in items 5, 7, and 8). The older students seemed to favor frequency-based reasoning and unpredictability, whilst the 14 year-olds tended to comment on the regularity or irregularity of the pattern. Table 6: Percentages of students arguments according to whether the sequence is considered to be random or not Jaime Jes s MarÌa LuÌs Argument Real Fake Real Fake Real Fake Real Fake Regular pattern Irregular pattern

14 The meaning of randomness Similar frequencies Different frequencies Clusters Unpredictability No argument We also found significant p-values in the Chi-squared test of independence between argument and whether the student considered the distribution to be random or not (pvalues were equal to.0001,.009,.004, and.0001). In Table 6 we can see that the reasons given when the student did not believe that the distribution was random were; regular pattern, the equality of frequencies of the different results, and the existence of cells with too many points. The irregularity of pattern, and unpredictability, were more often used to support a belief in the randomness of the distribution. Factors affecting the perception of randomness One research issue was whether the students employed the same arguments to reject or to confirm randomness, and whether these arguments changed according to the task variables in the items. In the previous section we observed that particular responses appeared to be associated with different items. We have also found variations between the responses employed to justify saying that a sequence was random (the child was not cheating) or otherwise. Since the classification of responses was made to allow comparison in the eight situations proposed, we have completed our results with a correspondence analysis, using BMDP software, with the aim of identifying which task variables were associated with each argument. The results of the analysis support our hypothesis that students recognized the characteristics that we changed in the items and, furthermore, that they used them to decide if the situation was random or not. These characteristics may be summarized in terms of the three factors identified below which, between them, explain 87.5% of the total variance: 1. Students attribute a local variability to random phenomena. Because of this, they expect frequent alternation of results and a lack of pattern in the order or in the spatial arrangement of results. 2. Students expect a global regularity given by the similarity between the frequencies of different results and the theoretical probabilities. 3. However, some discrepancy (though not much) between the theoretical and the observed distribution is expected. These discrepancies are easily observed in the linear sequences, but are not so obvious in the items concerned with distributions of points in arrays. DISCUSSION OF FINDINGS Our results show a mixture of correct and incorrect properties associated by students to randomness. On one hand. they perceived the local variability, lack of patterns in the lineal or spatial arrangement of outcomes, and unpredictability of the random processes underlying the tasks we gave them. In many cases, our students carried out a statistical analysis of the frequencies for the different events in the random sequences and compared these frequencies with an underlying equiprobability model. As regards to

15 The meaning of randomness two dimensional distributions, they rejected the item with only one counter by square as well as that with too many adjacent squares. We should take into account all these correct intuitions when organizing the teaching of probability, because when learning something new students construct their own meaning by connecting the new information to what they already believe to be true. We also must consider the students mistaken conceptions a review our ways of teaching in the light of changes after instruction: There was a too strong emphasis on the unpredictability and luck to justify randomness and this tendency seemed to increase in the older students when analyzing the two dimensional distributions, a type tasks that are not considered in the Spanish curriculum. The existence of runs of 5 and even 4 identical results in the sequences (squares with cluster of 3-4 markers in two dimensional distributions) was mistakenly associated by the students to lack of randomness, though after instruction a smaller proportion of students provided this type of arguments. Because understanding the existence of runs and clusters implies understanding independence, our results suggest that independence is not an intuitive idea and that students continue having difficulties with the idea of independence after instruction. The students arguments and responses also suggest underlying conceptions that parallel some of the meanings that randomness has received along history. In particular, students relate randomness to luck and unknown causes, and to probability in its classical and frequentist approach. Some of the features of the complexity approach (lack of pattern) and selection algorithms (unpredictability) are also shown in their responses. All these results are very close to that by Green, and therefore, his results could be extended to our group of 17 year-olds students. In addition, we have extended the classification of students arguments which Green carried out for linear sequences to two-dimensional distributions and have achieved a general synthesis using correspondence analysis. In summary, our experimental results as well as our previous analysis reveal the complexity of the meaning of randomness for which different properties need to be understood. It may in fact be preferable to consider the term randomness as a label with which we associate many concepts, such as experiment, event, sample space, probability, etc (Konold et al., 1993). In this sense, the word randomness refers us to a collection of mathematical concepts and procedures that we can apply in many situations. We should think more about an orientation we take toward the phenomenon that we qualify as random rather than a quality thereof. We apply a mathematical model to the situation, because it is useful to describe it and to understand it. But we do not believe that the situation will be identical to the model. Deciding when probability is more convenient or adapted to the situation than other mathematical models is part of the work of modeling that we should encourage among our students. REFERENCES Ayer, A. J. (1974). El Azar [ Chance]. In Kline, M. (Ed.), Matemáticas en el mundo moderno [ Mathematics in modern world] (pp ). Barcelona: Blume. Ayton, P., Hunt, A. J., & Wrigth, G. (1989). Psychological conceptions of randomness. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 2, Batanero, C., & Serrano, L. (1995). La aleatoriedad, sus significados e implicaciones

16 The meaning of randomness educativas. Uno: Revista de didáctica de las matematicas, (5), Batanero, C., Green, D. R., & Serrano, L. R. (1998). Randomness, its meanings and educational implications. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 29(1), Batanero, C., Serrano, L., & Garfield, J. (1996). Heuristics and biases in secondary students' reasoning about probability. In Proceedings of the PME Conference (Vol. 2, pp ). Bar-Hillel, M., & Wagenaar, W. A. (1991). The perception of randomness. Advances in Applied Mathematics, 12, Bennett, D. J. (1993). The development of the mathematical concept of randomness; educational implications. Ph. D. New York University. (DAI n ). Falk, R. (1981). The perception of randomness. In C. Laborde (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp ). University of Grenoble. Falk, R., & Konold, C. (1994). Random means hard to digest. Focus on Learning Problem Solving in Mathematics, 16, Falk, R., & Konold, C. (In press). Making sense of randomness: Implicit encoding as a basis for judgment. Psychological Review. Fine, T. L. (1973). Theories of Probability: An examination of foundations. New York: Academic Press. Fischbein, E., & Gazit, A. (1984). Does the teaching of probability probabilistic intuitions? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15 (1), improve Fischbein, E., Nello, M. S., & Marino, M. S. (1991). Factors affecting probabilistic judgements in children and adolescents. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, Green, D. R. (1983). A Survey of probabilistic concepts in 3000 students aged years. In D.R. Grey et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the FirstInternational Conference on Teaching Statistics (v.2, pp ). University of Sheffield. Green, D.R. (1989). Schools students' understanding of randomness. In R. Morris (Ed.), Studies in Mathematics Education. v.7: The Teaching of Statistics (pp ). Paris: Unesco. Green, D.R. (1991). A longitudinal study of pupil's probability concepts. Technical Report ME90/91. Loughborough University of Technology. Hawkins, A., Jolliffe, F., & Glickman, L. (1991). Teaching Statistical Concepts. London: Longman. Harten, G., & Steinbring, H. (1983). Randomness and stochastic independence. On the relationship between intuitive and mathematical definition. In R. W. Scholz (Ed.), Decision making under uncertainty (pp ). Amsterdan: Reidel. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Konold, C. (1991). Understanding students' beliefs about probability. In E. von Glasersfeld (Ed.), Radical constructivism in mathematics education (pp ).

Fairfield Public Schools

Fairfield Public Schools Mathematics Fairfield Public Schools AP Statistics AP Statistics BOE Approved 04/08/2014 1 AP STATISTICS Critical Areas of Focus AP Statistics is a rigorous course that offers advanced students an opportunity

More information

Association Between Variables

Association Between Variables Contents 11 Association Between Variables 767 11.1 Introduction............................ 767 11.1.1 Measure of Association................. 768 11.1.2 Chapter Summary.................... 769 11.2 Chi

More information

Discrete Mathematics and Probability Theory Fall 2009 Satish Rao, David Tse Note 10

Discrete Mathematics and Probability Theory Fall 2009 Satish Rao, David Tse Note 10 CS 70 Discrete Mathematics and Probability Theory Fall 2009 Satish Rao, David Tse Note 10 Introduction to Discrete Probability Probability theory has its origins in gambling analyzing card games, dice,

More information

AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Ms. Foglia Date AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Why do we study random chance and probability at the beginning of a unit on genetics? Genetics is the study of inheritance,

More information

Problem of the Month: Fair Games

Problem of the Month: Fair Games Problem of the Month: The Problems of the Month (POM) are used in a variety of ways to promote problem solving and to foster the first standard of mathematical practice from the Common Core State Standards:

More information

LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Period Date LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics Why do we study random chance and probability at the beginning of a unit on genetics? Genetics is the study of inheritance,

More information

INTRODUCING THE NORMAL DISTRIBUTION IN A DATA ANALYSIS COURSE: SPECIFIC MEANING CONTRIBUTED BY THE USE OF COMPUTERS

INTRODUCING THE NORMAL DISTRIBUTION IN A DATA ANALYSIS COURSE: SPECIFIC MEANING CONTRIBUTED BY THE USE OF COMPUTERS INTRODUCING THE NORMAL DISTRIBUTION IN A DATA ANALYSIS COURSE: SPECIFIC MEANING CONTRIBUTED BY THE USE OF COMPUTERS Liliana Tauber Universidad Nacional del Litoral Argentina Victoria Sánchez Universidad

More information

It is remarkable that a science, which began with the consideration of games of chance, should be elevated to the rank of the most important

It is remarkable that a science, which began with the consideration of games of chance, should be elevated to the rank of the most important PROBABILLITY 271 PROBABILITY CHAPTER 15 It is remarkable that a science, which began with the consideration of games of chance, should be elevated to the rank of the most important subject of human knowledge.

More information

Expect the Unexpected When Teaching Probability

Expect the Unexpected When Teaching Probability Boise State University ScholarWorks Curriculum, Instruction, and Foundational Studies Faculty Publications and Presentations Department of Curriculum, Instruction, and Foundational Studies 3-1-2015 Expect

More information

Sampling and Sampling Distributions

Sampling and Sampling Distributions Sampling and Sampling Distributions Random Sampling A sample is a group of objects or readings taken from a population for counting or measurement. We shall distinguish between two kinds of populations

More information

Non-random/non-probability sampling designs in quantitative research

Non-random/non-probability sampling designs in quantitative research 206 RESEARCH MET HODOLOGY Non-random/non-probability sampling designs in quantitative research N on-probability sampling designs do not follow the theory of probability in the choice of elements from the

More information

Mathematical goals. Starting points. Materials required. Time needed

Mathematical goals. Starting points. Materials required. Time needed Level S2 of challenge: B/C S2 Mathematical goals Starting points Materials required Time needed Evaluating probability statements To help learners to: discuss and clarify some common misconceptions about

More information

The Loss of Intuition - A Lesson for the School Teacher?

The Loss of Intuition - A Lesson for the School Teacher? The Loss of Intuition - A Lesson for the School Teacher? Flavia R Jolliffe - Surrey, England 1. The study Although there is a long tradition of research into concepts and intuitions regarding randomness

More information

Chapter 6 Experiment Process

Chapter 6 Experiment Process Chapter 6 Process ation is not simple; we have to prepare, conduct and analyze experiments properly. One of the main advantages of an experiment is the control of, for example, subjects, objects and instrumentation.

More information

Teaching & Learning Plans. Plan 1: Introduction to Probability. Junior Certificate Syllabus Leaving Certificate Syllabus

Teaching & Learning Plans. Plan 1: Introduction to Probability. Junior Certificate Syllabus Leaving Certificate Syllabus Teaching & Learning Plans Plan 1: Introduction to Probability Junior Certificate Syllabus Leaving Certificate Syllabus The Teaching & Learning Plans are structured as follows: Aims outline what the lesson,

More information

Book Review of Rosenhouse, The Monty Hall Problem. Leslie Burkholder 1

Book Review of Rosenhouse, The Monty Hall Problem. Leslie Burkholder 1 Book Review of Rosenhouse, The Monty Hall Problem Leslie Burkholder 1 The Monty Hall Problem, Jason Rosenhouse, New York, Oxford University Press, 2009, xii, 195 pp, US $24.95, ISBN 978-0-19-5#6789-8 (Source

More information

Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials

Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials Version 1.0 May 011 1. Power of a Test. Factors affecting Power 3. Required Sample Size RELATED ISSUES 1. Effect Size. Test Statistics 3. Variation 4. Significance

More information

Current Standard: Mathematical Concepts and Applications Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary

Current Standard: Mathematical Concepts and Applications Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary A student shall apply concepts of shape, space, and measurement to solve problems involving two- and three-dimensional shapes by demonstrating an understanding of:

More information

Guide to Writing a Project Report

Guide to Writing a Project Report Guide to Writing a Project Report The following notes provide a guideline to report writing, and more generally to writing a scientific article. Please take the time to read them carefully. Even if your

More information

WRITING A CRITICAL ARTICLE REVIEW

WRITING A CRITICAL ARTICLE REVIEW WRITING A CRITICAL ARTICLE REVIEW A critical article review briefly describes the content of an article and, more importantly, provides an in-depth analysis and evaluation of its ideas and purpose. The

More information

Comparison of frequentist and Bayesian inference. Class 20, 18.05, Spring 2014 Jeremy Orloff and Jonathan Bloom

Comparison of frequentist and Bayesian inference. Class 20, 18.05, Spring 2014 Jeremy Orloff and Jonathan Bloom Comparison of frequentist and Bayesian inference. Class 20, 18.05, Spring 2014 Jeremy Orloff and Jonathan Bloom 1 Learning Goals 1. Be able to explain the difference between the p-value and a posterior

More information

Practical Research. Paul D. Leedy Jeanne Ellis Ormrod. Planning and Design. Tenth Edition

Practical Research. Paul D. Leedy Jeanne Ellis Ormrod. Planning and Design. Tenth Edition Practical Research Planning and Design Tenth Edition Paul D. Leedy Jeanne Ellis Ormrod 2013, 2010, 2005, 2001, 1997 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Chapter 1 The Nature and Tools of Research

More information

CHAPTER 2 Estimating Probabilities

CHAPTER 2 Estimating Probabilities CHAPTER 2 Estimating Probabilities Machine Learning Copyright c 2016. Tom M. Mitchell. All rights reserved. *DRAFT OF January 24, 2016* *PLEASE DO NOT DISTRIBUTE WITHOUT AUTHOR S PERMISSION* This is a

More information

Hypothesis Testing for Beginners

Hypothesis Testing for Beginners Hypothesis Testing for Beginners Michele Piffer LSE August, 2011 Michele Piffer (LSE) Hypothesis Testing for Beginners August, 2011 1 / 53 One year ago a friend asked me to put down some easy-to-read notes

More information

CONTENTS OF DAY 2. II. Why Random Sampling is Important 9 A myth, an urban legend, and the real reason NOTES FOR SUMMER STATISTICS INSTITUTE COURSE

CONTENTS OF DAY 2. II. Why Random Sampling is Important 9 A myth, an urban legend, and the real reason NOTES FOR SUMMER STATISTICS INSTITUTE COURSE 1 2 CONTENTS OF DAY 2 I. More Precise Definition of Simple Random Sample 3 Connection with independent random variables 3 Problems with small populations 8 II. Why Random Sampling is Important 9 A myth,

More information

Decision Making under Uncertainty

Decision Making under Uncertainty 6.825 Techniques in Artificial Intelligence Decision Making under Uncertainty How to make one decision in the face of uncertainty Lecture 19 1 In the next two lectures, we ll look at the question of how

More information

E3: PROBABILITY AND STATISTICS lecture notes

E3: PROBABILITY AND STATISTICS lecture notes E3: PROBABILITY AND STATISTICS lecture notes 2 Contents 1 PROBABILITY THEORY 7 1.1 Experiments and random events............................ 7 1.2 Certain event. Impossible event............................

More information

Cosmological Arguments for the Existence of God S. Clarke

Cosmological Arguments for the Existence of God S. Clarke Cosmological Arguments for the Existence of God S. Clarke [Modified Fall 2009] 1. Large class of arguments. Sometimes they get very complex, as in Clarke s argument, but the basic idea is simple. Lets

More information

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT Pete Bibby Dept of Psychology 1 About Laboratory Reports The writing of laboratory reports is an essential part of the practical course One function of this course is to

More information

AP Stats - Probability Review

AP Stats - Probability Review AP Stats - Probability Review Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. I toss a penny and observe whether it lands heads up or tails up. Suppose

More information

MTH6120 Further Topics in Mathematical Finance Lesson 2

MTH6120 Further Topics in Mathematical Finance Lesson 2 MTH6120 Further Topics in Mathematical Finance Lesson 2 Contents 1.2.3 Non-constant interest rates....................... 15 1.3 Arbitrage and Black-Scholes Theory....................... 16 1.3.1 Informal

More information

How Students Interpret Literal Symbols in Algebra: A Conceptual Change Approach

How Students Interpret Literal Symbols in Algebra: A Conceptual Change Approach How Students Interpret Literal Symbols in Algebra: A Conceptual Change Approach Konstantinos P. Christou (kochrist@phs.uoa.gr) Graduate Program in Basic and Applied Cognitive Science. Department of Philosophy

More information

6.042/18.062J Mathematics for Computer Science. Expected Value I

6.042/18.062J Mathematics for Computer Science. Expected Value I 6.42/8.62J Mathematics for Computer Science Srini Devadas and Eric Lehman May 3, 25 Lecture otes Expected Value I The expectation or expected value of a random variable is a single number that tells you

More information

Study Designs. Simon Day, PhD Johns Hopkins University

Study Designs. Simon Day, PhD Johns Hopkins University This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this

More information

A Study to Predict No Show Probability for a Scheduled Appointment at Free Health Clinic

A Study to Predict No Show Probability for a Scheduled Appointment at Free Health Clinic A Study to Predict No Show Probability for a Scheduled Appointment at Free Health Clinic Report prepared for Brandon Slama Department of Health Management and Informatics University of Missouri, Columbia

More information

Programmed Learning Review

Programmed Learning Review Programmed Learning Review L-HO1-121907 Take another sheet of paper and cover the answers located in the right hand column. Then read through the unit filling in the blanks as you go. After filling in

More information

Analysis of academy school performance in GCSEs 2014

Analysis of academy school performance in GCSEs 2014 Analysis of academy school performance in GCSEs 2014 Final report Report Analysis of academy school performance in GCSEs 2013 1 Analysis of Academy School Performance in GCSEs 2014 Jack Worth Published

More information

Statistical tests for SPSS

Statistical tests for SPSS Statistical tests for SPSS Paolo Coletti A.Y. 2010/11 Free University of Bolzano Bozen Premise This book is a very quick, rough and fast description of statistical tests and their usage. It is explicitly

More information

Chapter ML:IV. IV. Statistical Learning. Probability Basics Bayes Classification Maximum a-posteriori Hypotheses

Chapter ML:IV. IV. Statistical Learning. Probability Basics Bayes Classification Maximum a-posteriori Hypotheses Chapter ML:IV IV. Statistical Learning Probability Basics Bayes Classification Maximum a-posteriori Hypotheses ML:IV-1 Statistical Learning STEIN 2005-2015 Area Overview Mathematics Statistics...... Stochastics

More information

6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence

6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence 222 CHAPTER 6. PROBABILITY 6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence Conditional Probability Two cubical dice each have a triangle painted on one side, a circle painted on two sides and a square painted

More information

Reality in the Eyes of Descartes and Berkeley. By: Nada Shokry 5/21/2013 AUC - Philosophy

Reality in the Eyes of Descartes and Berkeley. By: Nada Shokry 5/21/2013 AUC - Philosophy Reality in the Eyes of Descartes and Berkeley By: Nada Shokry 5/21/2013 AUC - Philosophy Shokry, 2 One person's craziness is another person's reality. Tim Burton This quote best describes what one finds

More information

Welcome back to EDFR 6700. I m Jeff Oescher, and I ll be discussing quantitative research design with you for the next several lessons.

Welcome back to EDFR 6700. I m Jeff Oescher, and I ll be discussing quantitative research design with you for the next several lessons. Welcome back to EDFR 6700. I m Jeff Oescher, and I ll be discussing quantitative research design with you for the next several lessons. I ll follow the text somewhat loosely, discussing some chapters out

More information

Fun Learning Activities for Mentors and Tutors

Fun Learning Activities for Mentors and Tutors Fun Learning Activities for Mentors and Tutors Mentors can best support children s academic development by having fun learning activities prepared to engage in if the child needs a change in academic/tutoring

More information

CONTINGENCY TABLES ARE NOT ALL THE SAME David C. Howell University of Vermont

CONTINGENCY TABLES ARE NOT ALL THE SAME David C. Howell University of Vermont CONTINGENCY TABLES ARE NOT ALL THE SAME David C. Howell University of Vermont To most people studying statistics a contingency table is a contingency table. We tend to forget, if we ever knew, that contingency

More information

Problem of the Month: Cutting a Cube

Problem of the Month: Cutting a Cube Problem of the Month: The Problems of the Month (POM) are used in a variety of ways to promote problem solving and to foster the first standard of mathematical practice from the Common Core State Standards:

More information

Teaching Trends in Education - A Review of Iase

Teaching Trends in Education - A Review of Iase Training Future Statisticians to Teach Statistics Batanero, Carmen Faculty of Education, Campus de Cartuja Granada, 18071, Spain E-mail: batanero@ugr.es Díaz, Carmen Faculty of Psychology, Campus de Cartuja

More information

Statistics courses often teach the two-sample t-test, linear regression, and analysis of variance

Statistics courses often teach the two-sample t-test, linear regression, and analysis of variance 2 Making Connections: The Two-Sample t-test, Regression, and ANOVA In theory, there s no difference between theory and practice. In practice, there is. Yogi Berra 1 Statistics courses often teach the two-sample

More information

Exact Nonparametric Tests for Comparing Means - A Personal Summary

Exact Nonparametric Tests for Comparing Means - A Personal Summary Exact Nonparametric Tests for Comparing Means - A Personal Summary Karl H. Schlag European University Institute 1 December 14, 2006 1 Economics Department, European University Institute. Via della Piazzuola

More information

Testing Research and Statistical Hypotheses

Testing Research and Statistical Hypotheses Testing Research and Statistical Hypotheses Introduction In the last lab we analyzed metric artifact attributes such as thickness or width/thickness ratio. Those were continuous variables, which as you

More information

Teachers should read through the following activity ideas and make their own risk assessment for them before proceeding with them in the classroom.

Teachers should read through the following activity ideas and make their own risk assessment for them before proceeding with them in the classroom. Mathematical games Teacher notes Teachers should read through the following activity ideas and make their own risk assessment for them before proceeding with them in the classroom. Aims: To use mathematics

More information

Reflections on Probability vs Nonprobability Sampling

Reflections on Probability vs Nonprobability Sampling Official Statistics in Honour of Daniel Thorburn, pp. 29 35 Reflections on Probability vs Nonprobability Sampling Jan Wretman 1 A few fundamental things are briefly discussed. First: What is called probability

More information

Cognitive Development

Cognitive Development LP 9C Piaget 1 Cognitive Development Piaget was intrigued by the errors in thinking children made. To investigate how these errors and how thinking changes as we grow older, Jean Piaget carefully observed

More information

People have thought about, and defined, probability in different ways. important to note the consequences of the definition:

People have thought about, and defined, probability in different ways. important to note the consequences of the definition: PROBABILITY AND LIKELIHOOD, A BRIEF INTRODUCTION IN SUPPORT OF A COURSE ON MOLECULAR EVOLUTION (BIOL 3046) Probability The subject of PROBABILITY is a branch of mathematics dedicated to building models

More information

COMMON CORE STATE STANDARDS FOR

COMMON CORE STATE STANDARDS FOR COMMON CORE STATE STANDARDS FOR Mathematics (CCSSM) High School Statistics and Probability Mathematics High School Statistics and Probability Decisions or predictions are often based on data numbers in

More information

Section 6.2 Definition of Probability

Section 6.2 Definition of Probability Section 6.2 Definition of Probability Probability is a measure of the likelihood that an event occurs. For example, if there is a 20% chance of rain tomorrow, that means that the probability that it will

More information

An Automated Test for Telepathy in Connection with Emails

An Automated Test for Telepathy in Connection with Emails Journal of Scientifi c Exploration, Vol. 23, No. 1, pp. 29 36, 2009 0892-3310/09 RESEARCH An Automated Test for Telepathy in Connection with Emails RUPERT SHELDRAKE AND LEONIDAS AVRAAMIDES Perrott-Warrick

More information

psychology and economics

psychology and economics psychology and economics lecture 9: biases in statistical reasoning tomasz strzalecki failures of Bayesian updating how people fail to update in a Bayesian way how Bayes law fails to describe how people

More information

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready (SCCCR) Probability and Statistics

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready (SCCCR) Probability and Statistics South Carolina College- and Career-Ready (SCCCR) Probability and Statistics South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Mathematical Process Standards The South Carolina College- and Career-Ready (SCCCR)

More information

Likelihood: Frequentist vs Bayesian Reasoning

Likelihood: Frequentist vs Bayesian Reasoning "PRINCIPLES OF PHYLOGENETICS: ECOLOGY AND EVOLUTION" Integrative Biology 200B University of California, Berkeley Spring 2009 N Hallinan Likelihood: Frequentist vs Bayesian Reasoning Stochastic odels and

More information

Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010

Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010 Curriculum Map Statistics and Probability Honors (348) Saugus High School Saugus Public Schools 2009-2010 Week 1 Week 2 14.0 Students organize and describe distributions of data by using a number of different

More information

Statistics 2014 Scoring Guidelines

Statistics 2014 Scoring Guidelines AP Statistics 2014 Scoring Guidelines College Board, Advanced Placement Program, AP, AP Central, and the acorn logo are registered trademarks of the College Board. AP Central is the official online home

More information

Big Ideas in Mathematics

Big Ideas in Mathematics Big Ideas in Mathematics which are important to all mathematics learning. (Adapted from the NCTM Curriculum Focal Points, 2006) The Mathematics Big Ideas are organized using the PA Mathematics Standards

More information

FOR MORE, go to www.brookespublishing.com/classroom-management. Problem Behavior in My Classroom?

FOR MORE, go to www.brookespublishing.com/classroom-management. Problem Behavior in My Classroom? 3 So How Do I Prevent Problem Behavior in My Classroom? Your perspective, whether limited to your classroom or more broadly in life, directly affects how you interpret the events in your daily life. Developing

More information

NON-PROBABILITY SAMPLING TECHNIQUES

NON-PROBABILITY SAMPLING TECHNIQUES NON-PROBABILITY SAMPLING TECHNIQUES PRESENTED BY Name: WINNIE MUGERA Reg No: L50/62004/2013 RESEARCH METHODS LDP 603 UNIVERSITY OF NAIROBI Date: APRIL 2013 SAMPLING Sampling is the use of a subset of the

More information

Representing Geography

Representing Geography 3 Representing Geography OVERVIEW This chapter introduces the concept of representation, or the construction of a digital model of some aspect of the Earth s surface. The geographic world is extremely

More information

ALGEBRA. sequence, term, nth term, consecutive, rule, relationship, generate, predict, continue increase, decrease finite, infinite

ALGEBRA. sequence, term, nth term, consecutive, rule, relationship, generate, predict, continue increase, decrease finite, infinite ALGEBRA Pupils should be taught to: Generate and describe sequences As outcomes, Year 7 pupils should, for example: Use, read and write, spelling correctly: sequence, term, nth term, consecutive, rule,

More information

Introduction to Hypothesis Testing

Introduction to Hypothesis Testing I. Terms, Concepts. Introduction to Hypothesis Testing A. In general, we do not know the true value of population parameters - they must be estimated. However, we do have hypotheses about what the true

More information

Regular Languages and Finite Automata

Regular Languages and Finite Automata Regular Languages and Finite Automata 1 Introduction Hing Leung Department of Computer Science New Mexico State University Sep 16, 2010 In 1943, McCulloch and Pitts [4] published a pioneering work on a

More information

This chapter discusses some of the basic concepts in inferential statistics.

This chapter discusses some of the basic concepts in inferential statistics. Research Skills for Psychology Majors: Everything You Need to Know to Get Started Inferential Statistics: Basic Concepts This chapter discusses some of the basic concepts in inferential statistics. Details

More information

LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSIS

LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSIS LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSIS C. Mitchell Dayton Department of Measurement, Statistics & Evaluation Room 1230D Benjamin Building University of Maryland September 1992 1. Introduction and Model Logistic

More information

Commutative Property Grade One

Commutative Property Grade One Ohio Standards Connection Patterns, Functions and Algebra Benchmark E Solve open sentences and explain strategies. Indicator 4 Solve open sentences by representing an expression in more than one way using

More information

Topic #6: Hypothesis. Usage

Topic #6: Hypothesis. Usage Topic #6: Hypothesis A hypothesis is a suggested explanation of a phenomenon or reasoned proposal suggesting a possible correlation between multiple phenomena. The term derives from the ancient Greek,

More information

Chapter 4. Probability and Probability Distributions

Chapter 4. Probability and Probability Distributions Chapter 4. robability and robability Distributions Importance of Knowing robability To know whether a sample is not identical to the population from which it was selected, it is necessary to assess the

More information

Glencoe. correlated to SOUTH CAROLINA MATH CURRICULUM STANDARDS GRADE 6 3-3, 5-8 8-4, 8-7 1-6, 4-9

Glencoe. correlated to SOUTH CAROLINA MATH CURRICULUM STANDARDS GRADE 6 3-3, 5-8 8-4, 8-7 1-6, 4-9 Glencoe correlated to SOUTH CAROLINA MATH CURRICULUM STANDARDS GRADE 6 STANDARDS 6-8 Number and Operations (NO) Standard I. Understand numbers, ways of representing numbers, relationships among numbers,

More information

A Few Basics of Probability

A Few Basics of Probability A Few Basics of Probability Philosophy 57 Spring, 2004 1 Introduction This handout distinguishes between inductive and deductive logic, and then introduces probability, a concept essential to the study

More information

This thesis focuses on the early detection by Dutch Preventive Child Healthcare (PCH) of children with psychosocial problems and especially on the

This thesis focuses on the early detection by Dutch Preventive Child Healthcare (PCH) of children with psychosocial problems and especially on the 10 Summary 139 This thesis focuses on the early detection by Dutch Preventive Child Healthcare (PCH) of children with psychosocial problems and especially on the question whether short questionnaires can

More information

3. Data Analysis, Statistics, and Probability

3. Data Analysis, Statistics, and Probability 3. Data Analysis, Statistics, and Probability Data and probability sense provides students with tools to understand information and uncertainty. Students ask questions and gather and use data to answer

More information

Wastage among evening students at Technical Colleges I

Wastage among evening students at Technical Colleges I The Vocational Aspect of Education ISSN: 0305-7879 (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjve19 Wastage among evening students at Technical Colleges I James Brown To cite this

More information

Sampling Techniques Surveys and samples Source: http://www.deakin.edu.au/~agoodman/sci101/chap7.html

Sampling Techniques Surveys and samples Source: http://www.deakin.edu.au/~agoodman/sci101/chap7.html Sampling Techniques Surveys and samples Source: http://www.deakin.edu.au/~agoodman/sci101/chap7.html In this section you'll learn how sample surveys can be organised, and how samples can be chosen in such

More information

3. Logical Reasoning in Mathematics

3. Logical Reasoning in Mathematics 3. Logical Reasoning in Mathematics Many state standards emphasize the importance of reasoning. We agree disciplined mathematical reasoning is crucial to understanding and to properly using mathematics.

More information

Lab 11. Simulations. The Concept

Lab 11. Simulations. The Concept Lab 11 Simulations In this lab you ll learn how to create simulations to provide approximate answers to probability questions. We ll make use of a particular kind of structure, called a box model, that

More information

Stages of development

Stages of development 112 Chapter 8: Cognitive development Jean Piaget, 1896 1980. An example of the dominance in play of assimilation over accommodation pretending that an old packing case, a box, and a tube are a tank. The

More information

Crosstabulation & Chi Square

Crosstabulation & Chi Square Crosstabulation & Chi Square Robert S Michael Chi-square as an Index of Association After examining the distribution of each of the variables, the researcher s next task is to look for relationships among

More information

EFFECTIVE STRATEGIC PLANNING IN MODERN INFORMATION AGE ORGANIZATIONS

EFFECTIVE STRATEGIC PLANNING IN MODERN INFORMATION AGE ORGANIZATIONS EFFECTIVE STRATEGIC PLANNING IN MODERN INFORMATION AGE ORGANIZATIONS Cezar Vasilescu and Aura Codreanu Abstract: The field of strategic management has offered a variety of frameworks and concepts during

More information

Measurement with Ratios

Measurement with Ratios Grade 6 Mathematics, Quarter 2, Unit 2.1 Measurement with Ratios Overview Number of instructional days: 15 (1 day = 45 minutes) Content to be learned Use ratio reasoning to solve real-world and mathematical

More information

Math/Stats 425 Introduction to Probability. 1. Uncertainty and the axioms of probability

Math/Stats 425 Introduction to Probability. 1. Uncertainty and the axioms of probability Math/Stats 425 Introduction to Probability 1. Uncertainty and the axioms of probability Processes in the real world are random if outcomes cannot be predicted with certainty. Example: coin tossing, stock

More information

Operations and Supply Chain Management Prof. G. Srinivasan Department of Management Studies Indian Institute of Technology Madras

Operations and Supply Chain Management Prof. G. Srinivasan Department of Management Studies Indian Institute of Technology Madras Operations and Supply Chain Management Prof. G. Srinivasan Department of Management Studies Indian Institute of Technology Madras Lecture - 41 Value of Information In this lecture, we look at the Value

More information

Five High Order Thinking Skills

Five High Order Thinking Skills Five High Order Introduction The high technology like computers and calculators has profoundly changed the world of mathematics education. It is not only what aspects of mathematics are essential for learning,

More information

Modern Science vs. Ancient Philosophy. Daniel Gilbert s theory of happiness as presented in his book, Stumbling on Happiness,

Modern Science vs. Ancient Philosophy. Daniel Gilbert s theory of happiness as presented in his book, Stumbling on Happiness, Laura Katharine Norwood Freshman Seminar Dr. Golden 10/21/10 Modern Science vs. Ancient Philosophy Daniel Gilbert s theory of happiness as presented in his book, Stumbling on Happiness, has many similarities

More information

Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley

Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley Word Count: 2,486 Definition Prospect Theory is a psychological account that describes how people make decisions under conditions of uncertainty. These may

More information

Find an expected value involving two events. Find an expected value involving multiple events. Use expected value to make investment decisions.

Find an expected value involving two events. Find an expected value involving multiple events. Use expected value to make investment decisions. 374 Chapter 8 The Mathematics of Likelihood 8.3 Expected Value Find an expected value involving two events. Find an expected value involving multiple events. Use expected value to make investment decisions.

More information

7.S.8 Interpret data to provide the basis for predictions and to establish

7.S.8 Interpret data to provide the basis for predictions and to establish 7 th Grade Probability Unit 7.S.8 Interpret data to provide the basis for predictions and to establish experimental probabilities. 7.S.10 Predict the outcome of experiment 7.S.11 Design and conduct an

More information

An Introduction to Basic Statistics and Probability

An Introduction to Basic Statistics and Probability An Introduction to Basic Statistics and Probability Shenek Heyward NCSU An Introduction to Basic Statistics and Probability p. 1/4 Outline Basic probability concepts Conditional probability Discrete Random

More information

SYSTEMS OF EQUATIONS AND MATRICES WITH THE TI-89. by Joseph Collison

SYSTEMS OF EQUATIONS AND MATRICES WITH THE TI-89. by Joseph Collison SYSTEMS OF EQUATIONS AND MATRICES WITH THE TI-89 by Joseph Collison Copyright 2000 by Joseph Collison All rights reserved Reproduction or translation of any part of this work beyond that permitted by Sections

More information

Lies My Calculator and Computer Told Me

Lies My Calculator and Computer Told Me Lies My Calculator and Computer Told Me 2 LIES MY CALCULATOR AND COMPUTER TOLD ME Lies My Calculator and Computer Told Me See Section.4 for a discussion of graphing calculators and computers with graphing

More information

1. The Fly In The Ointment

1. The Fly In The Ointment Arithmetic Revisited Lesson 5: Decimal Fractions or Place Value Extended Part 5: Dividing Decimal Fractions, Part 2. The Fly In The Ointment The meaning of, say, ƒ 2 doesn't depend on whether we represent

More information

Session 7 Bivariate Data and Analysis

Session 7 Bivariate Data and Analysis Session 7 Bivariate Data and Analysis Key Terms for This Session Previously Introduced mean standard deviation New in This Session association bivariate analysis contingency table co-variation least squares

More information

Research Methods & Experimental Design

Research Methods & Experimental Design Research Methods & Experimental Design 16.422 Human Supervisory Control April 2004 Research Methods Qualitative vs. quantitative Understanding the relationship between objectives (research question) and

More information

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS Chapter Seven Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS Section : An introduction to multiple regression WHAT IS MULTIPLE REGRESSION? Multiple

More information