The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires"

Transcription

1 NEUROBIOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MEMORY 70, (1998) ARTICLE NO. NL The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires Ann M. Graybiel Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts The basal ganglia have been shown to contribute to habit and stimulus response (S R) learning. These forms of learning have the property of slow acquisition and, in humans, can occur without conscious awareness. This paper proposes that one aspect of basal ganglia-based learning is the recoding of cortically derived information within the striatum. Modular corticostriatal projection patterns, demonstrated experimentally, are viewed as producing recoded templates suitable for the gradual selection of new input output relations in cortico-basal ganglia loops. Recordings from striatal projection neurons and interneurons show that activity patterns in the striatum are modified gradually during the course of S R learning. It is proposed that this recoding within the striatum can chunk the representations of motor and cognitive action sequences so that they can be implemented as performance units. This scheme generalizes Miller s notion of information chunking to action control. The formation and the efficient implementation of action chunks are viewed as being based on predictive signals. It is suggested that information chunking provides a mechanism for the acquisition and the expression of action repertoires that, without such information compression would be biologically unwieldy or difficult to implement. The learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia are thus seen as core features of the basal ganglia s influence on motor and cognitive pattern generators Academic Press INTRODUCTION James Parkinson might have been quite surprised if told that the basal ganglia, disabled in the disease that bears his name, have a prime function in learning and memory. He might also have questioned the view that the basal ganglia function mainly in the automatic execution of motor acts or in implementing habits. His idea was that the basal ganglia translate the dictates of the will into action, or, in more current phrasing, that the basal ganglia translate intention into action. In this paper, it is argued that these disparate views of basal ganglia function are neither contradictory nor mutually exclusive. This paper suggests that the basal ganglia may bring together these functions in part by promoting the building up of performance units made up of multiple acts that can be implemented in a particular temporal order a sensorimotor form of chunking (Miller, 1956). I take as a starting point the convincing experimental evidence that the This work was supported by NIH R03 MH57878 and NIH Javits award R01 NS Address correspondence and requests for reprints to Dr. Ann M. Graybiel, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 45 Carleton Street, E25-618b, Cambridge, MA Fax: (617) amg@wccf.mit.edu /98 $25.00 Copyright 1998 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

2 120 ANN M. GRAYBIEL basal ganglia function in some forms of implicit or habit learning (Graybiel, 1995; Hirsh, 1974; Knowlton, Mangels, & Squire, 1996; Mishkin, Malamut, & Bachevalier, 1984; Salmon & Butters, 1995; White, 1997). In humans, this learning is characterized by two key features: lack of awareness of the algorithm learned, and a slow rate of acquisition. In experimental animals, the apparently equivalent form of learning, or stimulus response (S R) learning, is also slowly acquired as a result of repeated pairing between stimulus and response. Both in humans and in other animals, habit learning can be dissociated from forms of explicit learning, mediated by hippocampal medial temporal cortical systems, and from affect-related learning associated with limbic structures such as the amygdala (Kesner, Bolland, & Dakis, 1993; Knowlton et al., 1996; McDonald & White, 1993; Packard & McGaugh, 1996; Reading, Dunnett, & Robbins, 1991). It is becoming clear that tuning of neural responses is a very general phenomenon in the central nervous system, so that in many and perhaps even all cortical areas the response properties of neurons are modifiable, not fixed (Bichot, Schall, & Thompson, 1996; Chen & Wise, 1995, 1996; Iriki, Tanaka, & Iwamura, 1996; Mitz, Godschalk, & Wise, 1991; Wang, Merzenich, Sameshima, & Jenkins, 1995). The challenge to the neurobiologist in trying to understand the learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia is thus to determine how the particular behavioral characteristics of basal ganglia-based learning and memory are related to neural activity in the system, and to understand how the learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia relate to the core functions of the basal ganglia in the control of action. CORTICOSTRIATAL MAPPING: COHERENT ACTIVATION, COMPARTMENTS, AND CONVERGENCE REMAPPING The largest single input to the striatum is from the neocortex, and this input system is thought to be the main information source for basal ganglia circuits. Most of these inputs terminate on the spines of the main type of striatal neuron, the medium spiny neurons. As many as 10,000 cortical inputs may converge on a single projection neuron, and any single input fiber may contact many such cells (Wilson, 1995). Wilson and his coworkers have shown that the membrane potentials of these striatal projection neurons fluctuate between down-states, in which they are hyperpolarized to ca. 080 mv and in which they do not fire action potentials, and up-states, in which the neurons are partially depolarized (to ca. 050 mv) and in which afferent inputs can generate action potentials (Wilson, 1993). The cortical inputs are glutamatergic, and the projection neurons have both non-nmda and NMDA receptors (Kita, 1996). When the projection neurons are in down-state, NMDA receptors are blocked, and glutamate acts through AMPA/kainate receptors. Under conditions of strong and temporally coherent activation, driving the neurons into up-state, NMDA receptors begin to become active with further inputs, promoting longer postsynaptic response times to favor input summation and also favoring NMDA-receptor mediated forms of plasticity (Kita, 1996). How could this coherence-dependent mechanism affect the mapping of cortical inputs to the striatum? One answer may lie in the distributed yet clumpy anatomy of many corticostriatal projection systems. Inputs from a small site in the neocortex tend to be widely distributed in the striatum, but they also tend to form a labyrinthine series of partially interconnected clusters. The

3 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 121 FIG. 1. Modular remapping in corticostriatal systems. Example shows two cortical areas (Area 1, Area 2) with distributed, clumpy projections to matrisomes in the striatum (A, B, C). It is proposed that such differential patterns of modular divergence and convergence set up templates in the striatum that can be used to recode cortical information. multiple input clusters from functionally related areas can overlap selectively and quite precisely. For example, inputs from the cortex of the hand representations of primary somatosensory (SI) areas 3b and 1, and of the primary motor (MI) hand cortex represention overlap with one another, whereas they do not overlap with input from cortex of the contralateral MI hand representations (Flaherty & Graybiel, 1993). We have suggested that systems of input modules (called matrisomes) could serve as templates for forming new associations in the striatum and for selecting which inputs lead to output activation (Flaherty & Graybiel, 1994; Malach & Graybiel, 1986; Parthasarathy & Graybiel, 1992). The fact that the dendritic trees of many projection neurons in the striatum are also oriented and in some cases curved at boundaries of modules provides anatomical support for this idea (Walker, Arbuthnott, Baughman, & Graybiel, 1993; Walker & Graybiel, 1993). This input organization not only may implement ongoing corticostriatal transmission promoting selection of output lines, but also could critically shape corticostriatal remapping, given the up-state/down-state fluctuations of striatal projection neurons. In the example shown in Fig. 1, inputs from cortical areas 1 and 2 overlap in striatal matrisome B but not in matrisomes A and C. This situation, given equal input efficacies, should promote B neurons achieving up-state more readily than A and C neurons. This would lead to more firing in B neurons, but also would lead to more activation of NMDA receptors in B neurons, which in turn could lead to long-term potentiation or depression (LTP or LTD), both demonstrated for corticostriatal synapses (Calabresi, Pisani, Mercuri, & Bernardi, 1996; Calabresi, Salardi, Pisani, Baik, Centonze, Mercuri, Bernardi, & Borrelli, 1997; Charpier & Deniau, 1997). On this basis, we suggest that coherent up-state transitions in modular groups of striatal projection neurons can promote long-term changes as well as transient activation evidenced by bursts of action potentials. A function of convergence mapping onto matrisomes thus would be to allow gradual changes in the input output relationships of particular populations of striatal neurons. The incremental nature of the corticostriatal reordering, dependent on repetition of particular coherent input patterns, could underlie in part the gradual learning mediated by the basal ganglia. Not all cortical inputs are equally patchy (see, e.g., Eblen & Graybiel, 1995; Parthasarathy & Graybiel, 1992), and modularity is not necessary for convergence. Yet nearly all cortical areas show some modu-

4 122 ANN M. GRAYBIEL larity. This could provide a fairly general mechanism for corticostriatal remapping during learning (and a critical one during postnatal development, given that dendrites and their synaptic inputs mature postnatally). CORTICOSTRIATAL REMAPPING AND S R LEARNING Why should this form of remapping promote an S R learning function for the striatum (basal ganglia) rather than an S S or context learning function (thought to be the specialty of the hippocampal/medial temporal system) (Eichenbaum, 1995)? At the systems level, one answer is that the outputs of the striatum (via its pallidal and nigral targets) are oriented mainly toward motor and cognitive action systems of the frontal cortex and toward brainstem premotor regions such as the superior colliculus. By contrast, much of the outflow from the hippocampal formation and medial temporal cortex leads to other association cortex and to the septum and anterior thalamic/mammillary body circuits (Suzuki, 1996). The output pattern of the striatum suggests that sufficient coherent activation of striatal neurons leads to the generation of action potentials off up-states, and that these can set off changes in activity leading through the system of connections and loop circuits of the basal ganglia. In the example of cortical areas 1 and 2 becoming active, B neurons would again be favored over A and C neurons by coherent activation of areas 1 and 2, and repetitions could lead to an increased or decreased probability of B responses, respectively, through LTP or LTD (Fig. 2). Based on fast GABA transmission from the striatum to the pallidum and the substantia nigra, LTP would lead to decreased outflow on activation of B neurons and LTD to increased outflow. But other long-term effects could also occur, in part through release of NMDA receptor-based effects in up-state. Coherent cortical activation imposed experimentally can lead to activation of immediate-early gene expression in striatal neurons (see Berretta, Parthasarathy, & Graybiel, 1997a; Liste, Rozas, Guerra, & Labandeira-Garcia, 1995; Sgambato, Abo, Rogard, Besson, & Deniau, 1997) and this activation is NMDA receptor dependent (Berretta et al., 1997a; Liste et al., 1995) and can be selective for particular matrisomes (Parthasarathy & Graybiel, 1997). Interestingly, among the documented genes known to be regulated in striatal projection neurons by cortical inputs are those coding for modulatory neuropeptides that are contained along with GABA in these neurons. There is an estimated convergence of between about 30:1 (rat) to 80:1 (monkey) in striatal projections onto its target neurons (Percheron, 1989; Oorschot, 1996). This means that, again, coherent network-level activity must be crucial in determining whether the pallidal and nigral targets of striatal projection neurons fire. We have found one anatomical pattern that may serve this purpose (Flaherty & Graybiel, 1994). At least some of the input matrisomes of the primate putamen coincide with output matrisomes identified as clusters of projection neurons whose outputs converge toward small sites in the pallidum. This sets up a divergence reconvergence architecture that bears a notable resemblance to the adaptive mixture of experts architecture of some learning theories (Jacobs, Jordan, Nowlan, & Hinton, 1991; Graybiel, Aosaki, Flaherty, & Kimura, 1994; see Fig. 3). In effect, the striatum then becomes like a hidden layer in which cortico-basal ganglia throughput can be tuned on moment-to-moment and long-term bases.

5 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 123 FIG. 2. Modifiable corticostriatal templates. The example shows two cortical areas (1 and 2, as in Fig. 1) with their similar activity levels shown by schematic X Y plots. The areas project to striatal matrisomes A, B, and C and their projections overlap in matrisome B. It is proposed that such patterns of overlap and nonoverlap can have consequences for on-line corticostriatal throughput to striatal output targets (e.g., producing greater probability of up-state transitions and action potential firing of projection neurons in B), and also longer-term consequences, symbolized by changes in phosphorylation (P) states of intracellular messengers and receptors, and changes in gene expression ( ). MODIFIABLE RESPONSES OF STRIATAL INTERNEURONS, GRADUAL S R LEARNING, AND BINDING Context dependence is a major response characteristic of striatal neurons, and the responses of many cells recorded in alert monkeys and other species are so specific (for example, to a particular sequence of movements or to a remembered stimulus) that the response must have been acquired through learning (Aosaki, Tsubokawa, Ishida, Watanabe, Graybiel, & Kimura, 1994b; Hikosaka, Sakamoto, & Usui, 1989a; Kermadi & Joseph, 1995; Romo, Scarnati, & Schultz, 1992; Schneider & Lidsky, 1981; Thorpe, Rolls, & Maddison, 1983). To try to understand this modifiability, we are taking the approach of recording from the striatum during learning. We first studied, with Aosaki, Kimura, and colleagues, the tonically active neurons (TANs) of the primate striatum, because they were known to exhibit conditioned sensory responses

6 124 ANN M. GRAYBIEL FIG. 3. Remapping in cortico-basal ganglia circuits as a mechanism for learning. (A) Example of divergent mapping from neocortex (1) to striatal matrisomes (A, B, C) with reconvergence at the level of the globus pallidus (GP). Adapted from Flaherty and Graybiel (1994) and Graybiel et al. (1994). The network is shown to be modulated by dopamine (DA)-containing inputs from the substantia nigra (SN). Compare with the learning network in (B). (B) Diagram modified from Jacobs et al. (1991) showing adaptive mixture of networks model. Note similarity of architecture to pattern of connectivity in (A). in monkeys highly trained on a sensorimotor conditioning task (Aosaki et al., 1994b). The TANs are thought to be the cholinergic interneurons of the striatum (Aosaki, Kimura, & Graybiel, 1995). Recording during acquisition, we found that the TANs acquired these conditioned responses very gradually during the course of training. The gradual recruitment of TANs as the behavioral response was acquired fits well with the slow acquisition of implicit (S R) learning. We reason that the TANs could serve an important function in the gradual stamping in of new S R patterns in the striatal projection neurons influenced by TANs; they do not lead the behavioral response itself (Aosaki et al., 1994b, 1995; Graybiel et al., 1994). A particular characteristic of the TAN responses suggests that they could have an important influence on the timesensitive aspects of this learning as well. The occurrence of their acquired responses (pauses in tonic firing) coincides within a similar ca. 270-ms time frame, which starts about 70 ms after the conditioned stimulus across a wide sector of the striatum (Aosaki et al., 1995). This characteristic suggests that

7 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 125 FIG. 3 Continued these sparsely but widely distributed striatal interneurons could play a role in motor binding (Graybiel et al., 1994). For anterior parts of the striatum preferentially influencing prefrontal cortex, the TANs could serve a comparable binding function in the striatum (Graybiel, 1997). The acquired TAN responses could, as argued below, also produce a time-stamp signal useful in neuronal coding of serial-order tasks. CHANGES IN ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF STRIATAL PROJECTION NEURONS DURING LEARNING In ongoing experiments to be reported elsewhere (Jog, Connolly, Hillegaart, Wilson, & Graybiel, 1996; Jog, Kubota, Connolly, Iyengar, & Graybiel, 1997; Jog, et al., in preparation), we are recording with multiple tetrodes from populations of neurons in the striatum of rats undergoing behavioral training in a T-maze task. With this method, we record activity from projection neurons and can record from one small region for days without having to move the electrodes. This gives us samples of activity in populations of neurons across days of training on this task, chosen as an example of a striatum-based win-

8 126 ANN M. GRAYBIEL stay task (McDonald & White, 1993). According to the behavioral characteristics of striatum-based learning, we would predict that the striatal projection neurons would only gradually change their firing patterns during behavioral training. And given the circuit-level considerations posed above, we would predict finding groups of nearby neurons with similar properties that would exhibit increased coherent activity during learning. These properties appear to hold. A particularly interesting finding is that at some recording sites the responses shift temporally, toward earlier phases of the behavioral trial (Jog et al., in preparation). This key feature may account for the repeated observation that in highly trained monkeys, overtrained on a variety of behavioral tasks, a large fraction of striatal neurons fire in an anticipatory mode early in the task (Alexander, 1987; Alexander & Crutcher, 1990; Hikosaka, Sakamoto, & Usui, 1989b; Jaeger, Gilman, & Aldridge, 1993). We hypothesize elsewhere that this predictive firing can be built up on the basis of repeated task exposure with the kinetics of implicit learning (Jog et al., in preparation). MODIFIABLE CORTICOSTRIATAL TEMPLATES AND S R LEARNING These new ensemble recording data demonstrate that gradual changes occur in the firing of striatal projection neurons as animals acquire behavioral learning. They do not show whether these changes are initiated in the striatum or elsewhere (for example, in the neocortex). There is ample reason to think, however, that some of the changes occurring during learning involve altering the parameters of corticostriatal transmission. The most extensively studied modifier of corticostriatal transmission is the nigrostriatal system (see, e.g., Calabresi et al., 1996; Kötter, 1994, for review). Dopamine receptor activation (or inactivation) affects both on-line activity of striatal neurons and plasticity in these neurons. For example, the production of LTP and LTD by tetanic stimulation in vitro critically depends on dopamine receptor activation (Calabresi et al., 1996, 1997; Wickens, Begg, & Arbuthnott, 1996). Dopamine receptor function also affects the capacity of cortical stimulation to induce gene expression in striatal neurons via an NMDA-dependent mechanism (Berretta, Sachs, & Graybiel, 1996; Berretta, Sachs, & Graybiel, in preparation; Liste et al., 1995). These results suggest that dopamine can affect the efficacy of corticostriatal transmission with long-term consequences that could affect striatum-based learning and memory. This conclusion is strongly supported by behavioral and physiological evidence. The early experiments of Routtenberg and his colleagues demonstrated that dopaminergic inputs to the rat s striatum are necessary for memory consolidation in a passive avoidance test (Routtenberg & Kim, 1978). This role for dopamine has been confirmed (White, 1997). In the monkey, we found that dopaminergic inputs are required for the expression of acquired responses of TANs to conditioned stimuli (Aosaki, Graybiel, & Kimura, 1994a). It is now known that dopamine-containing neurons in the midbrain nigral/ventral tegmental area complex can fire preferentially in response to novel salient stimuli and primary rewards and, in conditioned monkeys, to conditioned stimuli but no longer to the rewards themselves (Schultz, 1997). These experiments suggest that the dopamine-containing neurons of the midbrain serve a reinforcement function during learning and a predictive function after learning and that they affect the expression of learned responses.

9 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 127 Other striatal afferent systems are also likely to produce major effects on striatal neurons during learning. These include inputs from the thalamus and the amygdala, recursive inputs from the pallidum, and the large serotonergic inputs to the striatum and to the substantia nigra pars compacta (see Parent & Hazrati, 1995). Few direct studies of such potential directional effects have yet been made. But our collaborative work with Kimura and associates shows that inputs to the striatum from the intralaminar nuclei are necessary for expression of acquired TAN responses (Matsumoto, Hanakawa, Maki, Graybiel, & Kimura, 1997), and Packard and colleagues have shown that inputs from the amygdala carried by the stria terminalis are necessary for striatal cholinergic enhancement of learning in a conditioned avoidance paradigm (Packard, Introini-Collison, & McGaugh, 1996). These experiments suggest that remapping of corticostriatal throughput is multiply determined. Finally, striatal interneurons other than the TANs are likely to have important effects in shaping corticostriatal transmission (Kita, 1996; Parthasarathy & Graybiel, 1997). LEARNING HABITS AND EXPRESSING HABITS A paradox in the learning literature is that the basal ganglia are at once thought to be important for the production of habitual or automatic responses and yet are thought to be important for new S R learning. Does the same mechanism subserve both learning and expression of habits? The modifiable template mechanism proposed above could do so; but another possibility is that different parts of the system are specialized for acquisition or expression phases, in part depending on the nature of their cortical inputs. This is the suggestion made by Jueptner et al. (Jueptner, Stephan, Frith, Brooks, Frackowiak, & Passingham, 1997a; Jueptner, Frith, Brooks, Frackowiak, & Passingham, 1997b). They have studied cerebral metabolic activity by PET imaging as subjects learn new sequences or execute learned sequences in a buttonpress task. They find that during new learning, the anterior striatum (caudate nucleus) is differentially active, along with the anterior prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus. During the performance of already learned sequences, neither the anterior striatum nor the anterior cortical areas were differentially active; activity had shifted posteriorly, to the putamen and to the premotor and motor cortex. There is by no means unanimity about such anterior posterior differences (see Brown, 1997, for review). However, when subjects in the Jueptner et al. study were instructed to pay attention to the task during the learned sequence production, the anterior regions again became more active. This result suggests a special function of the anterior system in attention to action (Jueptner et al., 1997a, 1997b) which may be important for some forms of procedural learning but not for others (Brown, 1997). One notable feature of the anterior striatum, other than its massive inputs from prefrontal cortex, is that the neurochemically specialized striosomes are especially prominent there (Graybiel & Ragsdale, 1978). Striosomes are thought to provide a major source of input to the dopamine-containing neurons of the substantia nigra, which, as noted above, respond to salient (attentionattracting) stimuli and, in conditioned animals, respond to conditioned stimuli predictive of future reward. The major cortical inputs to the anterior striosomal system so far identified come from the anterior cingulate/medial prefrontal and caudal orbitofrontal cortex, regions interconnected with the amygdala and

10 128 ANN M. GRAYBIEL hippocampus (Eblen & Graybiel, 1995). Physiological recordings suggest that neurons in these cortical areas fire in relation to reward or signal lack of predicted rewards as though providing an error signal (Thorpe et al., 1983). The anterior striosomal system may thus play a special role in influencing striatal processing in relation to knowledge of results. More generally, the striatal substrates for procedural learning and for execution of procedures may not be identical everywhere in the striatum, but specialized according to behavioral modality. The relative functions of the striatum in the acquisition and expression should thus not be considered to be exclusive of one another. CORTICOSTRIATAL REMAPPING AND SEQUENCE LEARNING Tanji and coworkers have demonstrated exquisitely selective coding for temporally ordered sequences of movements by neurons in the supplementary motor area (SMA) and pre-supplementary motor area (pre-sma) of monkeys trained to perform three discrete movements in varying orders (Shima, Mushiake, Saito, & Tanji, 1996; Tanji & Shima, 1994). Populations of cortical neurons in these cortical areas respond selectively in relation to temporally correct sequences of two or all three of the movements, performed by memory. These firing patterns stand in sharp contrast to the activity patterns of most neurons recorded in primary motor cortex in the same task, which respond before single movements regardless of the sequence in which they are performed. In the pre-sma, there is an added population of neurons that fire selectively when a new sequence is to be performed and the previously performed sequence must be abandoned. The pre-sma has been singled out in an fmri study as differentially active during the learning (but not during the later performance) of movement sequences (Hikosaka, Sakai, Miyauchi, Takino, Sasaki, & Putz, 1996). Sequence-specific responses have also been found in arcuate, premotor, and prefrontal cortex (e.g., Barone & Joseph, 1989). These findings suggest that in the prefrontal, supplementary motor, and premotor cortex, there is a step-by-step building-up of a code for movement sequences the neural representation of elements of a motor plan. There is also strong representation of sequential patterns of movement in the basal ganglia. A large number of observations in the human, ranging from PET and fmri experiments to studies of patient populations, have implicated the basal ganglia as being active in the learning or execution of sequential behavior (Benecke, Rothwell, Dick, Day, & Marsden, 1987; Brown, 1997; Gerloff, Corwell, Chen, Hallett, & Cohen, 1997; Harrington & Haaland, 1991; Rauch, Savage, Brown, Curran, Alpert, Kendrick, Fischman, & Kosslyn, 1995; Rauch, Whalen, Savage, Curran, Kendrick, Brown, Bush, Breitner, & Rosen, 1997; Seitz, Roland, Bohm, Greitz, & Stone-Elander, 1990; Seitz & Roland, 1992; Vriezen & Moscovitch, 1990). In the most extensive demonstration of this in trained monkeys, Kermadi and Joseph (1995) have shown that many neurons in the caudate nucleus of highly trained monkeys code for learned sequences of eye and eye hand movements. Both the responses of these neurons to the sequentially presented sensory cues in the task and their movement-related responses were sequence-dependent. Moreover, many striatal units fired in anticipation of such cued sequences. Kermadi and Joseph suggest that the caudate nucleus codes for the temporospatial movement plan, helps to construct the plan based on representing the sequence of targets to be

11 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 129 acquired, and helps to update the plan and to select the next movement, consonant with evidence reviewed by Marsden (1987) indicating that the basal ganglia anticipate the next movement. Mushiake and Strick (1995) have reported selective coding for sequences in the globus pallidus of monkeys trained on a button-press task, showing that sequence coding is present in basal ganglia output neurons. Finally, Berridge and coworkers suggest that there is a specialized representation of instinctive (i.e., unlearned) sequential grooming movements in part of the rodent striatum (Cromwell & Berridge, 1996). Because each of the cortical areas implicated in coding movement sequences projects to the striatum, and the striatum, via the pallidonigral complex and thalamus, projects to and influences these cortical regions, it is not clear whether such representations of sequences are formed in the cortex or in the basal ganglia or in both. However, an ongoing collaborative study with the Kimura laboratory (Matsumoto et al., submitted) suggests that activity in the striatum may be critical to building up these representations. Two monkeys were trained on a three-button-press task, and a unilateral lesion of the nigrostriatal system was made prior to the training in one animal and after the training in the other. Behavioral measures of serial reaction times and EMG recordings from different sets of muscles engaged by the task showed the gradual acquisition of smooth, efficient performance only if the dopaminergic innervation of the striatum was present on the side contralateral to the arm being tested. The effects of dopamine depletion were strikingly evident when, after the monkeys were thoroughly trained on the button-press task (in which reward came after the last press), they were given reward early, one button earlier than expected. Both monkeys showed perseverative errors with the arm contralateral to the intact nigrostriatal system, continuing, as though by habit, to press the last button. There were also perseverative button presses with the arm contralateral to the posttraining nigrostriatal lesion. But in the monkey with the pretraining lesion of the dopaminergic input to the striatum, perseverative pushes were absent with the contralateral arm. It was as though, when trained without an intact nigrostriatal system, the monkey failed to bind the sequence of pushes together into a macro or performance unit. The dependence of this behavior on the presence of dopaminergic innervation of the striatum suggests that the striatum itself (as modulated by dopamine) was necessary for the acquisition of the performance unit. INFORMATION RECORDING IN CORTICO-BASAL GANGLIA LOOPS It is clear that learned sequences of movements can be coded both in the neocortex and in the basal ganglia, and it would be surprising not to find such information about learned sequences broadly represented in the motor system. What is proposed here is that the basal ganglia may be critically involved in constructing performance units of such sequence representations. I adopt the term chunk from Miller (1956), who used it to convey the idea that efficiency in information content can be gained by recoding bits of information to form packages (chunks of information) that, once learned, can be treated as entities in memory. For the sensorimotor system, this process of recoding could yield the automated behaviors attributed to the basal ganglia, and could help to account for some of the difficulties in performing sequential actions suffered by patients with basal ganglia disorders or by animals with basal ganglia lesions.

12 130 ANN M. GRAYBIEL What could do the recoding? The modular corticostriatal remapping discussed above might serve just such a purpose. By reordering cortically derived information within the striatum, notably within striatal matrisomes (and striosomes), a more efficient action-oriented representation could be produced. For example, the new representation might reduce the degrees of freedom in the representation by reducing the number of distinctively represented units. Our data both on TAN activity during S R learning in monkeys and on projection neuron activity during T-maze learning in rats suggest that the changes in neural responsiveness that occur with learning are slow, but that they incorporate two key features: a latch-on mechanism for temporal synchronization (exhibited by TANs) and a phase-advance tendency for predictive coding (exhibited by some projection neurons). The gradual tuning of cortically innervated modular populations of neurons (expert networks) in the striatum, the spatiotemporal binding by striatal interneurons, and the reconvergence of the information onto striatal output targets are candidate mechanisms for bringing about chunking. THE BASAL GANGLIA AND INFORMATION RECORDING The functions the basal ganglia perform in influencing motor pattern generators of the brainstem and spinal cord may have equivalents in the cognitive realm by virtue of influencing cognitive pattern generators in the forebrain (Graybiel, 1997). It seems reasonable, given the circuitry of the basal ganglia and their apparent involvement in some neuropsychiatric disorders, that any chunking functions of the basal ganglia may also extend beyond motor chunks to a range of action repertoires. In Miller s original proposal, the value of chunking was mnemonic: it lets us go beyond the 7 { 2 limits of short-term memory (Miller, 1956). One of his points was that the recoding from bits to chunks of information needs to be automatic, or nearly so, to be useful. Another was that linguistic encoding is a critical example of chunking. What is proposed here is that a similar recoding to achieve information compression can occur in the formation of action repetories, whether they be motoric or cognitive. One of the most compelling and well-documented examples of chunking of action repetoires comes from studies of patients with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). The caudate nucleus, together with the anterior cingulate cortex and orbital frontal cortex, shows consistent patterns of metabolic activity in symptomatic OCD patients, and abnormalities in metabolic activity in these cortical regions abate with treatment that decreases symptomatology (Baxter, Schwartz, Bergman, Szuba, Guze, Mazziotta, Alazraki, Selin, Ferng, & Phelps, 1992; Rauch, Jenike, Alpert, Baer, Breiter, Savage, & Fischman, 1994; Schwartz, Stoessel, Baxter, Martin, & Phelps, 1996; Swedo, Pietrini, Leonard, Schapiro, Rettew, Goldberger, Rapoport, Rapoport, & Grady, 1992). The symptoms themselves include repetitive, intrusive thoughts and compulsions that lead to ritualistic behaviors such as washing, counting, and checking. These behaviors involve sequential acts. But they are performed as chunks, unitized and driven by the extraordinary imperative of urges and compulsions that the patient recognizes as abnormal and outside the controlled range of the person s intent. These compulsions are now viewed in the context of a lack of normal error feedback (that the action has been done, that the hands have been washed). Disruption of the anterior

13 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 131 learning-performance circuits involving the caudate nucleus, anterior cingulate cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex, which may carry such signals, could thus be critically involved in the genesis of the symptoms. A BROADER VIEW There is a much more extensive circuitry of the basal ganglia than discussed here, and other potential functions, such as response selection, that have not been touched on. No integrated treatment of the learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia could be complete without considering these other mechanisms. In the example of response selection, multiple levels within the basal ganglia may be important, and the interplay between direct and indirect pathways may be critical (Berns & Sejnowski, 1996; Mink, 1996). Similar arguments hold for other proposed basal ganglia functions, ranging from response stabilitzation and scaling (Inase, Buford, & Anderson, 1996; Turner and Anderson, 1997) to the decision of whether and when to move (Graybiel and Kimura, 1995). I have, however, emphasized cortico-basal ganglia circuits, the templates they form in the striatum, and the modifiability of striatal coding during learning to make a particular point: that one function of the striatum may be to chunk performance sequences. It may be these chunked representations that are selected and scaled by the output circuits of the basal ganglia. How does this notion relate to the two key characteristics of habit or S R learning emphasized at the outset of this paper, that it occurs slowly and without conscious awareness? The slow kinetics of S R learning offer a great advantage for chunking in view of the fact that we do not want to collapse all encoded sequences (temporally ordered acts) into chunks. We need to select which sequences to recode in this form, because the behavioral macro or chunk, once formed, may be difficult to break apart. Neural coding of sequences, by contrast, appears to occur quickly and to be highly labile. For example, Tanji and coworkers can train monkeys on a series of three-movement sequences in a training session, and find corresponding switches in the encoding of the sequential movements in the SMA and pre-sma cortex (Shima et al., 1996). Part of what goes on in S R learning may be a selection of which sequential representations to chunk, based on reinforcement-related modulation. The lack of conscious awareness in S R learning may also be an advantageous property for a chunking mechanism in that the action chunks are treated as units (not, for example, as response chains). We do not want supervisory attention (Shallice, 1988) or attention to action (Jueptner et al., 1997a, 1997b) or conscious manipulation to intervene inside the macro unit itself. Chunks take their advantage from being manipulable as entities, and the intervention of consciousness or attention might actually disrupt their smooth implementation. Prediction should be a crucial guide to the implementation of chunks and to their formation. The central place of prediction in corticobasal ganglia loop functions was experimentally demonstrated by Flowers (1978) in a series of experiments on patients with Parkinson s disease, and it was Flowers who suggested that a fundamental sensorimotor defect in Parkinson s patients is a lack of predictive capacity. He suggested that this defect forced the patients toward modes of movement performance in which each movement is guided by sensory input rather than by an internal model allowing prediction and forward planning. The function of action chunking proposed here for the basal ganglia fits

14 132 ANN M. GRAYBIEL well within this framework. Prediction would allow the initiation of chunks and their formation. The recession in time that we are observing in the responses of some striatal projection neurons during learning is intriguing when viewed in this context. The dictates of the will alluded to by Parkinson could in part be implemented by prediction under the control of intent leading with repetition to action chunking and eventually to the implementation of action chunks no longer under conscious supervision. We suggest, in other words, that there probably are different kinds of linkage between intent and action. Intent may elicit or suppress single acts or series of acts. And, as shown by the example of OCD, some acts and sequences are not under the control of intent even though consciously performed. They can even coexist with different states of conscious intent, at least in disorders affecting cortico-basal ganglia loops. It is for all of these reasons that we emphasize the compatibility of otherwise seemingly contradictory views of the basal ganglia as being involved in generating either automatic acts or willed (intended) acts, or as being involved in either motor or mnemonic processes. One attractive feature of the chunking function proposed here is that these different aspects of basal ganglia operations seem natural allies. REFERENCES Alexander, G. E. (1987). Selective neuronal discharge in monkey putamen reflects intended direction of planned limb movements. Experimental Brain Research, 67, Alexander, G. E., & Crutcher, M. D. (1990). Preparation for movement: Neural representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64, Aosaki, T., Graybiel, A. M., & Kimura, M. (1994a). Effects of nigrostriatal dopamine system on acquired neural responses in the striatum of behaving monkey. Science, 265, Aosaki, T., Kimura, M., & Graybiel, A. M. (1995). Temporal and spatial characteristics of tonically active neurons of the primate s striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, Aosaki, T., Tsubokawa, H., Ishida, A., Watanabe, K., Graybiel, A. M., & Kimura, M. (1994b). Responses of tonically active neurons in the primate s striatum undergo systematic changes during behavioral sensorimotor conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, Barone, P., & Joseph, J.-P. (1989). Prefrontal cortex and spatial sequencing in macaque monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 78, Baxter, L. R., Jr., Schwartz, J. M., Bergman, K. S., Szuba, M. P., Guze, B. H., Mazziotta, J. C., Alazraki, A., Selin, C. E., Ferng, H.-K., & Phelps, M. E. (1992). Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavioral therapy of obsessive compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, Benecke, R., Rothwell, J. C., Dick, J. P., Day, B. L., & Marsden, C. D. (1987). Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson s disease. Brain, 110, Berns, G. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1996). How the basal ganglia make desisions. In A. R. Damasio, H. Damasio, & Y. Christen (Eds.), Neurobiology of decision making (pp ). Springer- Verlag, Berlin. Berretta, S., Parthasarathy, H. B., & Graybiel, A. M. (1997a). Local release of GABAergic inhibition in the motor cortex induces immediate-early gene expression in indirect pathway neurons of the striatum. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, Berretta, S., Sachs, Z., & Graybiel, A. M. (1996). Blockade of dopamine receptors amplifies cortical activation on induction of immediate-early genes in the striatum. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, Berretta, S., Sachs, Z., & Graybiel, A. M. (1997b). Blockade of striatal, but not cortical, D2-class

15 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 133 dopamine receptors increases cortically-driven fos induction in the striatum. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 193. Bichot, N. P., Schall, J. D., & Thompson, K. G. (1996). Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques. Nature, 381, Brown, R. (1997). The roles of cortico-striatal circuits in learning sequential information. In G. Stern (Ed.), Parkinson s disease: Advances in neurology. Philadelphia: Lippincott Raven. Brown, V. J., Desimone, R., & Mishkin, M. (1995). Responses of cells in the tail of the caudate nucleus during visual discrimination learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, Calabresi, P., Pisani, A., Mercuri, N. B., & Bernardi, G. (1996). The corticostriatal projection: From synaptic plasticity to dysfunctions of the basal ganglia. Trends in Neurosciences, 19, Calabresi, P., Salardi, A., Pisani, A., Baik, J.-H., Centonze, D., Mercuri, N. B., Bernardi, G., & Borrelli, E. (1997). Abnormal synaptic plasticity in the striatum of mice lacking dopamine D2 receptors. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, Charpier, S., & Deniau, J. M. (1997). In vivo activity-dependent plasticity at cortico-striatal connections: Evidence for physiological long-term potentiation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, Chen, L. L., & Wise, S. P. (1995). Neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, Chen, L. L., & Wise, S. P. (1996). Evolution of directional preferences in the supplementary eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, Cromwell, H. C., & Berridge, K. C. (1996). Implementation of action sequences by a neostriatal site: A lesion mapping study of grooming syntax. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, Eblen, F., & Graybiel, A. M. (1995). Highly restricted origin of prefrontal cortical inputs to striosomes in the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, Eichenbaum, H. (1995). Spatial learning: The LTP memory connection. Nature, 378, Flaherty, A. W., & Graybiel, A. M. (1993). Two input systems for body representation in the primate striatal matrix: Experimental evidence in the squirrel monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, Flaherty, A. W., & Graybiel, A. M. (1994). Input output organization of the sensorimotor striatum in the squirrel monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, Flowers, K. (1978). Lack of prediction in the motor behaviour of parkinsonism. Brain, 101, Gerloff, C., Corwell, B., Chen, R., Hallett, M., & Cohen, L. G. (1997). Stimulation over the human supplementary motor area interferes with the organization of future elements in complex motor sequences. Brain, 120, Graybiel, A. M. (1995). Building action repertoires: Memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, Graybiel, A. M. (1997). The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, Graybiel, A. M., Aosaki, T., Flaherty, A. W., & Kimura, M. (1994). The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. Science, 265, Graybiel, A. M., & Kimura, M. (1995). Adaptive neural networks in the basal ganglia. In J. C. Houk, J. L. Davis, & D. G. Beiser (Eds.), Models of information processing in the basal ganglia (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Graybiel, A. M., & Ragsdale, C. W., Jr. (1978). Histochemically distinct compartments in the striatum of human, monkey, and cat demonstrated by acetylthiocholinesterase staining. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 75, Harrington, D. L., & Haaland, K. Y. (1991). Sequencing in Parkinson s disease. Brain, 114, Hikosaka, O., Sakai, K., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Sasaki, Y., & Putz, B. (1996). Activation of human presupplementary motor area in learning of sequential procedures: A functional MRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76,

16 134 ANN M. GRAYBIEL Hikosaka, O., Sakamoto, M., & Usui, S. (1989a). Functional properties of monkey caudate neurons. II. Visual and auditory responses. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, Hikosaka, O., Sakamoto, M., & Usui, S. (1989b). Functional properties of monkey caudate neurons. III. Activities related to expectation of target and reward. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, Hirsh, R. (1974). The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: A theory. Behavioral Biology, 12, Inase, M., Buford, J. A., & Anderson, M. E. (1996). Changes in the control of arm position, movement, and thalamic discharge during local inactivation in the globus pallidus of the monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 75, Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, Jacobs, R. A., Jordan, M. I., Nowlan, S. J., & Hinton, G. E. (1991). Adaptive mixtures of local experts. Neural Computation, 3, Jaeger, D., Gilman, S., & Aldridge, J. W. (1993). Primate basal ganglia activity in a precued reaching task: Preparation for movement. Experimental Brain Research, 95, Jog, M., Connolly, C., Hillegaart, V., Wilson, M., & Graybiel, A. M. (1996). Ensemble recordings from striatum of freely-behaving rats. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, Jog, M., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C., Iyengar, D., & Graybiel, A. M. (1997). Behavior-related ensemble activity of neurons in rat striatum. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 464. Jueptner, M., Stephan, K. M., Frith, C. D., Brooks, D. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Passingham, R. E. (1997a). Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, Jueptner, M., Frith, C. D., Brooks, D. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Passingham, R. E. (1997b). Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical structures and learning by trial and error. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, Kermadi, I., & Joseph, J. P. (1995). Activity in the caudate nucleus of monkey during spatial sequencing. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, Kesner, R. P., Bolland, B. L., & Dakis, M. (1993). Memory for spatial locations, motor responses, and objects: Triple dissociation among the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and extrastriate visual cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 93, Kita, H. (1996). Glutamatergic and GABAergic postsynaptic responses of striatal spiny neurons to intrastriatal and cortical stimulation recorded in slice preparations. Neuroscience, 70, Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273, Kötter, R. (1994). Postsynaptic integration of glutamatergic and dopaminergic signals in the striatum. Progress in Neurobiology, 44, Liste, I., Rozas, G., Guerra, M. J., & Labandeira-Garcia, J. L. (1995). Cortical stimulation induces Fos expression in striatal neurons via NMDA glutamate and dopamine receptors. Brain Research, 700, Malach, R., & Graybiel, A. M. (1986). Mosaic architecture of the somatic sensory recipient sector of the cat s striatum. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, Marsden, C. D. (1987). What do the basal ganglia tell premotor cortical areas? In Motor areas of the cerebral cortex, Ciba Foundation Symposium 132 (pp ). Chichester: Wiley. Matsumoto, N., Hanakawa, T., Maki, S., Graybiel, A. M., & Kimura, M. (1997). The effects of unilateral nigrostriatal dopamine depletion on the learning and memory of sequence motor tasks in monkeys, submitted for publication. Matsumoto, N., Minamimoto, T., Graybiel, A. M., & Kimura, M. (1997). Expression of behaviorally conditioned responses of tonically active striatal neurons depends on thalamic input from the CM Pf complex. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 464. McDonald, R. J., & White, N. M. (1993). A triple dissociation of memory systems: Hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, Miller, G. A. (1956). The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Reviews, 63,

17 ACTION AND INFORMATION CHUNKING IN THE BASAL GANGLIA 135 Mink, J. W. (1996). The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Progress in Neurobiology, 50, Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., & Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of human learning and memory (pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Mitz, A. R., Godschalk, M., & Wise, S. P. (1991). Learning-dependent neuronal activity in the premotor cortex: Activity during the acquisition of conditional motor associations. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, Mushiake, H., & Strick, P. L. (1995). Pallidal neuron activity during sequential arm movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, Oorschot, D. E. (1996). Total number of neurons in the neostriatal, pallidal, subthalamic, and substantia nigral nuclei of the rat basal ganglia: A stereological study using the Cavalieri and optical disector methods. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366, Packard, M. G., Introini-Collison, I., & McGaugh, J. L. (1996). Stria terminalis lesions attenuate memory enhancement produced by intracaudate injections of oxotremorine. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65, Packard, M. G., & McGaugh, J. L. (1996). Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65, Parent, A., & Hazrati, L. N. (1995). Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I. The cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop. Brain Research Reviews, 20, Parthasarathy, H. B., & Graybiel, A. M. (1997). Cortically driven immediate-early gene expression reflects modular influence of sensorimotor cortex on identified striatal neurons in the squirrel monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, Parthasarathy, H. B., Schall, J. D., & Graybiel, A. M. (1992). Distributed but convergent ordering of striatal projections: The frontal eye field and the supplementary eye field in the monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, Percheron, G. (1989). In A. R. Crossman & M. A. Sambrook (Eds.), Neural mechanisms in disorders of movement (pp ). London: Libbey. Rauch, S. L., Jenike, M. A., Alpert, N. M., Baer, L., Breiter, H. C., Savage, C. R., & Fischman, A. J. (1994). Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, Rauch, S. L., Savage, C. R., Brown, H. D., Curran, T., Alpert, N. M., Kendrick, A., Fischman, A. J., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1995). A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning. Human Brain Mapping, 3, Rauch, S. L., Whalen, P. J., Savage, C. R., Curran, T., Kendrick, A., Brown, H. D., Bush, G., Breitner, H. C., & Rosen, B. R. (1997). Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 5, Reading, P. J., Dunnett, S. B., & Robbins, T. W. (1991). Dissociable roles of the ventral, medial and lateral striatum on the acquisition and performance of a complex visual stimulus response habit. Behavioural Brain Research, 45, Romo, R., Scarnati, E., & Schultz, W. (1992). Role of primate basal ganglia and frontal cortex in the internal generation of movements. II. Movement-related activity in the anterior striatum. Experimental Brain Research, 91, Rosenkilde, C. E., Bauer, R. H., & Fuster, J. M. (1981). Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys. Brain Research, 209, Routtenberg, A., & Kim, H.-J. (1978). The substantia nigra and neostriatum: Substrates for memory consolidation. In L. L. Butcher (Ed.), Cholinergic monoaminergic interactions in the brain (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1995). Neurobiology of skill and habit learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, Schneider, J. S., & Lidsky, T. I. (1981). Processing of somatosensory information in the striatum of behaving cats. Journal of Neurophysiology, 45,

18 136 ANN M. GRAYBIEL Schultz, W. (1997). Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, Schwartz, J. M., Stoessel, P. W., Baxter, L. R., Jr., Martin, K. M., & Phelps, M. E. (1996). Systemic changes in cerebral glucose metabolic rate after successful behavior modification treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, Seitz, R. J., & Roland, P. E. (1992). Learning of sequential finger movements in man: A combined kinematic and positron emission tomography (PET) study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 4, Seitz, R. J., Roland, P. E., Bohm, C., Greitz, T., & Stone-Elander, S. (1990). Motor learning in man: A positron emission tomographic study. NeuroReport, 1, Sgambato, V., Abo, V., Rogard, M., Besson, M.-J., & Deniau, J. M. (1997). Effect of electrical stimulation of the cerebral cortex on the Fos expression in the basal ganglia. Neuroscience, 81, Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge Univ. Press. Shima, K., Mushiake, H., Saito, N., & Tanji, J. (1996). Role for cells in the presupplementary motor area in updating motor plans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, Suzuki, W. A. (1996). Neuroanatomy of the monkey entorhinal, perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices: Organization of inputs and interconnections. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 8, Swedo, S. E., Pietrini, P., Leonard, H. L., Schapiro, M. B., Rettew, D. C., Goldberger, E. L., Rapoport, S. I., Rapoport, J. L., & Grady, C. L. (1992). Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhoodonset obsessive compulsive disorder: Revisualization during pharmacotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, Tanji, J., & Shima, K. (1994). Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead. Nature, 371, Thorpe, S. J., Rolls, E. T., & Maddison, S. (1983). The orbitofrontal cortex: Neuronal activity in the behaving monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 49, Turner, R. S., & Anderson, M. E. (1997). Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, Vriezen, E. R., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Memory for temporal order and conditional associatelearning in patients with Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychologia, 28, Walker, R. H., Arbuthnott, G. W., Baughman, R. W., & Graybiel, A. M. (1993). Dendritic domains of medium spiny neurons in the primate striatum: Relationships to striosomal borders. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 337, Walker, R. H., & Graybiel, A. M. (1993). Dendritic arbors of spiny neurons in the primate striatum are directionally polarized. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 337, Wang, X., Merzenich, M. M., Sameshima, K., & Jenkins, W. M. (1995). Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation. Nature, 378, White, N. M. (1997). Mnemonic functions of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, Wickens, J. R., Begg, A. J., & Arbuthnott, G. W. (1996). Dopamine reverses the depression of rat corticostriatal synapses which normally follows high-frequency stimulation of cortex in vitro. Neuroscience, 70, 1 5. Wilson, C. J. (1993). The generation of natural firing patterns in neostriatal neurons. In G. W. Arbuthnott & P. C. Emson (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research: Chemical signaling in the basal ganglia (pp ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Wilson, C. J. (1995). The contribution of cortical neurons to the firing pattern of striatal spiny neurons. In J. C. Houk, J. L. Davis, & D. G. Beiser (Eds.), Models of information processing in the basal ganglia (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Brain Structures That are Involved with Memory

Brain Structures That are Involved with Memory Early Theories of Brain Structures That are Involved with Psychology 372 Sensation Sensory Attention Rehearsal STM Storage Retrieval Physiological Psychology Steven E. Meier, Ph.D. Listen to the audio

More information

Changes in behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum during learning

Changes in behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum during learning Review Vol.26 No.6 June 2003 321 Changes in behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum during learning Wolfram Schultz, Léon Tremblay and Jeffrey R. Hollerman Institute of Physiology, University

More information

Basal Ganglia. Motor systems

Basal Ganglia. Motor systems 409 systems Basal Ganglia You have just read about the different motorrelated cortical areas. Premotor areas are involved in planning, while MI is involved in execution. What you don t know is that the

More information

Circuit Disorders of the Basal Ganglia: Parkinson s Disease Pathophysiology and Surgical Treatments

Circuit Disorders of the Basal Ganglia: Parkinson s Disease Pathophysiology and Surgical Treatments Circuit Disorders of the Basal Ganglia: Parkinson s Disease Pathophysiology and Surgical Treatments Mahlon R. DeLong M.D. W. P. Timmie Professor of Neurology Emory University School of Medicine The Basal

More information

Chapter 7: The Nervous System

Chapter 7: The Nervous System Chapter 7: The Nervous System Objectives Discuss the general organization of the nervous system Describe the structure & function of a nerve Draw and label the pathways involved in a withdraw reflex Define

More information

What is the basic component of the brain and spinal cord communication system?

What is the basic component of the brain and spinal cord communication system? EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY David Myers The Biology of Mind Chapter 2 Neural Communication Neurons How Neurons Communicate How Neurotransmitters Influence Us The Nervous System The Peripheral Nervous System The

More information

Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia

Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia 1 Contents Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia... 1 Introduction... 3 A brief review of cerebellar anatomy... 4 Basic Circuit... 4 Parallel and climbing fiber input has a very different

More information

CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS.

CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS. CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS. 6.1. CONNECTIONS AMONG NEURONS Neurons are interconnected with one another to form circuits, much as electronic components are wired together to form a functional

More information

CONTE Summer Lab Experience Application

CONTE Summer Lab Experience Application CONTE Summer Lab Experience Application When preparing your application for funding from the CONTE Summer Lab Experience through the Undergraduate Program in Neuroscience, please read these instructions

More information

Theta, Gamma, and Working Memory

Theta, Gamma, and Working Memory Theta, Gamma, and Working Memory Lecture 3.8 David S. Touretzky November, 2015 Outline Theta rhythm and gamma rhythms Phase precession in hippocampus Theta and gamma in entorhinal cortex Lisman working

More information

Motor dysfunction 2: Spinal cord injury and subcortical motor disorders ANATOMY REVIEW: Basal Ganglia

Motor dysfunction 2: Spinal cord injury and subcortical motor disorders ANATOMY REVIEW: Basal Ganglia Motor dysfunction 2: Spinal cord injury and subcortical motor disorders ANATOMY REVIEW: Basal Ganglia A group of subcortical nuclei caudate, putamen, globus pallidus Caudate & Putamen = Neostriatum caudate

More information

Meeting Report: XX WFN World Congress on Parkinson s Disease and Related Disorders

Meeting Report: XX WFN World Congress on Parkinson s Disease and Related Disorders Meeting Report: XX WFN World Congress on Parkinson s Disease and Related Disorders Karen Frei, Erik Ch. Wolters Abstract: The twentieth World Congress on Parkinson s Disease and Related Disorders was held

More information

AMPHETAMINE AND COCAINE MECHANISMS AND HAZARDS

AMPHETAMINE AND COCAINE MECHANISMS AND HAZARDS AMPHETAMINE AND COCAINE MECHANISMS AND HAZARDS BARRY J. EVERITT Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge Stimulant drugs, such as cocaine and amphetamine, interact directly with dopamine

More information

THE HUMAN BRAIN. observations and foundations

THE HUMAN BRAIN. observations and foundations THE HUMAN BRAIN observations and foundations brains versus computers a typical brain contains something like 100 billion miniscule cells called neurons estimates go from about 50 billion to as many as

More information

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex 1 Neuroscience 2 Neurons output integration axon cell body, membrane potential Frontal planning control auditory episodes soma motor Temporal Parietal action language objects space vision Occipital inputs

More information

Sheep Brain Dissection

Sheep Brain Dissection Sheep Brain Dissection http://www.carolina.com/product/preserved+organisms/preserved+animals+%28mammal s%29/sheep+organs/preserved+sheep+dissection.do Michigan State University Neuroscience Program Brain

More information

Learning with Your Brain. Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Learning with Your Brain. Teaching With the Brain in Mind Learning with Your Brain Should what (and how) we teach be associated with what we know about the brain and the nervous system? Jonathan Karp, Ph.D. Dept of Biology 5/20/2004 Teaching With the Brain in

More information

Structure and Function of Neurons

Structure and Function of Neurons CHPTER 1 Structure and Function of Neurons Varieties of neurons General structure Structure of unique neurons Internal operations and the functioning of a neuron Subcellular organelles Protein synthesis

More information

The Neuron and the Synapse. The Neuron. Parts of the Neuron. Functions of the neuron:

The Neuron and the Synapse. The Neuron. Parts of the Neuron. Functions of the neuron: The Neuron and the Synapse The Neuron Functions of the neuron: Transmit information from one point in the body to another. Process the information in various ways (that is, compute). The neuron has a specialized

More information

MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE (THE LIMBIC SYSTEM)

MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE (THE LIMBIC SYSTEM) MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE (THE LIMBIC SYSTEM) On the medial surface of the temporal lobe are three structures critical for normal human functioning. From rostral to caudal, they are the olfactory cortex, the

More information

UNIVERSITY OF BOLTON EDUCATION & PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY SEMESTER 1 EXAMINATIONS 2014/2015 COGNITIVE & BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES MODULE NO: PSC4003

UNIVERSITY OF BOLTON EDUCATION & PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY SEMESTER 1 EXAMINATIONS 2014/2015 COGNITIVE & BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES MODULE NO: PSC4003 [EDP 005] UNIVERSITY OF BOLTON EDUCATION & PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY SEMESTER 1 EXAMINATIONS 2014/2015 COGNITIVE & BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES MODULE NO: PSC4003 Date: Wednesday 21 st January, 2015 Time: 2.00pm

More information

Human Neuroanatomy. Grades 9-12. Driving Question: How did the evolution of the human brain impact the structure and function it has today?

Human Neuroanatomy. Grades 9-12. Driving Question: How did the evolution of the human brain impact the structure and function it has today? Human Neuroanatomy Grades 9-12 Driving Question: How did the evolution of the human brain impact the structure and function it has today? Objectives: Students will be able to Describe the basic parts and

More information

Slide 4: Forebrain Structures. Slide 5: 4 Lobes of the Cerebral Cortex. Slide 6: The Cerebral Hemispheres (L & R)

Slide 4: Forebrain Structures. Slide 5: 4 Lobes of the Cerebral Cortex. Slide 6: The Cerebral Hemispheres (L & R) Slide 1: [Film Clip: The Brain #2- Phineas Gage] Integrated Bodily Communications Within Brain (Hemispheres and structures) The remaining Nervous System Endocrine System (Hormonal communication) Our bodies-

More information

Brain Matters: Brain Anatomy

Brain Matters: Brain Anatomy 1 : Brain Anatomy Lesson Overview Students share what they already know about brain structure and function, and then, guided by descriptions of brain regions explore the G2C Online 3-D Brain to learn more

More information

Neurobiology of Depression in Relation to ECT. PJ Cowen Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford

Neurobiology of Depression in Relation to ECT. PJ Cowen Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford Neurobiology of Depression in Relation to ECT PJ Cowen Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford Causes of Depression Genetic Childhood experience Life Events (particularly losses) Life Difficulties

More information

Vision: Receptors. Modes of Perception. Vision: Summary 9/28/2012. How do we perceive our environment? Sensation and Perception Terminology

Vision: Receptors. Modes of Perception. Vision: Summary 9/28/2012. How do we perceive our environment? Sensation and Perception Terminology How do we perceive our environment? Complex stimuli are broken into individual features, relayed to the CNS, then reassembled as our perception Sensation and Perception Terminology Stimulus: physical agent

More information

Unit 2 - Subcortical systems, neurochemistry and brain function

Unit 2 - Subcortical systems, neurochemistry and brain function Unit 2 - Subcortical systems, neurochemistry and brain function Subcortical anatomy: Most of the five major subdivisions of the brain are subcortical. I. Telencephalon (cortical - part of forebrain) -

More information

NEUROANATOMY 6 Limbic System

NEUROANATOMY 6 Limbic System NEUROANATOMY 6 Limbic System The Limbic System The part of the brain involved with learning, memory and emotion. It is affected in many neuropsychiatric diseases including schizophrenia, Alzheimer s disease

More information

MITOCW MIT9_14S09_lec33-mp3

MITOCW MIT9_14S09_lec33-mp3 MITOCW MIT9_14S09_lec33-mp3 The following content is provided under a Creative Commons License. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high quality educational resources for free.

More information

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress Chapter Fourteen Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress EMOTIONS! Emotions generally include a Physical component, and a Subjective component or quality, and a Valence Emotions a product of Evolution?

More information

Multifamily Groups in the Treatment of Severe Psychiatric Disorders

Multifamily Groups in the Treatment of Severe Psychiatric Disorders Text from pages 7-12 of Multifamily Groups in the Treatment of Severe Psychiatric Disorders By William R. McFarlane (2002) ISBN 1-57230-743-9. Published by The guilford Press, 72 Spring Street, New York,

More information

Lecture One: Brain Basics

Lecture One: Brain Basics Lecture One: Brain Basics Brain Fractured Femur Bone Spinal Cord 1 How does pain get from here to here 2 How does the brain work? Every cell in your body is wired to send a signal to your brain The brain

More information

Reading Materials Required readings: Psychology 3FA3 courseware (available in Campus Bookstore) Background readings Supplementary readings

Reading Materials Required readings: Psychology 3FA3 courseware (available in Campus Bookstore) Background readings Supplementary readings Psych 3FA3: Neurobiology of Learning and Memory (2002-2003, Term 2) Instructor: Dr. Hongjin Sun, email: 3fa3@psychology.mcmaster.ca Office: Room 415, Psychology Building, Tel 905-525-9140, ext 24367 Lab:

More information

Origin of Electrical Membrane Potential

Origin of Electrical Membrane Potential Origin of Electrical Membrane Potential parti This book is about the physiological characteristics of nerve and muscle cells. As we shall see, the ability of these cells to generate and conduct electricity

More information

Nervous System Organization. PNS and CNS. Nerves. Peripheral Nervous System. Peripheral Nervous System. Motor Component.

Nervous System Organization. PNS and CNS. Nerves. Peripheral Nervous System. Peripheral Nervous System. Motor Component. Nervous System Organization PNS and CNS Chapters 8 and 9 Peripheral Nervous System (PNS) connects CNS to sensory receptors, muscles and glands Central Nervous System (CNS) control/integrating center brain

More information

The Consolidation of Memory

The Consolidation of Memory Emily Wang 9.00 Paper II Due October 8, 2004 The Consolidation of Memory Specific Aims Memory is a phrase that does not describe one entity, but rather encompasses many different processes in the brain

More information

Skill acquisition. Skill acquisition: Closed loop theory Feedback guides learning a motor skill. Problems. Motor learning practice

Skill acquisition. Skill acquisition: Closed loop theory Feedback guides learning a motor skill. Problems. Motor learning practice Motor learning theories closed loop theory schema theory hierarchical theory Skill acquisition Motor learning practice Fitt s three stages motor imagery physical changes Skill acquisition: Closed loop

More information

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology 1.Obtaining Knowledge 1. Correlation 2. Causation 2.Hypothesis Generation & Measures 3.Looking into

More information

Appendix 4 Simulation software for neuronal network models

Appendix 4 Simulation software for neuronal network models Appendix 4 Simulation software for neuronal network models D.1 Introduction This Appendix describes the Matlab software that has been made available with Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation (Rolls

More information

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention Logan Trujillo, Ph.D. Post-Doctoral Fellow University of Texas at Austin Cognitive Science Course, Fall 2008 What is Attention? Everyone knows what attention

More information

Chapter 3 The Anatomy of the Nervous System

Chapter 3 The Anatomy of the Nervous System Chapter 3 The Anatomy of the Nervous System Systems, Structures, and Cells That Make Up Your Nervous System 1 General Layout of the Nervous System Central Nervous System (CNS) Brain (in the skull) Spinal

More information

Seminar/Talk Calendar

Seminar/Talk Calendar Seminar/Talk Calendar Tuesday, February 3rd Dr. John Neumaier, Professor of Psychiatry and Pharmacology, University of Washington DREADDing Addiction Dr. Neumaier s laboratory is studying stress and addiction

More information

Introduction to Neurophysiological Psychology (PSY 451)

Introduction to Neurophysiological Psychology (PSY 451) Portland State University Physiological Psychology Page 1 Introduction to Neurophysiological Psychology (PSY 451) Bill Griesar, Ph.D., Adjunct Instructor, bgriesar@pacifier.com OFFICE HOURS: Mondays, 11:30

More information

How are Parts of the Brain Related to Brain Function?

How are Parts of the Brain Related to Brain Function? How are Parts of the Brain Related to Brain Function? Scientists have found That the basic anatomical components of brain function are related to brain size and shape. The brain is composed of two hemispheres.

More information

Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control Kenji Doya

Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control Kenji Doya 732 Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control Kenji Doya The classical notion that the basal ganglia and the cerebellum are dedicated to motor control has been challenged

More information

Prevention & Recovery Conference November 28, 29 & 30 Norman, Ok

Prevention & Recovery Conference November 28, 29 & 30 Norman, Ok Prevention & Recovery Conference November 28, 29 & 30 Norman, Ok What is Addiction? The American Society of Addiction Medicine (ASAM) released on August 15, 2011 their latest definition of addiction:

More information

MULTIPLE REWARD SIGNALS IN THE BRAIN

MULTIPLE REWARD SIGNALS IN THE BRAIN MULTIPLE REWARD SIGNALS IN THE BRAIN Wolfram Schultz The fundamental biological importance of rewards has created an increasing interest in the neuronal processing of reward information. The suggestion

More information

Biological Neurons and Neural Networks, Artificial Neurons

Biological Neurons and Neural Networks, Artificial Neurons Biological Neurons and Neural Networks, Artificial Neurons Neural Computation : Lecture 2 John A. Bullinaria, 2015 1. Organization of the Nervous System and Brain 2. Brains versus Computers: Some Numbers

More information

A Biophysical Network Model Displaying the Role of Basal Ganglia Pathways in Action Selection

A Biophysical Network Model Displaying the Role of Basal Ganglia Pathways in Action Selection A Biophysical Network Model Displaying the Role of Basal Ganglia Pathways in Action Selection Cem Yucelgen, Berat Denizdurduran, Selin Metin, Rahmi Elibol, N. Serap Sengor Istanbul Technical University,

More information

The intermedius nucleus of the medulla: A potential site for the integration of cervical information and the generation of autonomic responses

The intermedius nucleus of the medulla: A potential site for the integration of cervical information and the generation of autonomic responses The intermedius nucleus of the medulla: A potential site for the integration of cervical information and the generation of autonomic responses 1 Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy November 2009, 38, pp.

More information

CHAPTER 5 SIGNALLING IN NEURONS

CHAPTER 5 SIGNALLING IN NEURONS 5.1. SYNAPTIC TRANSMISSION CHAPTER 5 SIGNALLING IN NEURONS One of the main functions of neurons is to communicate with other neurons. An individual neuron may receive information from many different sources.

More information

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data.

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data. Masters research projects 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data. Background. Granger causality is a concept introduced in the field of economy to determine which variables influence, or cause,

More information

In a number of recent studies investigators have shown that

In a number of recent studies investigators have shown that Cortical correlates of learning in monkeys adapting to a new dynamical environment F. Gandolfo, C.-S. R. Li*, B. J. Benda, C. Padoa Schioppa, and E. Bizzi Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts

More information

Neurophysiology. 2.1 Equilibrium Potential

Neurophysiology. 2.1 Equilibrium Potential 2 Neurophysiology 2.1 Equilibrium Potential An understanding of the concepts of electrical and chemical forces that act on ions, electrochemical equilibrium, and equilibrium potential is a powerful tool

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect04: Brain & Spine Neuroanatomy

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect04: Brain & Spine Neuroanatomy CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect02: BOSS Discrete Event Simulator Lect04: Brain & Spine Neuroanatomy Appendix of Purves et al., 4e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook

More information

Sensory-motor control scheme based on Kohonen Maps and AVITE model

Sensory-motor control scheme based on Kohonen Maps and AVITE model Sensory-motor control scheme based on Kohonen Maps and AVITE model Juan L. Pedreño-Molina, Antonio Guerrero-González, Oscar A. Florez-Giraldo, J. Molina-Vilaplana Technical University of Cartagena Department

More information

Nerves and Nerve Impulse

Nerves and Nerve Impulse Nerves and Nerve Impulse Terms Absolute refractory period: Period following stimulation during which no additional action potential can be evoked. Acetylcholine: Chemical transmitter substance released

More information

ANIMATED NEUROSCIENCE

ANIMATED NEUROSCIENCE ANIMATED NEUROSCIENCE and the Action of Nicotine, Cocaine, and Marijuana in the Brain Te a c h e r s G u i d e Films for the Humanities & Sciences Background Information This program, made entirely of

More information

Slide 1: Introduction Introduce the purpose of your presentation. Indicate that you will explain how the brain basically works and how and where

Slide 1: Introduction Introduce the purpose of your presentation. Indicate that you will explain how the brain basically works and how and where Slide 1: Introduction Introduce the purpose of your presentation. Indicate that you will explain how the brain basically works and how and where drugs such as heroin and cocaine work in the brain. Tell

More information

Introduction to Psychology, 7th Edition, Rod Plotnik Module 3: Brain s Building Blocks. Module 3. Brain s Building Blocks

Introduction to Psychology, 7th Edition, Rod Plotnik Module 3: Brain s Building Blocks. Module 3. Brain s Building Blocks Module 3 Brain s Building Blocks Structure of the Brain Genes chains of chemicals that are arranged like rungs on a twisting ladder there are about 100,000 genes that contain chemical instructions that

More information

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain).

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). Functional neuroimaging Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). The brain is bloody & electric Blood increase in neuronal activity increase in metabolic demand for glucose

More information

Name: Teacher: Olsen Hour:

Name: Teacher: Olsen Hour: Name: Teacher: Olsen Hour: The Nervous System: Part 1 Textbook p216-225 41 In all exercises, quizzes and tests in this class, always answer in your own words. That is the only way that you can show that

More information

Adrian Owens Research

Adrian Owens Research *Classic View: Not entirely correct Nervous System Central Nervous System is made up of the brain and the spinal cord. Peripheral Nervous System is broken down into a component called the somatic division.

More information

DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN

DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN DISSECTION OF THE SHEEP'S BRAIN Introduction The purpose of the sheep brain dissection is to familiarize you with the threedimensional structure of the brain and teach you one of the great methods of studying

More information

Bayesian probability theory

Bayesian probability theory Bayesian probability theory Bruno A. Olshausen arch 1, 2004 Abstract Bayesian probability theory provides a mathematical framework for peforming inference, or reasoning, using probability. The foundations

More information

Visual Attention and Emotional Perception

Visual Attention and Emotional Perception Visual Attention and Emotional Perception Luiz Pessoa 1 and Leslie G. Ungerleider 2 (1) Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, RI (2) Laboratory of Brain & Cognition, National Institute

More information

Standards Alignment Minnesota Science Standards Alignment Matrix www.brainu.org/resources/mnstds

Standards Alignment Minnesota Science Standards Alignment Matrix www.brainu.org/resources/mnstds Lesson Summary: Neurons transfer information by releasing neurotransmitters across the synapse or space between neurons. Students model the chemical communication between pre-synaptic and post-synaptic

More information

31.1 The Neuron. BUILD Vocabulary. Lesson Objectives

31.1 The Neuron. BUILD Vocabulary. Lesson Objectives Name Class Date 31.1 The Neuron Lesson Objectives Identify the functions of the nervous system. Describe the function of neurons. Describe how a nerve impulse is transmitted. BUILD Vocabulary A. The chart

More information

Chapter 7: The Nervous System

Chapter 7: The Nervous System Chapter 7: The Nervous System I. Organization of the Nervous System Objectives: List the general functions of the nervous system Explain the structural and functional classifications of the nervous system

More information

Chapter 9 Nervous System

Chapter 9 Nervous System Chapter 9 Nervous System Nervous System function: The nervous system is composed of neurons and neuroglia. at the ends of peripheral nerves gather information and convert it into nerve impulses. When sensory

More information

Brain Damage & Recovery: The Resilience of the Brain, Addiction Impact & Therapeutic Repair. Michael Fishman, MD Director of Young Adult Program

Brain Damage & Recovery: The Resilience of the Brain, Addiction Impact & Therapeutic Repair. Michael Fishman, MD Director of Young Adult Program Brain Damage & Recovery: The Resilience of the Brain, Addiction Impact & Therapeutic Repair Michael Fishman, MD Director of Young Adult Program How Addiction Takes Hold Large & rapid upsurges in dopamine

More information

Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: a pharmacological treatment approach

Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: a pharmacological treatment approach Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: a pharmacological treatment approach Professor Alasdair Vance Head, Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics University of Melbourne Royal Children s Hospital

More information

Auditory neuroanatomy: the Spanish heritage. Santiago Ramón y Cajal, 1852 1934

Auditory neuroanatomy: the Spanish heritage. Santiago Ramón y Cajal, 1852 1934 Auditory neuroanatomy: the Spanish heritage Santiago Ramón y Cajal, 1852 1934 Rafael Lorente de Nó, 1902 1990 3 The nervous system is made up of cells. Estimates of the number of cells vary from

More information

CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY CENTRE FOR BRAIN REPAIR A layman's account of our scientific objectives What is Brain Damage? Many forms of trauma and disease affect the nervous system to produce permanent neurological

More information

The Brain. What is it? Neurons Glial Cells Connective Tissue Connective Fiber White Matter Grey Matter Cerebro-spinal Fluid

The Brain. What is it? Neurons Glial Cells Connective Tissue Connective Fiber White Matter Grey Matter Cerebro-spinal Fluid The Brain What is it? Neurons Glial Cells Connective Tissue Connective Fiber White Matter Grey Matter Cerebro-spinal Fluid A More Realistic View When we look at the brain we see mostly the Cerebral Cortex

More information

: " ; j t ;-..,-.: ',-. LEARNING AND MEMORY AN INTEGRATED APPROACH. Second Edition. John R. Anderson Carnegie Mellon University

:  ; j t ;-..,-.: ',-. LEARNING AND MEMORY AN INTEGRATED APPROACH. Second Edition. John R. Anderson Carnegie Mellon University : " ; j t ;-..,-.: ',-. 2008 AGI-Information Management Consultants May be used for personal purporses only or by libraries associated to dandelon.com network. LEARNING AND MEMORY AN INTEGRATED APPROACH

More information

How To Understand The Function Of The Basal Ganglia

How To Understand The Function Of The Basal Ganglia 122 NEUROCIRCUITRY OF PARKINSON S DISEASE THOMAS WICHMANN MAHLON R. DELONG Recent progress in neuroscience research has led to major insights into the structure and function of the basal ganglia and into

More information

Chapter 13. The Nature of Somatic Reflexes

Chapter 13. The Nature of Somatic Reflexes Chapter 13 The Nature of Somatic Reflexes Nature of Reflexes (1 of 3) A reflex is an involuntary responses initiated by a sensory input resulting in a change in a gland or muscle tissue occur without our

More information

3. The neuron has many branch-like extensions called that receive input from other neurons. a. glia b. dendrites c. axons d.

3. The neuron has many branch-like extensions called that receive input from other neurons. a. glia b. dendrites c. axons d. Chapter Test 1. A cell that receives information and transmits it to other cells via an electrochemical process is called a(n) a. neuron b. hormone c. glia d. endorphin Answer: A difficulty: 1 factual

More information

Neurotransmission: Muscle Messages

Neurotransmission: Muscle Messages 2 Neurotransmission: Muscle Messages GOAL The goal of this unit is to reinforce the process of neuromuscular transmission with hands-on materials. Set-up: -Reaction Time poster -Nerve-muscle poster -Synapse

More information

FUNCTIONAL EEG ANALYZE IN AUTISM. Dr. Plamen Dimitrov

FUNCTIONAL EEG ANALYZE IN AUTISM. Dr. Plamen Dimitrov FUNCTIONAL EEG ANALYZE IN AUTISM Dr. Plamen Dimitrov Preamble Autism or Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD) is a mental developmental disorder, manifested in the early childhood and is characterized by qualitative

More information

Nervous System sensor input integration motor output sensory organs central nervous system

Nervous System sensor input integration motor output sensory organs central nervous system Nervous System Nervous system performs three overlapping functions of sensor input, integration, and motor output. This process is generally the same even at a very primitive level of nervous system, but

More information

How songbirds sing birdsongs?

How songbirds sing birdsongs? How songbirds sing birdsongs? Liora Las Michale Fee (MIT) Veit et al. (J Neurophysiol, 2011) Outline: 1) Introduction to songbirds as a model. 2) Neuronal circuits underlying mature song production (motor

More information

http://abcnews.go.com/politics/video/obama-says-brain-initiative-will-be-transformative-18861944

http://abcnews.go.com/politics/video/obama-says-brain-initiative-will-be-transformative-18861944 http://abcnews.go.com/politics/video/obama-says-brain-initiative-will-be-transformative-18861944 What are the nervous system s functions? The nervous system organizes and controls an individual s appropriate

More information

How to test ocular movements in PSP Jan Kassubek

How to test ocular movements in PSP Jan Kassubek How to test ocular movements in PSP Jan Kassubek Universitätsklinik für Neurologie, Ulm Bedside Screening: PSP initially slowing of vertical saccades slowing of downward saccades is considered the hallmark

More information

Review Paper Cognitive Neuroscience and Education: Understanding the Teaching Learning Strategies, Learning Disabilities and Neuromyths

Review Paper Cognitive Neuroscience and Education: Understanding the Teaching Learning Strategies, Learning Disabilities and Neuromyths Research Journal of Educational Sciences ISSN 2321-0508 Review Paper Cognitive Neuroscience and Education: Understanding the Teaching Learning Strategies, Learning Disabilities and Neuromyths Abstract

More information

Function (& other notes)

Function (& other notes) LAB 8. ANATOMY OF THE HUMAN BRAIN In this exercise you each will map the human brain both anatomy and function so that you can develop a more accurate picture of what s going on in your head :-) EXTERNAL

More information

Chapter 15. Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) and Visceral Reflexes. general properties Anatomy. Autonomic effects on target organs

Chapter 15. Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) and Visceral Reflexes. general properties Anatomy. Autonomic effects on target organs Chapter 15 Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) and Visceral Reflexes general properties Anatomy Autonomic effects on target organs Central control of autonomic function 15-1 Copyright (c) The McGraw-Hill Companies,

More information

American Society of Addiction Medicine

American Society of Addiction Medicine American Society of Addiction Medicine Public Policy Statement: Definition of Addiction (Long Version) Addiction is a primary, chronic disease of brain reward, motivation, memory and related circuitry.

More information

Chapter 28. Drug Treatment of Parkinson s Disease

Chapter 28. Drug Treatment of Parkinson s Disease Chapter 28 Drug Treatment of Parkinson s Disease 1. Introduction Parkinsonism Tremors hands and head develop involuntary movements when at rest; pin rolling sign (finger and thumb) Muscle rigidity arthritis

More information

Nervous System: PNS and CNS

Nervous System: PNS and CNS Nervous System: PNS and CNS Biology 105 Lecture 10 Chapter 8 Outline I. Central Nervous System vs Peripheral Nervous System II. Peripheral Nervous System A. Somatic Nervous System B. Autonomic Nervous

More information

Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will

Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will Patrick Haggard Abstract The capacity for voluntary action is seen as essential to human nature. Yet neuroscience and behaviourist psychology have traditionally

More information

Addiction Neurobiology

Addiction Neurobiology Addiction Neurobiology Stephen Jurd University of Sydney Australia Richard W is sick Apology The site of pathology IF Addiction has a neurobiological basis THEN we should be able to: Define addiction AND

More information

NEUROLOCALIZATION MADE EASY

NEUROLOCALIZATION MADE EASY NEUROLOCALIZATION MADE EASY Jared B. Galle, DVM, Diplomate ACVIM (Neurology) Dogwood Veterinary Referral Center 4920 Ann Arbor-Saline Road Ann Arbor, MI 48103 Localizing a neurologic problem to an anatomical

More information

Physiological Basis of the BOLD Signal. Kerstin Preuschoff Social and Neural systems Lab University of Zurich

Physiological Basis of the BOLD Signal. Kerstin Preuschoff Social and Neural systems Lab University of Zurich Physiological Basis of the BOLD Signal Kerstin Preuschoff Social and Neural systems Lab University of Zurich Source: Arthurs & Boniface, 2002 From Stimulus to Bold Overview Physics of BOLD signal - Magnetic

More information

Human Physiology Study Questions-2

Human Physiology Study Questions-2 Human Physiology Study Questions-2 Action potentials: Handout-8, Chapter 8 1. Explain the positive feedback component of an action potential that is, how the opening of one voltage-gated sodium (or calcium)

More information

Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel

Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel Brain Development and Traumainformed Services for Emerging Adults Hood River, OR February 21, 2015 2015 Conference: Making Connections Julie M.

More information

The Cranium Connection

The Cranium Connection Your Brain! The brain is the command center of your body. It controls just about everything you do, even when you are sleeping. Weighing about 3 pounds, the brain is made up of many parts that all work

More information

THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MEMORY

THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MEMORY THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MEMORY What is memory? It is often associated with the thinking of again or recalling to the mind of something learned at an earlier time. Descriptions of this sort imply

More information

Chapter 15. The Autonomic Nervous. The Autonomic Nervous System. Autonomic Motor Pathways. ANS vs. SNS

Chapter 15. The Autonomic Nervous. The Autonomic Nervous System. Autonomic Motor Pathways. ANS vs. SNS The Autonomic Nervous System Chapter 15 The subconscious involuntary nervous system Regulates activity of smooth muscle, cardiac muscle & certain glands The Autonomic Nervous System 1 2 ANS vs. SNS Somatic

More information