Sign Language Phonology Wendy Sandler

Similar documents
Phonology, phonetics and the nondominant hand

SIGN LANGUAGE: AN OVERVIEW Wendy Sandler The University of Haifa

How To Understand Two Handed Signs In Ksl

Sign Language Linguistics Course texts Overview Assessment Week 1: Introduction and history of sign language research

Effective Animation of Sign Language with Prosodic Elements for Annotation of Digital Educational Content

Stricture and Nasal Place Assimilation. Jaye Padgett

Are signed languages "real" languages? Evidence from American Sign Language and Langue des Signes Québécoise Reprinted from

ED Foreign Language. Requirement? Why Not American Sign. Language? ERIC Digests. ERIC Development Team

Speech Production 2. Paper 9: Foundations of Speech Communication Lent Term: Week 4. Katharine Barden

Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language*

Linguistic research over the last few decades

Intonation difficulties in non-native languages.

Understanding sign language classifiers through a polycomponential approach

American Sign Language and ASL Linguistics

The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Significance of the Lexicon. Australian? The Significance of the Lexicon 澳 大 利 亚 奥 地 利

The Role of Gestalt in Language Processing

Gesture and ASL L2 Acquisition 1

Text-To-Speech Technologies for Mobile Telephony Services

Towards automated surfacelevel phonetic analysis of SL. Still cont d! Preamble

Functional Auditory Performance Indicators (FAPI)

Segmentti (1)? Segmentit ja niiden problematiikka. Edmondson (1990: 70) (id., 1990: 72) The WLB in Linguistics. de Saussure ([1916: 65] 1983: 41)

American Sign Language From a Psycholinguistic and Cultural Perspective Spring 2012 Syllabus Linguistics 242

A Survey of ASL Tenses

Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) Certificate Programs

University of Massachusetts Boston Applied Linguistics Graduate Program. APLING 601 Introduction to Linguistics. Syllabus

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. At the completion of this study there are many people that I need to thank. Foremost of

Movement Phases in Signs and Co-Speech Gestures, and their Transcription by Human Coders

Contemporary Linguistics

Sign language transcription conventions for the ECHO Project

ADVERBIAL MORPHEMES IN TACTILE AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE PROJECT DEMONSTRATING EXCELLENCE. Submitted to the

How To Understand A Deaf Person

Aspects of North Swedish intonational phonology. Bruce, Gösta

209 THE STRUCTURE AND USE OF ENGLISH.

Prosodic focus marking in Bai

Technical Report #2 Testing Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing

Master of Arts in Linguistics Syllabus

Developing an e-learning platform for the Greek Sign Language

Phonetic and phonological properties of the final pitch accent in Catalan declaratives

Extended Projections of Adjectives and Comparative Deletion

COURSE SYLLABUS ESU 561 ASPECTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE. Fall 2014

What Is Linguistics? December 1992 Center for Applied Linguistics

Basic Theory of Intermedia Composing with Sounds and Images

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

American Sign Language

CURRICULUM VITAE Allan B. Smith, Ph.D., CCC-SLP. Education Ph.D. 2002, University of Connecticut (Communication Science).

How Children Acquire Language: A New Answer by Dr. Laura Ann Petitto

Toward computational understanding of sign language

SWING: A tool for modelling intonational varieties of Swedish Beskow, Jonas; Bruce, Gösta; Enflo, Laura; Granström, Björn; Schötz, Susanne

Office Phone/ / lix@cwu.edu Office Hours: MW 3:50-4:50, TR 12:00-12:30

The syllable as emerging unit of information, processing, production

The Languages of Africa LIN 4930/6932. SSA Spring T 7 T 7-8. WEIM 1084

SPEECH OR LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION

Mary E. Beckman Curriculum Vitae Education and employment: Extramural research and teaching: Other professional activities:

TOOLS for DEVELOPING Communication PLANS

UNIVERSITY OF PUERTO RICO RIO PIEDRAS CAMPUS COLLEGE OF HUMANITIES DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH

Monitoring Modality vs. Typology

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DELL AQUILA CENTRO LINGUISTICO DI ATENEO

Mahesh Srinivasan. Assistant Professor of Psychology and Cognitive Science University of California, Berkeley

Things to remember when transcribing speech

Prosodic Phrasing: Machine and Human Evaluation

What s happening with HAPPEN? The Grammaticalization of HAPPEN in American Sign Language

A Database Tool for Research. on Visual-Gestural Language

Reading Competencies

Running head: FINGERSPELLING IN ASL DISCOURSE 1

Julia Hirschberg. AT&T Bell Laboratories. Murray Hill, New Jersey 07974

Perspective taking strategies in Turkish Sign Language and Croatian Sign Language

Reporting of Interpreting as a Related Service. on the PEIMS 163 Student Data Record

Swedish for Immigrants

MICHIGAN TEST FOR TEACHER CERTIFICATION (MTTC) TEST OBJECTIVES FIELD 062: HEARING IMPAIRED

Lexical Competition: Round in English and Dutch

CURRICULUM VITA. ELIZABETH A. WINSTON, Ph.D. Center

Bachelor in Deaf Studies

Prosodic Characteristics of Emotional Speech: Measurements of Fundamental Frequency Movements

Manual signs: what does research tell?

Pronunciation Difficulties of Japanese Speakers of English: Predictions Based on a Contrastive Analysis Steven W. Carruthers

A limitation on reading as a source of linguistic input: Evidence from deaf learners

INVESTIGATING DISCOURSE MARKERS IN PEDAGOGICAL SETTINGS:

Difficulties that Arab Students Face in Learning English and the Importance of the Writing Skill Acquisition Key Words:

Instructional Design: Objectives, Curriculum and Lesson Plans for Reading Sylvia Linan-Thompson, The University of Texas at Austin Haitham Taha,

2) Language: Lesion Studies Demonstrating the Left Hemisphere s Dominance for Language - Broca s Aphasia, Wernicke s Aphasia

Using an Animation-based Technology to Support Reading Curricula for Deaf Elementary Schoolchildren

WinPitch LTL II, a Multimodal Pronunciation Software

Ph.D in Speech-Language Pathology

Author's Name: Stuart Davis Article Contract Number: 17106A/0180 Article Serial Number: Article Title: Loanwords, Phonological Treatment of

Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language

Module Catalogue for the Bachelor Program in Computational Linguistics at the University of Heidelberg

1. Introduction 1.1 Contact

Historical Linguistics. Diachronic Analysis. Two Approaches to the Study of Language. Kinds of Language Change. What is Historical Linguistics?

From Gesture to Sign Language: Conventionalization of Classifier Constructions by Adult Hearing Learners of British Sign Language

Socio-Cultural Knowledge in Conversational Inference

Pronunciation in English

Issues in Second Language Literacy Education with Learners who are Deaf. Connie Mayer Faculty of Education, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Visualizing WordNet Structure

Reading Specialist (151)

ADVANTAGES OF EARLY VISUAL LANGUAGE

Speaking of Writing and Writing of Speaking

LEARNING WITH SIGN AND LIPREADING : ONLINE MULTIMEDIA EDUCATIONAL SOFTWARE

English Speech Timing: A Domain and Locus Approach. Laurence White

Common Phonological processes - There are several kinds of familiar processes that are found in many many languages.

Annotation in Language Documentation

Transcription:

Sign Language Phonology. Wendy Sandler. The University of Haifa. To appear in: The Oxford International Encyclopedia of Linguistics. William Frawley, Ed. Sign Language Phonology Wendy Sandler Phonology is more closely tied to the production and perception systems than any other abstract level of linguistic structure. As sign languages are transmitted in a different physical modality, the discovery that they too have a phonology has therefore been considered especially significant. Stokoe (1960) demonstrated that the signs in the lexicon of American Sign Language (ASL) are not holistic gestures, but are comprised of a relatively small number of meaningless units that may recombine to produce a potentially large lexicon. Since that pioneering work, other researchers have shown that these units are subject to constraints on their combination, and are systematically altered in different phonological and morphological contexts. In addition to these shared general characteristics, certain formal similarities between phonologies in the two modalities have also emerged. Most is known about the phonology of ASL; however, other sign languages, such as Sign Language of the Netherlands and Israeli Sign Language, are being shown to have similar phonological properties. Stokoe showed that features of handshape, location, and movement can recombine to form minimal pairs of signs. For example, the signs DRY, UGLY and SUMMER in ASL are minimally distinguished by features of location. DRY UGLY SUMMER Figure 1. Minimal triplet in ASL, distinguished by features of location. (Reprinted with permission from Klima and Bellugi, 1979.) While early analyses emphasize the apparent simultaneity with which the formational elements are articulated, subsequent studies point to significant sequential properties (Liddell, 1984a; Sandler, 1989; Brentari, 1998), and models have been developed to reflect this sequentiality (Liddell and Johnson, 1989; Sandler, 1989; Perlmutter, 1992; van der Hulst, 1993). One of many examples of sequentiality is seen in the morphological operation of verb agreement, which requires distinct reference to the beginning and ending of the sign.

2 Figure 2. I GIVE YOU; S/HE GIVES ME reprinted with permission from A Basic Course in American Sign Language. The illustrations above show that on the surface, signs are typically sequences of two locations with a movement in between. The number of sequential units is limited, which contributes to the impression of simultaneity. Another reason for this impression is that key features of the most complex element, the handshape, generally remain constant throughout a sign (Mandel, 1981). To account for both sequential and simultaneous properties, the theories of autosegmental phonology (Goldsmith, 1976) and nonconcatenative morphology (McCarthy, 1979) have been adopted from spoken language research. The hand configuration class with long distance properties may be represented on a separate tier, a representation that has explanatory consequences for other aspects of the phonology and morphology (Sandler 1986, 1989). HC L M L Figure 3. Sequential Location and Movement segments and a Hand Configuration augosegment (Sandler, 1989) This separation of the HC category from other categories facilitated the further development of hierarchical representations of articulator-based feature classes within the HC category (following Clements, 1985), which capture partial and total assimilation phenomena (Sandler, 1987; Corina, 1990a). Various morphological operations require signs to conform to a prosodic template that may be partially prespecified (Liddell 1984b). This templatic approach expresses formally the observation that the morphological complexity of sign language words is in some sense simultaneous -- i.e., nonconcatenative. For example, some ASL temporal aspect morphology changes the single straight movement path of a verb to a repeated circular one (Klima & Bellugi, 1979). This is analyzed as associating a verbal root to an LML template with an arc feature preassociated to the movement segment, and then reduplicating (Sandler, 1989, 1990). Phonological and morphological rules interact in sign language as in spoken language. And, as in spoken language, the lexical and postlexical components are distinct (Kiparsky, 1982). In particular, it has been shown that the output of phonological rules cannot undergo derivational morphological rules (Padden & Perlmutter, 1987). One sign language phenomenon with no parallel in the spoken modality is the existence of two anatomically identical potential articulators, the two hands. Despite this potential, and although both hands are involved in the formation of many signs, there is only one primary

active articulator in lexical items (the dominant hand), as there is only one primary active articulator in spoken words (the tongue). Normally, within a word, the nondominant hand either copies what the dominant hand is doing, or it does not articulate at all, serving as a place of articulation like the head or trunk (Stokoe, 1960; Battison, 1978; Sandler, 1993a). Abstracting away from these two roles, the nondominant hand can be seen as essentially subordinate to the dominant hand regardless of its role (Brentari & Goldsmith, 1993). The tenets of the theory of dependency phonology (Anderson and Ewen, 1987) allow direct representation of the particular types of asymmetries that exist between the two hands: the dominant hand is represented as the head and the nondominant hand as the dependent of the articulatory node of structure (van der Hulst, 1996). In addition to autosegmental structure, researchers have also found evidence for suprasegmental structure. A principled distinction can be made between a sign language syllable spanning a single movement (or a simultaneous combination of path movement with handshape change or orientation change) and morphosyntactic units like morphemes or words (Brentari, 1990). Several researchers suggest that movement represents a kind of visual sonority (Brentari, 1990, 1998; Corina, 1990; Perlmutter, 1992; Sandler, 1993b). Arguing further that all dynamic elements form a phonological class, Brentari (1998) proposes a model of sign structure that represents the sonorous elements separately. The distribution of various types of movement has been a key tool for developing sonority scales and models of syllable structure. Perlmutter (1992) uses the distribution of secondary movement to support the claim that there is a syllable peak which usually occurs on the movement element of a sequentially organized syllable similar to the one pictured in Figure 3, in which the locations are compared to consonants and movements to vowels. Other researchers (e.g., Brentari, 1998) see sonority as simultaneously realized over the whole syllable, and in this sense quite different from its spoken language counterpart. Recent work has looked to higher levels of prosody, investigating the phrasal phonology of sign languages. Sign language utterances, like those of spoken language, are rhythmically chunked into units that have regular prominence patterns and that partly correspond to syntactic constituents (Wilbur, 1991, 1994, 1999; Nespor & Sandler, 1999; Sandler & Lillo-Martin, in preparation). It has even been suggested by these researchers that prosodic constituents carry intonational patterns of facial expression. The pictures below show facial intonation, or superarticulation, in Israeli Sign Language, conveying wh-questions (mainly furrowed brows), shared information (mainly squinted eyes), and a simultaneous combination of the two. Although the prosodic domains for intonation appear to be the same as in spoken language (Nespor & Sandler, 1999), such simultaneous componentiality is different from the sequential nature of componential pitch excursions in spoken language intonation (Pierrehumbert & Hirschberg, 1990). 3

4 wh question superarticulation shared information superarticulation wh-question and shared information superarticulation Figure 4. Simultaneous componential intonation (superarticulation) in Israeli Sign Language (Reprinted with permission from Meir & Sandler, in press) Research on sign language phonology leads to two conclusions, which are also important directions for future research in this relatively new field: (1) there are universal properties of phonological organization common to natural language in radically different physical modalities, but (2) there are substantial areas in which the physical production and perception systems mold the phonology of both modalities.

5 BIBLIOGRAPHY Anderson, John & Ewen, Colin. 1987. Principles of dependency phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Battison, Robbin. 1978. Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press Brentari, Diane & Goldsmith, John. 1993. Secondary llicensing and the nondominant hand in ASL phonology. In G. Coulter (ed.), Phonetics and Phonology. Vol 3, Current Issues in ASL Phonology. 19-41. Brentari, Diane. 1990. Theoretical foundations of American Sign Language Phonology. PhD dissertation: University of Chicago. Brentari, Diane. 1998. A prosodic model of sign language phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Clements, G.N. 1985. The geometry of phonological features. Phonology Yearbook 2. 225-252. Corina, David. 1990a. Handshape assimilation in hierarchical phonological representations. In C. Lucas (ed.), Sign language research: Theoretical issues. 27-49. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. Corina, David. 1990b. Reassessing the role of sonority in syllable structure: evidence from a visual-gestural language. Papers from the Chicago Linguistics Society. 26:2. 33-44. Goldsmith, John. 1976. Autosegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT. Distributed by Indiana University Linguistics Club. Humphries, Tom, Carol Padden & Terrence J. O Rourke. 1994. A Basic Course in American Sign Language (Second Edition). Silver Spring, Md: TJ Publishers. Hulst, Harry van der. 1993. Units in the analysis of signs. Phonology 10. 209-242. Hulst, Harry van der. 1996. On the other hand. Lingua 98: 1-3. 121-144. Kiparsky, Paul. 1982. Lexical phonology and morphology. In I.S. Yang (ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm. Seoul: Hanshin. Klima, Edward & Bellugi, Ursula. 1979. The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Liddell, Scott & R. Johnson. 1989. American Sign Language: The phonological base. Sign Language Studies 64. 197-277. Liddell, Scott. 1984a. THINK and BELIEVE: Sequentiality in American Sign Language. Language 60. 372-399. Mandel, Mark. 1981. Phonotactics and morphophonology in American Sign Language. PhD dissertation. Unviersity of California, Berkeley. McCarthy, John. 1979. Formal problems in Semitic phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation: MIT. Meir, Irit & Wendy Sandler. In press. Safa bamerxav: ashnav al sfat hasimaim hayisraelit. Language in Space: A Window on Israeli Sign Language. Haifa: The University of Haifa Press. Nespor, Marina, & Sandler, Wendy. 1999. Prosody in Israeli Sign Language. Language and Speech 42:2&3. 143-176. Padden, Carol & Perlmutter, David. 1987. American Sign Language and the architecture of phonological theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5. 335-375. Perlmutter, David. 1992. Sonority and syllable structure in American Sign Language. Linguistic Inquiry 23. 407-442. Pierrehumbert, Janet & Hirschberg, Julia. 1990. The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. in P. Cohen and M.E. Pollack (eds.), Intentions in communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 271-311. Sandler, Wendy. 1986. The spreading hand autosegment of American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 50. 1-28.

Sandler, Wendy. 1987. Assimilation and feature hierarchy in American Sign Language. papers from the Chicago Linguistics Society; Parasession on Autosegmental Phonology. 23:2. 266-278. Sandler, Wendy. 1989. Phonological representation of the sign. Dordrecht: Foris. Sandler, Wendy. 1990. Temporal aspects and American Sign Langaueg phonology. In S. Fischer and P. Siple (eds.), Theoretical issues in sign language research, 1. 7-35. Sandler, Wendy. 1993a. Hand in hand: the role of the nondominant hand in ASL. The Linguistic Review 10. 337-390. Sandler, Wendy. 1993b. A sonority cycle in American Sign Language. Phonology 10:2. 243-279. Sandler, Wendy. 1999. The medium and the message: Prosodic interpretation of linguistic content in Israeli Sign Language. Sign Language Linguistics 2:2. 187-215. Sandler, Wendy & Lillo-Martin, Diane. In preparation. Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stokoe, William. 1960. Sign language stucture: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. (Studies in Linguistics, Occasional papers, 8.) Buffalo: Department of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo. 2d ed., Silver Spring, Md: Linstok Press, 1978. Wilbur, Ronnie. 1991. Intonation and focus in American Sign Language. In Y. No & M. Libucha (eds.), ESCOL 90: Proceedings of the Seventh Eastern States Conference on Linguistics. 320-331. Wilbur, Ronnie. 1994. Foregrounding structures in ASL. Journal of Pragmatics 22. 647-672. Wilbur, Ronnie. 1999. Stress in ASL. Language and Speech 42:2&3. 229-250. 6