The sequencing of verbal-complement structures

Similar documents
EAP Grammar Competencies Levels 1 6

A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English. with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs

Syntactic and Semantic Differences between Nominal Relative Clauses and Dependent wh-interrogative Clauses

UNIVERSITY OF PUERTO RICO RIO PIEDRAS CAMPUS COLLEGE OF HUMANITIES DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH

GMAT.cz GMAT.cz KET (Key English Test) Preparating Course Syllabus

Correlation: ELLIS. English language Learning and Instruction System. and the TOEFL. Test Of English as a Foreign Language

COURSE SYLLABUS ESU 561 ASPECTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE. Fall 2014

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DELL AQUILA CENTRO LINGUISTICO DI ATENEO

The structure of the English Sentence

Using a Dictionary for Help with GERUNDS and INFINITIVES

stress, intonation and pauses and pronounce English sounds correctly. (b) To speak accurately to the listener(s) about one s thoughts and feelings,

9 The Difficulties Of Secondary Students In Written English

COURSE OBJECTIVES SPAN 100/101 ELEMENTARY SPANISH LISTENING. SPEAKING/FUNCTIONAl KNOWLEDGE

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

A Report on my Foreign Language Learning Experience BA English Language and Communication

EiM Syllabus. If you have any questions, please feel free to talk to your teacher or the Academic Manager.

The Passive Voice. Forms and Functions. Noelia Malla García. Complutense University of Madrid Spain

LINKING WORDS AND PHRASES

TIME AND TENSE: A CRITICAL REVIEW OF LITERATURE

Online Tutoring System For Essay Writing

Sentence Blocks. Sentence Focus Activity. Contents

Semantic Features of Verbs and Types of Present Perfect in English

Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006

Running head: MODALS IN ENGLISH LEARNERS WRITING 1. Epistemic and Root Modals in Chinese Students English Argumentative Writings.

Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) Certificate Programs

L2 Tense and Time Reference

CORRECTING AND GIVING FEEDBACK TO WRITING

LESSON THIRTEEN STRUCTURAL AMBIGUITY. Structural ambiguity is also referred to as syntactic ambiguity or grammatical ambiguity.

SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING ERRORS THEIR TYPES, CAUSES, AND TREATMENT

English Grammar Passive Voice and Other Items

ESL 005 Advanced Grammar and Paragraph Writing

A discourse approach to teaching modal verbs of deduction. Michael Howard, London Metropolitan University. Background

SYNTAX: THE ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE STRUCTURE

Structure of Clauses. March 9, 2004

Grammarin Use. Practice makes perfect. /inuse. Includes sample units from both levels

BOOK REVIEW. John H. Dobson, Learn New Testament Greek (Grand Rapids: Baker Academic, 3rd edn, 2005). xiii pp. Hbk. US$34.99.

Course Syllabus My TOEFL ibt Preparation Course Online sessions: M, W, F 15:00-16:30 PST

Year 1 reading expectations (New Curriculum) Year 1 writing expectations (New Curriculum)

Rethinking the relationship between transitive and intransitive verbs

How Can Teachers Teach Listening?

Teaching Vocabulary to Young Learners (Linse, 2005, pp )

Basic English Grammar Module Unit 1A: Grammatical Units

Three Ways to Clarify Your Writing

GRAMMAR, SYNTAX, AND ENGLISH LANGUAGE LEARNERS

GESE Initial steps. Guide for teachers, Grades 1 3. GESE Grade 1 Introduction

Lexico-Semantic Relations Errors in Senior Secondary School Students Writing ROTIMI TAIWO Obafemi Awolowo University, Ile-Ife, Nigeria

Nouns may show possession or ownership. Use an apostrophe with a noun to show something belongs to someone or to something.

Discourse Markers in English Writing

English auxiliary verbs

CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE. CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4. CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7. CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14

Albert Pye and Ravensmere Schools Grammar Curriculum

English Descriptive Grammar

Finding and Applying for Teaching Jobs

A Beautiful Four Days in Berlin Takafumi Maekawa (Ryukoku University)

ONLINE ENGLISH LANGUAGE RESOURCES

THE UNIVERSITY OF BIRMINGHAM. English Language & Applied Linguistics SECOND TERM ESSAY

UNIT ONE A WORLD OF WONDERS

PS I TAM-TAM Aspect [20/11/09] 1

English Discoveries Online Alignment with Common European Framework of Reference

WHY DO TEACHERS OF ENGLISH HAVE TO KNOW GRAMMAR?

Differences in linguistic and discourse features of narrative writing performance. Dr. Bilal Genç 1 Dr. Kağan Büyükkarcı 2 Ali Göksu 3

Index. 344 Grammar and Language Workbook, Grade 8

English Language (first language, first year)

Language Study Booklet

An Overview of Applied Linguistics

Adjective, Adverb, Noun Clauses. Gerund,Participial and Infinitive Phrases. English Department

English. Universidad Virtual. Curso de sensibilización a la PAEP (Prueba de Admisión a Estudios de Posgrado) Parts of Speech. Nouns.

Advanced Grammar in Use

Grammar learning and teaching: Time, tense and verb

WRITING PROOFS. Christopher Heil Georgia Institute of Technology

ENGLISH FILE Pre-intermediate

Keywords academic writing phraseology dissertations online support international students

Mixed Sentence Structure Problem: Double Verb Error

Handbook on Test Development: Helpful Tips for Creating Reliable and Valid Classroom Tests. Allan S. Cohen. and. James A. Wollack

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation

TEXT LINGUISTICS: RELEVANT LINGUISTICS? WAM Carstens School of Languages and Arts, Potchefstroom University for CHE

CHANCE ENCOUNTERS. Making Sense of Hypothesis Tests. Howard Fincher. Learning Development Tutor. Upgrade Study Advice Service

Assessing Writing Performance Level B1

Transportation: Week 2 of 2

Oxford Dictionary of Current Idiomatic English

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, HAYWARD DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH Assessment of Master s Programs in English

MA in English language teaching Pázmány Péter Catholic University *** List of courses and course descriptions ***

A Writer s Reference, Seventh Edition Diana Hacker Nancy Sommers

Compound Sentences and Coordination

According to the Argentine writer Jorge Luis Borges, in the Celestial Emporium of Benevolent Knowledge, animals are divided

1 Basic concepts. 1.1 What is morphology?

MARY. V NP NP Subject Formation WANT BILL S

Teacher training worksheets- Classroom language Pictionary miming definitions game Worksheet 1- General school vocab version

ANGOL CSOPORTOS TANFOLYAMOK TEMATIKA

Speaking for IELTS. About Speaking for IELTS. Vocabulary. Grammar. Pronunciation. Exam technique. English for Exams.

Business School Writing a Report

Elementary (A1) Group Course

Accelerated Professional Program (APP) Absolute Beginner Varies (Typically 50+ units)

English Grammar in Use A reference

Study Guide for the Library Media Specialist Test Revised 2009

and the Common European Framework of Reference

BBC LEARNING ENGLISH 6 Minute Grammar Past perfect continuous

School of Humanities. Diploma in English Language Teaching DELT Syllabus Course Duration: 1year

An Analysis of the Eleventh Grade Students Monitor Use in Speaking Performance based on Krashen s (1982) Monitor Hypothesis at SMAN 4 Jember

Transcription:

point and counterpoint The sequencing of verbal-complement structures Walter Petrovitz In this section we present contrasting views on a topic of current interest. The first article is one that has been reviewed by the editorial panel and acccepted for publication; the second is a commissioned response, to which the author of the original article is invited to make a brief reply. Reactions from readers are particularly sought, either in the form of a letter to the Editor, or as a brief article (no more than 1,250 words), which will be considered for publication in the normal way. Gerunds and infinitives are among the most diªcult topics to teach, and a continuing source of errors even among advanced learners. Treated as merely structural variants, these forms are usually grouped into a single grammar unit filled with di ering syntactic specifications and long lists of verbs grouped according to their complement type. Significant grammatical distinctions between gerunds and infinitives, as well as pedagogical considerations, suggest that they should be separated and taught at di erent points in a grammar syllabus. This article presents a concise review of the linguistic evidence concerning important di erences between gerunds and infinitives, and makes recommendations on the sequencing of these topics within a course of instruction. Introduction There are a number of reasons for the diªculties students encounter with regard to gerunds and infinitives. First, these forms seem to contradict what they have learnt about English verb morphology. The formal presentation of gerunds and infinitives is normally begun once students have some degree of familiarity with the comparatively more principled system of finite verbs. Suddenly their attention is drawn to participles without auxiliaries, and to verb forms inflected neither for the person and number of their subjects nor consistently for the temporal context. Second, infinitive and gerund constructions possess, to varying degrees, some of the characteristics of clauses, making their relationship to the larger sentence sometimes diªcult to comprehend. Third, the choice of either gerund or infinitive is presented as a largely arbitrary matter. 172 ELT Journal Volume 55/2 April 2001 Oxford University Press

The common practice in ELT is to introduce infinitives and gerunds at the same time in a single unit of instruction, as reflected in most grammar texts (see, for example, Azar 1989, Eastwood 1994, Murphy 1994). Although certain general properties of these forms will be discussed, the present study will focus on the heart of the diªculty with this topic, and the most frequent source of student error: the use of the simple gerund (i.e. lacking an expressed independent subject) and infinitive complements of verbs. The claim will be made here that gerunds and infinitives di er significantly enough to deserve distinct pedagogical treatment. Certain similarities between modals and infinitive-complement verbs will be considered, and an alternative sequence of presentation suggested. I ve got a little list! Di erences between gerunds and infinitives Productivity In more ways than one, an instructor teaching gerunds and infinitives may feel like Gilbert and Sullivan s Lord High Executioner, and students justifiably yearn to be set from scholastic trammels free upon seeing in their grammar texts the disheartening pages of verbs, all organized on the basis of whether they are followed by a gerund or an infinitive. Memorization, even if possible, would be of little value for spontaneous language use, and these enumerations are not entirely satisfactory even as reference material, since, despite its length, the list of gerundcomplement verbs is never really complete. It is doubtful, for example, that any summary of such verbs has ever contained any of the main verbs found in 1: 1 a The coach criticized drinking beer before the game. b The law encourages conserving natural resources. c We can t defend building such a monstrosity. The demands made by such a large amount of material would be bad enough, but defensible if the forms in question represented a uniform grammatical phenomenon. There is ample reason to believe, however, that this is not the case. Although they are usually grouped together in textbooks because of the grammatical functions they purportedly share, infinitives and gerunds di er considerably even here. For example, infinitives are relatively rare in subject position and cannot serve as the objects of prepositions, while gerunds are commonly found in these syntactic environments. (For a detailed discussion of these and other distinctions, see Emonds 1972, Quirk et al. 1972, and Horn 1975.) More important, however, than the syntactic di erences, at least with regard to the sequencing of these structures within the syllabus, are the lexical and semantic distinctions, which will be discussed in the following sections. As illustrated in 1, gerund complements can easily be found with verbs that are not on any of the standard lists, which are deceiving in the way they are presented. While it would appear from grammar texts that verbs taking gerund complements are approximately equal in number to those taking infinitive complements, the former are far more numerous, and have never been exhaustively tallied. Even the lengthy, and supposedly comprehensive, gerund-complement verb list in Householder (1964) is The sequencing of verbal-complement structures 173

incomplete. The reason for this is that gerunds possess the distributional properties of noun phrases, and may therefore be used with any semantically compatible transitive verbs, except those which are reserved exclusively for the infinitive. In contrast, the infinitive-complement structure is no longer productive in English because it is associated with a fixed set of verbs to which none may be added. Thus, aside from being a dull and time-consuming task, marching students through various lists of verbs may cause them to form incorrect hypotheses about the grammar of English: either that once the lists are mastered, every occurrence of infinitive and gerund complements will be accounted for, or that for every verb they encounter they will have to learn the complement type. Clearly, a more reasonable approach would be to adopt the same strategy followed with other non-productive forms, such as irregular verbs, namely to focus attention on them, and simply indicate that the productive forms are to be used elsewhere. Aspect The analysis presented in Bolinger (1968) broadly associates gerunds and infinitives with a perfect, and a hypothetical or future aspect respectively (Bolinger uses the terms reification vs. hypothesis or potentiality). This di erence is most tangible with those verbs that may take either complement: 2 a Bill should remember closing the window. b Bill should remember to close the window. Of course, the aspect is relative to the tense of the main verb: whenever the remembering occurs, the closing is anterior in the case of the gerund, and posterior in the case of the infinitive. While not every verb taking either complement evidences such a clear-cut distinction (e.g. start and try), it is never the case that the anterior meaning is associated with the simple infinitive. See Bolinger for a more complete discussion. Although gerunds and infinitives both have perfective forms, Celce- Murcia and Larsen-Freeman (1983) note that these forms are not semantically equivalent. The perfective forms of infinitives are consistently distinct in aspect from their non-perfective counterparts, as illustrated in 3: 3 a *He claims to work for a bank before taking his present job. b He claims to have worked for a bank before taking his present job. There is, however, no consistent corresponding di erence between simple and perfective forms of gerunds, as shown by the synonymy of 4a and 4b: 4 a He doesn t remember working for a bank before taking his present job. b He doesn t remember having worked for a bank before taking his present job. Here again, presenting the perfective forms of gerunds and infinitives together gives the impression that these forms are equivalent. Placing infinitives earlier in the syllabus, on the other hand, and more closely following the presentation of perfective finite verbs, would gain from the advantage of having the basic concepts fresh in the students minds. 174 Walter Petrovitz

Postponing gerunds to a place in the syllabus closer to dependent finite clauses would also make greater sense, since here as well, aspectual distinctions are sometimes erased, as shown in the synonymy of 5a and 5b: 5 a We should wait until the police arrive. b We should wait until the police have arrived. Understood subjects Another area in which infinitives di er from gerunds involves the understood subjects of these verbs. Horn (1975) noted that the unexpressed subjects of gerund complements need not be same as the subject of the sentence. Thus, in 6a, the subject of the gerund is interpreted to be the same as the subject of the main verb, but in 6b there is no such identity of reference: 6 a Robin tried soliciting money from the students. b Robin denounced soliciting money from the students. In contrast, the understood subject of a bare infinitive complement must be the same as that of the main verb: 7 Robin tried/decided/threatened to solicit money from the students. Once again, placing gerunds and infinitives within the same unit implies a semantic parallelism that is simply not there. The comparison with We see, then, that there are essential di erences between gerunds and modal auxiliaries infinitives in terms both of their syntactic distribution and their semantic interpretation. This should be suªcient for them to be treated as independent topics in the grammar syllabus. In addition, despite obvious grammatical di erences (involving inflection, question formation, and negation), infinitive-complement verbs are much more similar to modal auxiliaries with regard to the distinctions discussed above than they are to gerund-complement verbs. These similarities have implications both for the sequencing of gerunds and infinitives within the grammar syllabus, and the methods used to teach these structures. Productivity Like modals, infinitive-complement verbs belong to a closed, although admittedly larger, lexical class. Since they comprise a fixed set, they could be grouped semantically, much the way modals are often presented. Verbs with similar meanings, such as attempt, endeavour, try, and undertake, would thus be practiced together, rather than merely appearing scattered throughout a list. Low-frequency verbs which would be unfamiliar to most students could be introduced with other semantically similar verbs, as an aid to remembering both their meaning and their structural properties. Thus, verbs such as long and yearn could be presented together with want and desire. Modals are similar to some other non-productive lexical classes (such as irregular verbs) in that they are very high-frequency, and thus usually introduced early, while infinitives and gerunds are placed in a later part of the syllabus. However, research on the frequency of these structures reported in Celce-Murcia and Larsen-Freeman (1983), shows a much higher occurrence of infinitive complements than of gerund complements. This would also suggest that the teaching of infinitive The sequencing of verbal-complement structures 175

complements should be placed at a much earlier position in the grammar syllabus, closely following the presentation of modals (an approach taken in Thomson and Martinet 1986). Aspect Understood subjects Hofmann (1966) noted that as with infinitive complements, the perfective forms of the verbs which may follow certain modals mark true aspectual distinctions, as shown in 8: 8 a *She must leave before we got there. b She must have left before we got there. Since the perfective forms in both constructions are similar in both form and meaning, the introduction of infinitive complements closely following modals would benefit from the students familiarity with these forms. Contextualized exercises using stories or pictures can be used to practise the two structures and review the forms involved, as illustrated in 9: 9 a Could John have visited his sister? b No. He claims (to have been sick/working/delayed) The referential properties of the understood subjects are also the same for both modals and verbs that take the bare infinitive: the subject of the modal must be the same as that of the following verb. This allows the progression from constructions with modals to those with infinitivecomplement verbs (perhaps through the intermediate step of periphrastic modals) to proceed under a single conceptual framework regarding the agency of the complement verb. Again, contextualized activities can be used to practice both structures. These can include pronoun-reference exercises (an often neglected topic in grammar texts, although interestingly a more prominent feature of the new computerized TOEFL), which would be especially useful when passive complements are being practiced. This leaves the question of overt subjects of infinitive complements. Up to this point, we have been considering only subjectless forms, but there are, of course, infinitives with independent subjects, such as those in 10: 10 a We urged her to take the job. b I warned you not to go. Although this may seem to be in contradiction to the claims made concerning the examples in 7, there are good reasons to regard the structures in 10 as suªciently di erent in nature to relegate them to a separate portion of the syllabus. First, there is only a small overlap between verbs taking simple infinite complements, and those taking infinitive complements with independent subjects. Presenting them together simply invites confusion. Second, in the latter category the full range of thematic roles are realized within the constituent. They are thus much more like noun clauses, and could be introduced at a point in the syllabus closer to this topic. Substitution exercises, as shown in 11, can be used to give students a better feel for the full range of clausal complements. 176 Walter Petrovitz

11 a We told her to take the job. b We said that (she should take the job) Conclusion The presentation of grammar in ELT can be greatly enriched by the findings of syntactic research. Such evidence should especially be taken into account with regard to areas which have traditionally been problematic. While certain grammatical topics present inherent and unavoidable diªculties, problems may also be caused by a misapprehension of the nature of the structures involved. This has been the case with the teaching of gerunds and infinitives, with a large amount of disparate material forced into a single teaching unit. A principled redistribution would allow for a more natural and comprehensible presentation. Received January 2000 References Azar, B. 1989. Understanding and Using English Grammar. (2nd edn.). Englewood Cli s, New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Bolinger, D. 1968. Entailment and the meaning of structures. Glossa 2/2: 119 27. Celce-Murcia, M. and D. Larsen-Freeman. 1983. The Grammar Book: An ESL/EFL Teacher s Course. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury. Eastwood, J. 1994. Oxford Guide to English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Emonds, J. 1972. A reformulation of certain syntactic transformations in S. Peters (ed.). Hofmann, T. 1966. Past tense replacement and the English modal system. Harvard Computational Laboratory, NSF Report 17. Horn, G. 1975. On the nonsentential nature of the POSS-ING construction. Linguistic Analysis 1: 333 87. Householder, F. 1964. Some Classes of Verbs in English. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club. Murphy, R. 1994. English Grammar in Use (2nd edn.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Peters, S. (ed.). 1972. Goals of Linguistic Theory. Englewood Cli s, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik. 1972. A Grammar of Contemporary English. London: Longman. Thomson, A. and A. Martinet. 1986. A Practical English Grammar. (4th edn.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. The author Walter Petrovitz is Associate Professor ESL at St. John s University in New York City. He completed his doctoral work in linguistics at the City University of New York. His publications include work in both theoretical linguistics and secondlanguage pedagogy. His current research interests focus on the ways in which semantics and discourse analysis can be used in the teaching of grammar. Email: wp@stjohns.edu The sequencing of verbal-complement structures 177