MORPHOLOGICAL ANALYSIS AS A STEP IN AUTOMATED SYNTACTIC ANALYSIS OF A TEXT

Similar documents
Morphology. Morphology is the study of word formation, of the structure of words. 1. some words can be divided into parts which still have meaning

Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

Language at work To be Possessives

Index. 344 Grammar and Language Workbook, Grade 8

Pupil SPAG Card 1. Terminology for pupils. I Can Date Word

How the Computer Translates. Svetlana Sokolova President and CEO of PROMT, PhD.

GMAT.cz GMAT.cz KET (Key English Test) Preparating Course Syllabus

LESSON THIRTEEN STRUCTURAL AMBIGUITY. Structural ambiguity is also referred to as syntactic ambiguity or grammatical ambiguity.

Efficient Techniques for Improved Data Classification and POS Tagging by Monitoring Extraction, Pruning and Updating of Unknown Foreign Words

Syntactic and Semantic Differences between Nominal Relative Clauses and Dependent wh-interrogative Clauses

Livingston Public Schools Scope and Sequence K 6 Grammar and Mechanics

stress, intonation and pauses and pronounce English sounds correctly. (b) To speak accurately to the listener(s) about one s thoughts and feelings,

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006

Albert Pye and Ravensmere Schools Grammar Curriculum

Parsing Technology and its role in Legacy Modernization. A Metaware White Paper

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation

SPANISH Kindergarten

Rethinking the relationship between transitive and intransitive verbs

COMPUTATIONAL DATA ANALYSIS FOR SYNTAX

Adjectives and Adverbs. Lecture 9

Nouns are naming words - they are used to name a person, place or thing.

Problems with the current speling.org system

SYNTAX: THE ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE STRUCTURE

TERMS. Parts of Speech

Linguistic Universals

Writing Common Core KEY WORDS

Resources: Encore Tricolore 1 (set textbook), Linguascope, Boardworks, languagesonline, differentiated resources devised by MFL staff.

Electronic offprint from. baltic linguistics. Vol. 3, 2012

Syntax: Phrases. 1. The phrase

Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent

Paraphrasing controlled English texts

Grade 4 Writing Assessment. Eligible Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills

Little Pocket Sorts : Irregular Past-Tense Verbs

Glossary of literacy terms

CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE. CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4. CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7. CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14

PREP-009 COURSE SYLLABUS FOR WRITTEN COMMUNICATIONS

About Middle English Grammar

One Ring to rule them all, One Ring to find them One Ring to bring them all, and in the darkness bind them In the Land of Mordor where shadows lie

Morphemes, roots and affixes. 28 October 2011

Sample only Oxford University Press ANZ

Parts of Speech. Skills Team, University of Hull

COURSE OBJECTIVES SPAN 100/101 ELEMENTARY SPANISH LISTENING. SPEAKING/FUNCTIONAl KNOWLEDGE

English-Japanese machine translation

Lerninhalte ALFONS Lernwelt Englisch 5. Klasse

1 Basic concepts. 1.1 What is morphology?

English auxiliary verbs

Correlation: ELLIS. English language Learning and Instruction System. and the TOEFL. Test Of English as a Foreign Language

BOOK REVIEW. John H. Dobson, Learn New Testament Greek (Grand Rapids: Baker Academic, 3rd edn, 2005). xiii pp. Hbk. US$34.99.

According to the Argentine writer Jorge Luis Borges, in the Celestial Emporium of Benevolent Knowledge, animals are divided

This image cannot currently be displayed. Course Catalog. Language Arts Glynlyon, Inc.

English. Universidad Virtual. Curso de sensibilización a la PAEP (Prueba de Admisión a Estudios de Posgrado) Parts of Speech. Nouns.

EAP Grammar Competencies Levels 1 6

Curriculum Catalog

EAST PENNSBORO AREA COURSE: LFS 416 SCHOOL DISTRICT

A Beginner s Guide To English Grammar

The Ambiguity Review Process. Richard Bender Bender RBT Inc. 17 Cardinale Lane Queensbury, NY

April Online Payday Loan Payments

Year 1 reading expectations (New Curriculum) Year 1 writing expectations (New Curriculum)

Focus: Reading Unit of Study: Research & Media Literary; Informational Text; Biographies and Autobiographies

English Language Proficiency Standards: At A Glance February 19, 2014

Third Grade Language Arts Learning Targets - Common Core

English Grammar in Use A reference

A Framework for Syntactic Translation V. H. Yngve, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts

Level 1 Teacher s Manual

Current Situations and Issues of Occupational Classification Commonly. Used by Private and Public Sectors. Summary

SEPTEMBER Unit 1 Page Learning Goals 1 Short a 2 b 3-5 blends 6-7 c as in cat 8-11 t p

Written Language Curriculum Planning Manual 3LIT3390

Intensive Language Study: Beginning Modern Standard Arabic ARAB (6 credits, 90 class hours)

Cambridge Primary English as a Second Language Curriculum Framework

Three Ways to Clarify Your Writing

Word Completion and Prediction in Hebrew

ENGLISH LANGUAGE - SCHEMES OF WORK. For Children Aged 8 to 12

THERE ARE SEVERAL KINDS OF PRONOUNS:

INTERNATIONAL COMPARISONS OF PART-TIME WORK

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DELL AQUILA CENTRO LINGUISTICO DI ATENEO

Association Between Variables

ONLINE ENGLISH LANGUAGE RESOURCES

Hungarian teachers perceptions of dyslexic language learners

Towards a RB-SMT Hybrid System for Translating Patent Claims Results and Perspectives

The Dictionary of the Common Modern Greek Language is being compiled 1 under

National Quali cations SPECIMEN ONLY

Strand: Reading Literature Topics Standard I can statements Vocabulary Key Ideas and Details

Chapter 26: The Regular Comparison of Adjectives

Intending, Intention, Intent, Intentional Action, and Acting Intentionally: Comments on Knobe and Burra

1. segment spoken words into sounds/syllables. M R R R 2. count sounds in spoken words. M R R R 3. blend spoken sounds into words.

French 2A. Course Overview

Writing a degree project at Lund University student perspectives

Section 8 Foreign Languages. Article 1 OVERALL OBJECTIVE

GCSE Speaking Support Meetings. GCSE Polish Speaking. Introduction 2. Guidance 3. Assessment Criteria 4-5. Student 1 - Speaking Commentary 6-7

Level 4 Teacher s Manual

Editing and Proofreading. University Learning Centre Writing Help Ron Cooley, Professor of English

MODERN WRITTEN ARABIC. Volume I. Hosted for free on livelingua.com

Lecture 5. Verbs and Verb Phrases I

Guide to Parsing. Guide to Parsing

Lausanne Procedure of Speech- and Text Analysis at BAMF Office/Germany (BAMF = Federal Office for Migration and Refugees)

Testing Data-Driven Learning Algorithms for PoS Tagging of Icelandic

openmind 1 Practice Online

Modern Greek. Specification. Pearson Edexcel International GCSE in Modern Greek (4MG0) Issue 3. First examination 2011

Curriculum Catalog

Transcription:

GUSTAV LEUNBACH MORPHOLOGICAL ANALYSIS AS A STEP IN AUTOMATED SYNTACTIC ANALYSIS OF A TEXT Introduction. The general purpose of this study is to investigate the possibility of an almost completely automated syntactic analysis of a given text of a known language. This has in itself some theoretical linguistic interest, and to the extent that it succeeds, it will save a large amount of labour in relation to automated indexing and translation, etc.. One step in this analysis is to count the occurrences of all wordforms of the text, not for statistical purposes, but for the purpose of selecting a list of highly frequent word-forms for which the labour of adding syntactic information before the automated analysis is most fruitful. This list contains some substance words for which the syntactic information mainly consists of assigning a word class; this helps the automated analysis just because the words are frequent; they appear in many periods of the text. But mostly the list contains structural words (prepositions, pronouns, adverbs, auxiliary verbs, etc.); without information on the syntactic function of these words it is usually impossible to analyse a text at all. Another step in the investigation - the one with which this paper deals - is the automated utilization of the morphological information contained in the text: a search of the implicit information on word class and syntactic function which is given by the knowledge that two or more word-forms are flexions of a common stem. The concept of a lemma. As indicated above, a lemma is defined as a group of two or more word-forms of the text which morphologically may be flexed forms of a common word stem within one of the flexion paradigms included

270 GUSTAV LE't,INBACH in the analysis (most so-called irregular paradigms being excluded as too difficult to automate). The language in question is Danish, and a list of 25 paradigms and paradigm variants for that language is given as Table 1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 E ES - S ET ETS ENE ENES E MS - S EN ENS MNE ENES E MS Z - Z S ET ETS ENE ENES E ES Z - Z S EN ENS ENE ENES E ES X - X S ET ETS ENE ENES E ES X - X S EN ENS ENE ENES ER. ER`S - S ET ETS ER.NE ER.NMS ER. ER.S - S EN ENS MR.NM EkNMS ER. ER.S Z - Z S ET MTS ER.NE ER.NES ER. ER.S Z - Z S EN ENS MIKNE ER.NES Elk ER.S X - X S ET ETS MRNE ER.NMS ER ER`S X- X S EN ENS ERNE ER`NES E ES - S ET ETS NE NES E ES - S EN ENS NE NES E ES ER. EKS ET MTS MR.NE ER.NES E ES EIK ER`S EN ENS EI~NE EKNMS EDM EDES - S ET ETS ENDE ENDMS E ES ETE ETES ET ETS ElK E ES EDE EDES ET ETS ENDE ENDMS E ES EDE EDES ET ETS ENDE ENDES M ES T TS TE TES ENDE ENDES E ES X T X TS X TE X TES ENDE ENDES E MS ST EST EKE EKES ESTE ESTES M MS ST EST ERE EKES ESTE ESTES E ES ST EST ERE ER.ES ESTE ESTES R. MR. - MR. X- ER. MR. X- T - X T X- Z T Z- STE STES TABLE 1 25 PARADIGMS AND PARADIGM VAR.IANTS OF STANDAR.D DANISH THE FIKST 16 AlKE NOMINAL PARADIGMS, NOS. 17 TO 22 AlKE VERBAL, THE LAST THREE ADJECTIVAL. THE ORDER. OF ENDINGS WITHIN EACH LINE HAS NO SIGNIFICANCE WHATEVER. X Z X AND Z BEFORE ENDINGS INDICATE STEM MODIFICATIONS THE STEM IS FOKMED BY DOUBLING THE LAST LETTER. BEFOR. THE ENDING AN E BEFOR`E THE LAST LETTER. BEFORE THE ENDING (FLUID E) IS DELETED AND A POSSIBLE DOUBLE LETTER BEFORE THIS E IS DISSOLVED. The text has been read into a computer, giving each word-form an index number the ftrst time it occurred and storing in one file the text represented by index numbers and in another the word-forms. Lemmatization was performed on this latter file by reading each word

MORPHOLOGICAL ANALYSIS 271 backwards. If the last letter was one which could form an ending or a part of an ending the preceding letter was also searched, etc.. All cases of a possible stem and ending have been treated as putative stems. To quote the extreme example, a form XXSTES is treated as a putative stem in itself and as derived from the following putative stems: XXSTESS (this is the x-modification of Table 1, the double consonant only appears before endings containing an e); XXSTS (the z-modification); XXST; XXS and XX. The last three all occur in common Danish words; the three first do not, but this only means that another derivation from the same putative stem does not appear unless by a very rare coincidence. The putative stems are stored in a list, and each one is compared with all preceding until it is revealed as either one of them or a new one. It is essential for the practical maintenance of the analysis that the wordforms are sorted at least by flrst letter and the comparison made one group at a time; actually the words were ordered nearly alphabetically, but no general rule could be devised for stopping the search earlier. After the search has been completed, all putative stems which appear only once are discarded. Of the rest, only those are conserved where at least two endings belong to the same paradigm, that is to the same one among the 25 lines of Table 1. Finally, a file of the index numbers of all word-forms is loaded with information on every lemma that each word-form belongs to and with which ending. If the same word-form belongs to more than one lemma, there is a conflict, see later. The text is a short Western novel of about 41,000 words which have been sorted into slightly more than 5,000 word-forms. Of these, about 2,950 have been marked as members of lemmas. However, this does not mean that useful syntactic information can be derived for nearly 60 pct. of the word-forms of the text. Unless the lemma is a pure coincidence (homograph), between two unrelated words apart from some letters which may be flexion endings; this is a problem of the same kind as ordinary homography is for any automated text analysis), it indicates a semantic bond between its members, but the syntactic information depends on the set of endings contained in the lemma. In the following, lemmas will be divided into three main categories, unambiguous, ambiguous and conflicting. So far, a complete analysis has been performed on the 323 lemmas formed by words with initial letters A-G, including 882 out of 1568 word-forms.

272 GUSTAV LEUN'BACH Unambiguous lemmas. A lemma is called unambiguous if the endings of its members determine unambiguously which of the three word classes, noun, adjective and verb, the lemma may belong to. Normally, this also means that the endings can only belong to one of the 25 paradigms of Table 1. Of the 323 lemmas formed with the initials A-G, 162 are classified as unambiguous. Nouns account for 79 lemmas. Most of these contain the ending -en, the definitite article in singular for nouns of the common gender, which does not belong to any adjectival or verbal paradigm; several contain the plural definitite article. One lemma has 6 members; a computer-aided inspection has revealed this to be the word "bandit ", a very appropriate word for this type of text, several of its forms have high frequency. Two lemmas have each 5 members and seven have 4 each, but more than half have only 2 members. With only two forms represented, the risk of a chance homography is of course high. Adjectives account for 14 cases; they nearly all include the comparative ending -ere or the superlative -st which can be extended with -e or -es. A 15th lemma was also by the computer classified as unambiguously adjectival, but it contained only two forms with the endings -ste and -stes, and as the example above of different borders between putative stem and ending shows, either -s- or -st- may belong to the stem. (Inspection showed the word actually to be a verb with a stem ending in -s.) Two of the fourteen lemmas have 4 members each and eight have 3. Verbs: 69 lemmas. 22 of these include the ending -o which in almost all verbal paradigms has the function of imperative which is unlikely to occur so frequently. Instead, the form with zero ending is probably in most cases a noun of the same stem, but not represented in any of those forms which differ from verbal flexion endings, and thus not causing a conflict between two lemmas. Two verbal lemmas have 6 members each (both including the dubious imperative), one has 5 and ten have 4 (six of them including the zero form, the dubious imperative); almost half have only 2 members. If a lemma is by this definition unambiguous, and the set of endings contained in it gives no reason to suspect its validity, the syntactic information on each word-form is in most cases quite obvious. With

MORPHOLOGICAL ANALYSIS 273 nouns and adjectives, the forms ending in -s are of particular interest; they are possessive forms with function of adjectives, but with a peculiar distribution of articles. In verbs the participles may form composite tenses (the present participle almost never) or may function as adjectives with restricted flexion patterns. Ambiguous lemmas. These 115 lemmas have none of the forms which are characteristic of either of the three word classes; all are short, 13 of them have 3 members and the rest only 2. 58 are classified as possibly nouns or verbs, but 37 of these contain the zero ending, mostly together with -et which may be either the definite article of a neuter noun or a past participle, or with -er which may be plural or present tense. As the flexion-less form of a noun is much more frequent than the imperative of a verb, the large majority of these words are certainly nouns. It may be reasonable to classify them together with some of the shorter nominal or verbal lemmas as "almost unambiguous ". Conflicting lemmas. The same word-form may be a member of more than one lemma, either with the same border between stem and ending or with a different one. In each of these cases, each lemma is followed through until all word-forms interconnected directly or indirectly are summed up, and the whole complex is defined as one conflicting lemma. There are 46 conflicting lemmas in the part of the vocabulary investigated, containing from 3 to 7 members, apart from one of 10 members which on inspection proved to be a mixture of demonstrative pronouns. Most of the others can be explained either by a noun and a verb formed from the same root (occasionally also an adjective) or by homography between two unrelated words, each with some flexed forms represented. When both one or more of the shorter endings of a paradigm and one of the longer endings are present, one may with reasonable certainty assess the word class and syntactic function of the longer form, 18

274 GUSTAV LEUNBACH even if the shorter form may belong to more than one paradigm and no inference can be drawn on its word class. Danish compared to other languages. The same analysis performed on a text in English will be somewhat easier in computational work because of the fewer paradigms and fewer forms in each, but I expect the yield to be much less. I would expect a lot of ambiguous lemmas (noun or verb), containing the zero form and the ending -s, some verbal lemmas, containing -ed or -ing or both, and a few adjectival lemmas with comparative or superlative endings. A noun can only be identified by the endings -'s or -s', which I expect to be rare in most texts - and which can be found by more mechanical means also. Many verbal lemmas would be expected to hide a noun of the same root. On the other hand, applied to a language with richer flexions the computational work involved in this analysis might get out of hand, and I am not sure that the yield would in all cases increase comparably. In German I expect many ambiguous lemmas to be found because several endings occur in paradigms of different classes. French has the same problem as English that nouns can hardly be identified by their endings; moreover the boundary between regular and irregular paradigms is rather vague in written French, making it somewhat uncertain how many paradigms it pays to include in the automated analysis. Russian seems to me to offer somewhat better possibilities. There are many verbal paradigms with various stem modifications which it may be impossible to automate, but the forms of the present tense on one hand, and most other forms separately may make it possible to identify a verb by two different stems. The risk of finding ambiguous lemmas is small, though conflicting lemmas may occur. And the specific information on case which many nominal forms contain is often relevant to the syntactis analysis, although it may be difficult to utilize it in an automated manner. S. Hellberg's analysis of Swedish. After the computational work for this paper had been completed my attention was drawn to an article by S. HELLBV.RC: Computerized

MORPHOLOGICAL ANALYSIS 275 Lemmatization without the Use of a Dictionary, in ~ Computers and the Humanities )), VI (1972) 4, pp. 209-212. The following discussion of his method is built mainly on a stencilled report in Swedish mentioned in a footnote to the article. Hellberg does not investigate all possible ways of dividing one word-form into stem and ending, but investigates for two word-forms at a time whether they can be the same stem with two flexion endings within the same paradigm. This procedure depends heavily on a complete alphabetization of all word-forms. In the cases where Swedish has the same stem modifications that my program takes into account, Hellberg has to define the stem as the shortest part of the word which is identical in all flexed forms; this increases the number of different endings, and the definition of a stem comes rather far from the ordinary grammatical concept, while mine corresponds rather closely to it. Hellberg's method may be faster than mine in computation time, but I believe that he runs a greater risk of missing lemmas which are actually present than I; the concept of a conflicting lemma does not appear in his system, instead he discusses the frequency of wrong lemmafizations. Praaical results. Of a vocabulary of slightly above 5,000 word-forms in a novel of about 41,000 words, nearly 60 pct have proved to be members of lemmas, that is sets of two or more word-forms which may be flexions of the same stem within one of the regular paradigms. An inspection of those lemmas which are formed by words with initials A-G shows that of the word-forms contained in them, about 60 Pct can with reasonable certainty be assigned to a definite word class, about half as nouns, somewhat less as verbs, and a few as adjectives. The rest cannot be thus assigned, either because all the endings of the lemma may belong to paradigms of different word classes, or because the same word-form belongs to lemmas of different paradigms, thus indicating the possibility of a homograph. Of the endings pertaining to a given paradigm, some convey a definitite syntactic function while others do not. These figures summarize the yield of the analysis as regards its function as a step in an automated syntactic analysis of the text.

276 GUSTAV LEUNBACH A different summarization of the results of the analysis, which has some general interest, concerns the distribution of lemmas by length. The part of the lemmas investigated in detail show three lemmas of 6 members and three of 5 members as the longest. Which words occur in so many flexions depends of course on the content of the text, but the numerical structure I suppose to be characteristic of a much wider range of self-contained texts; if the investigation is carried out on the vocabularies of different texts, an interesting by-product may be the study of the stability of this structure. One may be interested in a different type of statistical analysis, namely of the frequencies of various grammatical categories, for instance, the frequencies of singular and plural forms of nouns, of present and past tense. For this purpose, however, the analysis performed here is not sufficient, not even combined with the frequency count of all word-forms mentioned in the introduction: firstly, the analysis expressly only recognizes a flexion ending if at least two different endings to the same stem occur; that is, the frequency of e.g. plural forms may be counted only for those nouns which are represented in at least two forms. Secondly, in the Danish nominal paradigms not all forms are with certainty identifiable as either singular or plural, and in the regular verbal paradigms the simple past is always the same form as a participle which is used in an adjectival function, and not in composite tenses. (For the purpose of test parsing, the latter ambiguity is not a fundamental obstacle; whenever such a form is encountered two possibilities must be left open, and one must hope that other words of the period will close one possibility).