Dimensions of Affective Semantic Meaning Behavioral and Evoked Potential Correlates in Chinese Subjects

Similar documents
Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

TECHNICAL SPECIFICATIONS, VALIDATION, AND RESEARCH USE CONTENTS:

Semantic Anomalies in Second Language Processing: An Electrophysiological Study*

Video-Based Eye Tracking

Data Analysis Methods: Net Station 4.1 By Peter Molfese

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data.

Gilles Pourtois Æ Sylvain Delplanque Æ Christoph Michel Æ Patrik Vuilleumier

Ten Simple Rules for Designing and Interpreting ERP Experiments Steven J. Luck University of Iowa

An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks

Overview of Methodology. Human Electrophysiology. Computing and Displaying Difference Waves. Plotting The Averaged ERP

Neural Systems Mediating American Sign Language: Effects of Sensory Experience and Age of Acquisition

Classic EEG (ERPs)/ Advanced EEG. Quentin Noirhomme

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex

Electrophysiology of language

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind

Electrophysiological evidence for a natural/artifactual dissociation

CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS.

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention

NeXus: Event-Related potentials Evoked potentials for Psychophysiology & Neuroscience

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain).

Technique and Safety of. by Pierluigi Castellone, Electronics Engineer Brain Products General Manager

Documentation Wadsworth BCI Dataset (P300 Evoked Potentials) Data Acquired Using BCI2000's P3 Speller Paradigm (

Electrophysiological Activity Generated During the Implicit Association Test: A Study Using

How To Create A Signal Data System

Modelling, Extraction and Description of Intrinsic Cues of High Resolution Satellite Images: Independent Component Analysis based approaches

ERPs in Cognitive Neuroscience

Prospectus for Recording EEG and ERP in Infants

Brain Computer Interfaces (BCI) Communication Training of brain activity

Type-D EEG System for Regular EEG Clinic

Music matters: Preattentive musicality of the human brain

Sentiments. 2.1 Evaluation, Potency, and Activity (EPA)

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Telemetry in monkey neurophysiology Remote monitoring of neuronal brain signals

LEARNING STYLES AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE OF STUDENTS IN ENGLISH AS A SECOND-LANGUAGE CLASS IN IRAN

Vision: Receptors. Modes of Perception. Vision: Summary 9/28/2012. How do we perceive our environment? Sensation and Perception Terminology

A Study of Brainwave Entrainment Based on EEG Brain Dynamics

Electrophysiological correlates of verbal and tonal working memory

Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition

Electrophysiology of normal and pathological language processing

Effect of Background Music and Visual Display on Shopping Website Browsing and Purchasing Process

ZEBRAFISH SYSTEMS SYSTEMS ZEBRAFISH. Fast, automatic analyses

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics

P300 Spelling Device with g.usbamp and Simulink V Copyright 2012 g.tec medical engineering GmbH

Investigation of Brain Potentials in Sleeping Humans Exposed to the Electromagnetic Field of Mobile Phones

ARTICLE IN PRESS Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx xxx

Event-related potentials in humans during spatial navigation

Bayesian probability theory

NATIONAL COMPETENCY SKILL STANDARDS FOR PERFORMING AN ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAM

Don t Forget to Warm-Up Your Brain! Caroline Liddy. Before beginning a race, do you jump right into it and run as fast as you can,

Michel, GF. Right handedness: A consequence of infant supine head orientation preference? Science 1981; 212:

Technical Report. Overview. Revisions in this Edition. Four-Level Assessment Process

Study on Brainwave Responses from Ears: the Event-related Synchronization under the Auditory Stimulus

Artifact Removal Procedure Distorts Multifocal Electroretinogram

Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory

Dr V. J. Brown. Neuroscience (see Biomedical Sciences) History, Philosophy, Social Anthropology, Theological Studies.

History of eye-tracking in psychological research

Electrophysiological studies of prosody

Cortical Visual Impairment An introduction

Topographic ERP Analyses: A Step-by-Step Tutorial Review

How To Find Out How Fast A Car Is Going

User Eye Fatigue Detection via Eye Movement Behavior

N.Galley, R.Schleicher and L.Galley Blink parameter for sleepiness detection 1

Implementation of a 3-Dimensional Game for developing balanced Brainwave

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion. Macaque visual areas

The electrophysiology of introspection q

Have you ever missed a call while moving? : The Optimal Vibration Frequency for Perception in Mobile Environments

CASE REPORT. Electrophysiological Evaluation of Disruption in Semantic Memory Induced by Hypnosis: Two Case Reports

Dennis L. Molfese, Victoria J. Molfese, and Spencer Kelly University of Louisville

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Electrophysiology of the expectancy process: Processing the CNV potential

ERP signatures of subject verb agreement in L2 learning

An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers

Does the frequency of the antecedent noun affect the resolution of pronominal anaphors? An ERP study

The Effects of Colour and Light on Space Perception

Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus

Research on physiological signal processing

THERE is a broad consensus in research with event-related

S.J. Luck ERP Boot Camp All rights reserved

Subjects. Subjects were undergraduates at the University of California, Santa Barbara, with

GAZE STABILIZATION SYSTEMS Vestibular Ocular Reflex (VOR) Purpose of VOR Chief function is to stabilize gaze during locomotion. Acuity declines if

Decoding Information Processing When Attention Fails: An Electrophysiological Approach

Jonathan Robert Folstein, Ph.D Macalester College, St. Paul, Minnesota B.A., Philosophy

DRIVER SLEEPINESS ASSESSED BY ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY DIFFERENT METHODS APPLIED TO ONE SINGLE DATA SET

Andrea Greve, a, Mark C.W. van Rossum, a,1 and David I. Donaldson b,2. Introduction

Infant Behavior and Development

PERSPECTIVE. How Top-Down is Visual Perception?

Bernice E. Rogowitz and Holly E. Rushmeier IBM TJ Watson Research Center, P.O. Box 704, Yorktown Heights, NY USA

ERGONOMIC FIELD ASSESSMENT OF BUCKING BARS DURING RIVETING TASKS

Instructional Design. Educating Today s Students for Tomorrow. Susan Owens MS OTR

C o p yr i g ht 2015, S A S I nstitute Inc. A l l r i g hts r eser v ed. INTRODUCTION TO SAS TEXT MINER

Single trial analysis for linking electrophysiology and hemodynamic response. Christian-G. Bénar INSERM U751, Marseille

Word Frequency and Emotion - A Review

A PANEL STUDY FOR THE INFLUENTIAL FACTORS OF THE ADOPTION OF CUSTOMER RELATIONSHIP MANAGEMENT SYSTEM

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION

Prof Dr Dr Friedemann Pulvermüller Freie Universität Berlin WS 2013/14 Progress in Brain Language Research Wed, 4-6 pm ct, K 23/11

Behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging studies

Building a Simulink model for real-time analysis V Copyright g.tec medical engineering GmbH

The brainwave response of optical illusion stimulus

BRAIN ASYMMETRY. Outline

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Transcription:

Dimensions of Affective Semantic Meaning Behavioral and Evoked Potential Correlates in Chinese Subjects W. Skrandies 1 * and M. J. Chiu² 1 Institute of Physiology, Justus-Liebig University, 35392 Giessen, Germany ² Department of Neurology, National Taiwan University Hospital, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan Keywords: Evoked Potential, Brain Electrical Topography, Language, Semantic Meaning, Semantic Differential * Address for correspondence: Institute of Physiology, Justus-Liebig University, Aulweg 129, 35392 Giessen, Germany Phone: +49 641 99 47 270 Fax: +49 641 99 47 279 e-mail: wolfgang.skrandies@physiologie.med.uni-giessen.de Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 1

Abstract The affective meaning of words can be quantified statistically by the "semantic differential technique". We studied a total of 55 Chinese adults in two experiments: first, 210 nouns were rated by 32 subjects. Factor analysis on these data yielded three independent semantic dimensions. Semantically unique words were used in electrophysiological experiments in another group of 23 healthy right-handed adults. Words of similar physical appearance belonging to different semantic classes were presented visually in random order. The EEG was recorded in 32 channels. Evoked activity was computed for each semantic class. Significant differences in electrical brain activation between semantic word classes were seen as early as 80 ms after stimulus onset confirming related findings in German subjects. These results illustrate similar early neural activation in subject groups of different language and culture. Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 2

It has been shown that the recording of electrical brain activity with high time resolution elicited by linguistic material yields sensitive neuronal indicators of human language processing [10, 17]. In an earlier report from our laboratory we demonstrated that semantic meaning of words influences already very early stages of processing as reflected by changes of evoked potential fields [13]. This is in contrast to studies concerned with recordings of brain activity during language processing that centered on the so-called N400 component which occurs at long latency after the presentation of a word which unexpectedly closes a sentence [6], and relates to the comprehension of sentences or jokes [1, 3]. According to the pioneering work of Osgood et al. [9], also in Hongkong Chinese the affective meaning of words can be quantified by the "semantic differential technique" that results in statistically defined independent dimensions where every word is uniquely located on the three dimensions evaluation (E, "good bad"), potency (P, "strong weak"), and activity (A, "active passive"). Brain potentials elicited during reading of single German words show systematic influences of meaning, and these effects occur as early as 100 ms [13]. In the present study we extend such experiments to Chinese adults. We are concerned with the question of whether the semantic dimensions are similar in German and Chinese subjects, and whether early electrophysiological effects can also be observed with different language material in subjects with a vastly different cultural background. A total of 55 Chinese adults participated in two experiments: first, 210 nouns were rated on twelve adjective scales of opposite descriptors by 32 subjects (mean age 29.3; SD 9.6 years; 27 females, 5 males). All words consisted of two characters and had a comparable frequency of occurrence in the Chinese language [18]. Principal component analysis on the questionnaires extracted three varimax-rotated components which corresponded to the E, P, and A dimensions of Osgood et al. [8, 9]. Component scores of each word were computed, and words that scored highly positive (or negative) on one component (>0.70) but had only low scores on the other two dimensions were selected (i.e., stimuli were clearly associated with only one of the semantic dimensions). This resulted in six semantic word classes which were used subsequently. For electrophysiological experiments, list were prepared that contained a total of 1440 words stemming from the 6 classes in randomized order. Twenty-three different healthy volunteers (mean age 29.8; SD 6.0 years; 13 females, 10 males) with Chinese as native language were studied. All subjects were right-handed [5]. The EEG was recorded in 32 channels simultaneously from an array of electrodes located at the following Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 3

positions of the International 10/20 System: Fp 1, Fp 2, Fp z, F 4, C 4, P 4, O 2, F 8, T 8, TP 8, F 3, C 3, P 3, O 1, F 7, T 7, TP 7, F z, C z, CP z, P z, PO z, FC 2, C 2, CP 2, FC 6, CP 6, FC 1, C 1, CP 1, FC 5, CP 5 (see Figure 1). Linked ears served as recording reference; for analysis all signals were referred to the computed average reference. Data acquisition was preformed with a NK Neurofax EEG 1000 system (bandpass: 0.1 60 Hz; sampling rate: 200 Hz). For computing ERPs, the class of each stimulus presented was coded by a trigger signal. Stimuli were presented according to the randomized lists described above sequentially on a computer monitor for 1 s each. Viewing distance was 1 m, all stimuli were of high contrast (> 95%, 43 lx mean luminosity) and subtended 1.43 x 2.86. Size was similar for all word classes. The subjects' task was to visualize the words, and recall was tested at the end of the experiment by a questionnaire. The performance on this task was routinely checked in order to verify that the subjects had actively observed and processed the stimuli. In addition, all subjects filled in semantic differential questionnaires in order to assure reliability of the stimulus material. The EEG was written continuously to harddisk, and was visually inspected offline. Eye movement and blink artifacts were detected at the three most frontal electrodes (located at Fp 1, Fp 2, and Fp z ) that were not included in the analyses, leaving a mean of 141 segments per stimulus class. Artifact-free EEG following stimulus onset was averaged over 1000 ms according to stimulus class, and was digitally filtered (1 30 Hz). This resulted in six sets of average evoked potentials for each subject. Evoked components were identified topographically at peak times of the Global Field Power curve (GFP, [7, 12]), and we will report on components that occurred early after stimulus onset. At individual component latency, the topography of the field distributions was determined for each subject and experimental condition. Component latencies, field strength, and the topographic features of the evoked distributions were compared between stimulus conditions. The significance of the influence of the factors word class (E, P or A) and polarity (+ or ) was determined by ANOVAs with repeated measurements. Interactions between word class and polarity will be referred to as word dimension. Varimax-rotated principal components computed on the questionnaire data revealed three components which accounted for about 60% of the variance (64.1% in the questionnaire group; 62.5% in the EEG group). These components could be clearly identified as Osgood s factors Evaluation (E), Potency (P), and Activity (A). Factorial similarity exceeded 0.82 for each factor indicating a very high reliability of the dimensions revealed by the semantic differential. Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 2

A sequence of potential fields evoked by words between 70 and 895 ms after the stimulus is illustrated in Figure 1. For Figure 1 Potential fields elicited by Chinese words between 70 and 895 ms after stimulus onset. Maps are shown every 25 ms. Recordings from 29 electrodes as indicated by head scheme. Lines are in steps of 0.6 µv, dark areas are negative with respect to the average reference. Mean data of 23 subjects. illustration we display the grand means computed over all word classes. There occur several components starting around 95 ms as reflected by different topographical distribution patterns that occur sequentially. The strength and topography of the evoked fields changes over time. Steps of information processing are reflected by the occurrence of components that are associated with high field strength and stable topography [7, 12]. This is evident in Figure 1 around 95, 170, or 270 ms latency. All subjects individual components were identified at the peak latency of maximal field strength. We analyzed how latency, field strength, and scalp topography (positive or negative centroid locations in the anterior posterior or in the left right direction) of the components were influenced by the semantic dimensions. In the following we will concentrate on effects at small latency that suggest top-down influences on the processing of incoming meaningful information. We note that the potential distributions were similar in all conditions, and their features were modulated by semantic meaning. The earliest component which was reminiscent of the classical P100 appeared between 80 and 130 ms, and its topographical Figure 2 Effects of semantic meaning on a component occurring between 80 and 130 ms latency. A B Scalp topography as seen from front, back, right and left side of the head. Lines are in steps of 0.6 µv, dark areas are negative with respect to the average reference. Significant effects of word polarity on component latency. distribution is displayed in Figure 2. Word polarity had a significant influence on component latency (mean latency for positive Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 3

words: 108.2 ms; negative words: 105.0 ms; F(1,22) = 4.31, p<0.05) indicating that the characteristics good, strong, and active are followed by larger latencies than bad, weak, and passive (see Figure 2). The strength of the same component (reflected by GFP) showed a tendency to be influenced by word dimension (F(2,44) = 2.81; p<0.070). Three significant effects were seen with another component which occurred between 130 and 195 ms (mean latency 170.4 ms; SD 13.1 ms). Its topography is illustrated in Figure 3A. Field strength was modulated by word dimension (F(2,44) = 5.78; p<0.006) In addition, the locations of the negative centroids in the anterior posterior direction displayed a significant interaction between word class and polarity. (F(2,44)=3.89; p<.03).while negative stimuli were systematically located more over posterior areas from E to P to A, positive words showed an opposite relation (Figure 3C). When topography was compared across hemispheres (i.e., the location of the positive centers of gravity in the left-right direction), a significant influence of word class was observed. P-stimuli elicited scalp activity closest to the midline, and E-stimuli showed Figure 3 Effects of semantic meaning on a component occurring between 130 and 195 ms latency. A B C D Scalp topography as seen from front, back, right and left side of the head. Lines are in steps of 0.6 µv, dark areas are negative with respect to the average reference. Influence of word dimension on global field power. Locations of the negative centroids in the anterior-posterior direction. There is a significant interaction between word class and polarity. The ordinate indicates scalp location in percentages of the nasion-inion distance. Locations of the positive centers of gravity in the left-right direction. There is a significant influence of word class with E stimuli lateralized over the left and A stimuli over the right hemisphere. The ordinate indicates scalp location in percentages of the nasioninion distance, 0 corresponds to the midline. Mean data of 23 subjects. where weak stimuli yielded largest responses (Figure 3B). a tendency towards the left, while A-stimuli were located over the right hemisphere (Figure 3D). Although these differences were Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 4

small, they were consistent over subjects and statistically significant (F(2,44) = 3.56, p<0.037). The analysis of the behavioral questionnaire data resulted in the same semantic dimensions as described earlier [8, 9], and were very similar to those obtained in German subjects [13]. We could confirm that the dimension evaluation (E) explains most of the variance (> 30% in each group). Our electrophysiological data illustrate that internal representations of semantic meaning of verbal stimulus material can be assessed by analysis of evoked potentials as has been reported with single channel recordings [2] or in topographical recordings [13]. There was a significant effect of semantic meaning on brain electric activity occurring as early as 80 ms latency. This points to fast processing in higher-level neural networks which may receive the information in parallel and in turn condition the visual cortex. Thus, semantic meaning classified in higher-level structures might represent top-down processing mechanisms. This early component, however, is topographically different from the classical P100 component [13]. Differences were observed with component latency that corresponds to processing time and response strength indicating amount of synchronous activation of cortical neuronal assemblies. In addition to these effects we also describe topographical differences that indicate that not identical generator populations respond to verbal stimuli stemming from different semantic classes. The findings that (1) brain activity elicited by words is influenced by semantic meaning, and (2) that early processing steps are related to language processing confirms earlier findings on German subjects [13]. Both components presented here are very similar to those reported earlier [13]: the potential distributions display a highly significant correlation of 0.80 at 95 ms and 0.81 at 170 ms latency (p<0.001). Of course, the effects are not identical, but the congruence is quite remarkable indicating that the processing of affective meaning of verbal material is universal: Chinese and German language is vastly different, not only according to linguistic categories but there are also large cultural differences. Thus, the E-P-A dimensions of the semantic differential may be regarded as robust and culture-free. Early semantic effects in the order of about 100 ms may come as a surprise since most electrophysiological experiments on language processing have reported much larger latencies [1, 6, 17]. We note, however, that subjects are able to read and comprehend words very fast: when words are shown with rapid serial visual presentation, processing time drops well below 100 ms [11, discussed in 14]. In addition, components with latencies in the order of 100 ms reflect cortical activation as has been demonstrated in studies on 3D perception that indisputably triggers cortical processing [15]. Further Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 5

evidence stems from intracranial evoked potential recordings in humans [4], and such early brain activity is also sensitive to task relevance [16]. Our results illustrate that significant differences in scalp topography between semantic classes are not restricted to late "cognitive" components, but brain activity in primary visual areas is affected by semantic meaning of verbal stimuli suggesting topdown mechanisms which confirms the results of a similar study on German subjects. Acknowledgments Supported by DAAD D / 9922885 and NSC. We thank Ms Y. R. Lin for assistance with data collection and primary analysis, and Ms C. Seidler for help with preparing the figures. References [1] Brandeis, D., Lehmann, D., Michel, C.M. and Mingrone, W., Mapping event-related brain potential microstates to sentence endings, Brain Topogr., 8 (1995) 145-159. [2] Chapman, R.M., McCrary, J.W., Chapman, J.A. and Martin, J.K., Behavioral and neural analyses of connotative meaning: word classes and rating scales, Brain and Lang., 11 (1980) 319-339. [3] Coulson, S. and Kutas, M., Getting it: human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders, Neurosci. Lett., 316 (2001) 71-74. [4] Ducati, A., Fava, E. and Motti, E.D.F., Neuronal generators of the visual evoked potentials: intracerebral recording in awake humans, Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 71 (1988) 89-99. [5] Hamsher, K.S., Handedness Questionnaire, University of Wiscosin Medical School, Department of Neurology, Milwaukee (1981). [6] Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A., Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity, Science, 207 (1980) 204-206. [7] Lehmann, D. and Skrandies, W., Reference free identification of components of checkerboard evoked multichannel potential fields, Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 48 (1980) 609-621. [8] Osgood, C.E., May, W.H. and Miron, M.S., Cross-cultural Universals of Affective Meaning, University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill., 1975, 486 pp. [9] Osgood, C.E., Suci, G.J. and Tannenbaum, P.H., The Measurement of Meaning, University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill, 1957. [10] Pulvermüller, F., Mohr, B. and Schleichert, H., Semantic or lexicosyntactic factors: what determines word-class specific activity in the human brain?, Neurosci. Lett., 275 (1999) 81-84. [11] Rubin,G.S. and Turano, K., Reading without saccadic eye movements. Vision Res., 32 (1992) 895-902 [12] Skrandies, W., The upper and lower visual field of man: electrophysiological and functional differences. Prog. Sens. Physiol., 8 (1987) 1-93. [13] Skrandies, W., Evoked potential correlates of semantic meaning: a brain mapping study, Cogn. Brain Res., 6 (1998) 173-183. [14] Skrandies, W., Early effects of semantic meaning on electrical brain activity, Behav. Brain Scie., 22 (1999) 301-302. [15] Skrandies,W., The processing of stereoscopic information in human visual cortex: Psychophysical and Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 6

electrophysiological evidence. Clin. EEG, 32 (2001) 152-159. [16] Skrandies, W., Chapman, R.M., McCrary, J.W. and Chapman, J.A., Distribution of latent components related to information processing, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 425 (1984) 271-277. [17] Weyerts, H., Münte, T.F., Smid, H.G.O.M. and Heinze, H., Mental representations of morphologically complex words: an event-related potential study with adult humans, Neurosci. Lett., 206 (1996) 125-128. [18] Huang, C.R., Chen, K.J., Gao, Z.M., Chen, F.Y. and Shen, Z.Z., Chinese Knowledge Information Processing Group Technical Report no. 98-01: Word Frequency Dictionary, Taipei: CKIPG, Academia Sinica, 1998. Neuroscience Letters, 2003, 341, 45-48 7