The Acquisition of the Verbs Ser and Estar in Undergraduate Students. Jacqueline Hilton Egan

Similar documents
Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

HYPOTHESIS TESTING: CONFIDENCE INTERVALS, T-TESTS, ANOVAS, AND REGRESSION

SPANISH Kindergarten

Abstract The purpose of this paper is to present the results of my action research which was conducted in several 7 th /8 th grade language arts

Writing Thesis Defense Papers

HOW ACCURATE ARE THOSE THERMOCOUPLES?

Study Guide for the Special Education: Core Knowledge Tests

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS INTERMEDIATE SPANISH II SPAN 2020

GRAMMAR, SYNTAX, AND ENGLISH LANGUAGE LEARNERS

PROFICIENCY TARGET FOR END OF INSTRUCTION, SPANISH I

Study Guide for the Library Media Specialist Test Revised 2009

6.4 Normal Distribution

GCSE Speaking Support Meetings. GCSE Polish Speaking. Introduction 2. Guidance 3. Assessment Criteria 4-5. Student 1 - Speaking Commentary 6-7

HAVERHILL PUBLIC SCHOOLS Spanish I College Prep Curriculum Map. AR Verbs Present Regular Tense

ELL Considerations for Common Core-Aligned Tasks in English Language Arts

CALCULATIONS & STATISTICS

Q1. The graph below shows how a sinusoidal alternating voltage varies with time when connected across a resistor, R.

Study Guide for the Physical Education: Content and Design Test

Department of Chemistry University of Colorado Denver Outcomes Assessment Plan. Department Overview:

1. I have 4 sides. My opposite sides are equal. I have 4 right angles. Which shape am I?

Faculty Response to Grammar Errors in the Writing of ESL Students. by Lyndall Nairn, Lynchburg College, Lynchburg, VA,

Speech-Language Pathology Study Guide

PRELIMINARY ITEM STATISTICS USING POINT-BISERIAL CORRELATION AND P-VALUES

Bexley City School World Language Program Overview

Study Guide for the Middle School Science Test

Rubrics for AP Histories. + Historical Thinking Skills

Study Guide for the Elementary Education: Content Knowledge Test

Number Factors. Number Factors Number of factors , 2, 4, 8, , , , , 2, 3, 6, 9, , 2, , 19 2

Current California Math Standards Balanced Equations

Association Between Variables

Title: Transforming a traditional lecture-based course to online and hybrid models of learning

PREPARATION MATERIAL FOR THE GRADUATE RECORD EXAMINATION (GRE)

Depth-of-Knowledge Levels for Four Content Areas Norman L. Webb March 28, Reading (based on Wixson, 1999)

How To Proofread

Examiner s report F8 Audit & Assurance December 2014

Study Guide for the English Language, Literature, and Composition: Content Knowledge Test

The Effect of Explicit Feedback on the Use of Language Learning Strategies: The Role of Instruction

Unit 1 Number Sense. In this unit, students will study repeating decimals, percents, fractions, decimals, and proportions.

Handbook on Test Development: Helpful Tips for Creating Reliable and Valid Classroom Tests. Allan S. Cohen. and. James A. Wollack

COURSE OBJECTIVES SPAN 100/101 ELEMENTARY SPANISH LISTENING. SPEAKING/FUNCTIONAl KNOWLEDGE

ESL 005 Advanced Grammar and Paragraph Writing

The Oxford Learner s Dictionary of Academic English

Guidelines for Preparing an Undergraduate Thesis Proposal Department of Agricultural Education and Communication University of Florida

3. Logical Reasoning in Mathematics

Comparison of the Cambridge Exams main suite, IELTS and TOEFL

Study Guide for the Pre-Professional Skills Test: Writing

Division of Arts, Humanities & Wellness Department of World Languages and Cultures. Course Syllabus SPANISH I LAN 113

Study Guide for the Music: Content Knowledge Test

Preparing for the IELTS test with Holmesglen Institute of TAFE

Transitions between Paragraphs

How Can Teachers Teach Listening?

Effects of Different Response Types on Iranian EFL Test Takers Performance

chapter >> Consumer and Producer Surplus Section 3: Consumer Surplus, Producer Surplus, and the Gains from Trade

COURSE SYLLABUS SPANISH IA

Using of the First Language in English classroom as a way of scaffolding for both the students and teachers to learn and teach English

THERE ARE SEVERAL KINDS OF PRONOUNS:

Data Analysis, Statistics, and Probability

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS BEGINNING SPANISH I SPAN Laboratory Hours: 0.0 Date Revised: Summer 10

SPAN 203: Intermediate Spanish I

The sophomore Spanish class is team taught by two Spanish teachers during a one hour block. Each

Student Performance Q&A:

Test Information Guide for Nuclear Security Officer Battery

How to Plan and Guide In Class Peer Review Sessions

Examiner s report F7 Performance Management June 2016

Tracking translation process: The impact of experience and training

Introduction to the Practice of Statistics Fifth Edition Moore, McCabe

Translating between Fractions, Decimals and Percents

Predictability of Vocabulary Size on Learners EFL Proficiency: Taking VST, CET4 and CET6 as Instruments

Spanish 401: Elementary Spanish Spring 2013 Course Outline

Principles of Data-Driven Instruction

ESL RUBRICS. As we learned in the workshop entitled Incorporating Rubrics into Instruction which is part of

Integrating a Factory and Supply Chain Simulator into a Textile Supply Chain Management Curriculum

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness

Youngstown State University Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures Syllabus Summer Session I 2015

SPANISH FOR CRIMINAL JUSTICE INSTRUCTOR: Latrina R. Thompson OFFICE: Paulding Campus

PING-PONG BALLS AND PRIMARY LITERATURE IN THE CLASSROOM: THE INTERSECTION OF STUDENT ENGAGEMENT AND FACULTY DEVELOPMENT

Adlai E. Stevenson High School Course Description

EQUATING TEST SCORES

3.7 USING AN ACTIVE-LEARNING QUIZ SERIES IN AN INTRODUCTORY METEOROLOGY COLLEGE COURSE

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CLASSROOM MANAGEMENT STYLES OF TEACHERS AND THE OCCURRENCE OF DISCIPLINE PROBLEMS IN INTERNATIONAL SCHOOLS BANGKOK, THAILAND

The Darwinian Revolution as Evidence for Thomas Kuhn s Failure to Construct a Paradigm for the Philosophy of Science

Author Gender Identification of English Novels

The European Financial Reporting Advisory Group (EFRAG) and the Autorité des Normes Comptables (ANC) jointly publish on their websites for

Spanish Curriculum - Course Objectives and Descriptions

The. Languages Ladder. Steps to Success. The

International Undergraduates Classroom Experiences

Decomposing Numbers (Operations and Algebraic Thinking)

Get Ready for IELTS Writing. About Get Ready for IELTS Writing. Part 1: Language development. Part 2: Skills development. Part 3: Exam practice

Primary and Secondary Qualities Charles Kaijo

Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials

Parental Occupation Coding

Syllabus. Dr. Calderón connects instructional practice with the Common Core State Standards, and backs up her recommendations with research:

Integrating Reading and Writing for Effective Language Teaching

Cambridge English: First (FCE) Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs)

to Become a Better Reader and Thinker

TERMS. Parts of Speech

Transcription:

The Acquisition of the Verbs Ser and Estar in Undergraduate Students Jacqueline Hilton Egan English has only one verb that means to be, but Spanish has two versions: ser and estar. Ser is used with much greater frequency, and it indicates stable traits and objective descriptions, such as where a person is from or his personality, while estar is used to indicate temporary conditions and subjective opinions. This grossly oversimplified description of the differences between ser and estar might lead one to believe that these verbs are relatively easy to acquire in people studying Spanish as a second language. Indeed, the use of these verbs is often covered in an introductory Spanish course. However, proficient use of these verbs requires more than an introductory textbook s explanation. As teachers of Spanish are frustrated to discover, errors in the use of these verbs persist even in students at the advanced levels of Spanish who have recently focused on these two verbs. One question should come to mind at this point: can the process of acquiring these two verbs be accelerated by changing standard pedagogical approaches, or is it simply a natural acquisition process that must run its course in its own time? This study does not explore whether or how an instructor s teaching method can effect a more rapid acquisition of ser and estar. Rather, we examine the written use of these two verbs (and in particular, the accuracy of this use) in college students studying Spanish, the vast majority of whom are native speakers of English. When a group of Spanish professors (Elizabeth A. Martínez- Gibson, Silvia Rodríguez and Joseph R. Weyers) conceived of a study to design a more effective teaching method for ser and estar, they realized Chrestomathy: Annual Review of Undergraduate Research at the College of Charleston Volume 3, 2004: pp. 63-71 2004 by the College of Charleston, Charleston SC 29424, USA. All rights to be retained by the author. 63

64 Chrestomathy: Volume 3, 2004 that first they needed to assess the proficiency exhibited by students in the use of these verbs. To do this, they designed a 50-item objective test that forced students to choose between ser and estar, and a composition that was intended to compel students to use these two verbs. The first part consists of two paragraphs in which students must select whether ser or estar is appropriate for a given phrase. Both ser and estar are in the correct tense and person to eliminate the possibility that trying to conjugate the verb might distract students. The first paragraph is a woman s description of her husband, while the second is a woman s letter about her trip to Spain. In content both are comparable to the type of material beginning and intermediate students are exposed to during class and in their reading: ordinary descriptions of people and places using everyday vocabulary. The third part is a series of 10 statements in English. The participant is asked to select whether he would use ser or estar if he translated these statements into Spanish. Having this section in English provides a control for vocabulary. Otherwise, it is possible that a low scorer understood the concept but was distracted by having a poor understanding of the vocabulary. All of these statements are very straightforward except #29, All of the sunflowers are so big this year! While introductory level students are drilled to know that descriptions of size, color, etc. are always to use ser, the more advanced student might be tempted to use estar, since this is a more subjective description. (The sunflowers look big this year compared to the sunflowers of the previous years.) In the fourth section are ten descriptions in Spanish of people followed by a description that makes sense given the first statement. For example, el profesor tiene unos veinticinco años (the professor is about 25 years old) is followed by Es joven (he is young). The statement that follows the first is given in two versions: one with ser and one with estar. This is designed to assess skill that may not be seen by simply filling in the blank with ser or estar. Lastly, section five has students choose the correct adjective based on a description that contains the verb ser or estar. (The adjective is always the correct gender so as not to give away the correct answer.) For example, Pedro va a muchas fiestas y le gusta hablar con todo el mundo.

Egan: Acquisition of Ser and Estar in Undergraduates 65 Él es muy (Pedro goes to many parties and likes to talk to everyone. He is very ) The choices are hablador (talkative), borracho (drunk/drunkard), estudiante (student), and abogado (lawyer). The correct answer is hablador, because to say that Pedro is borracho with the verb ser means that he is a drunkard. With the verb estar, it means that he is drunk right now but is not necessarily a habitual drinker, which is the meaning learned at the introductory level and is probably the intended meaning. Pedro might be drunk at this party, but it has nothing to do with liking to talk to everyone, and neither does being a student or lawyer. At first glance, this section might seem irrelevant, but it does assess some conceptual knowledge given that some choices would only be appropriate with the verb ser, while others would only be appropriate with estar. The other advantage to this part might be that students are not concentrating on whether to choose ser or estar, and therefore answering these questions correctly reflects a deeper understanding of these verbs. (Anyone can memorize a list of uses for each verb and correctly apply the list to a multiple-choice test.) The last and perhaps most useful section is the composition. Students are asked to describe their dorm room or apartment and are given guiding questions like Where is your room or apartment located? that should elicit the use of ser and estar. Not only is it harder for students to get by with a shallow (memorized) understanding of these verbs, because they must produce these verbs themselves, but they are most likely concentrating on vocabulary, so the usage of these verbs is more natural in this section. Method A Windows machine (Dell Dimension 2400 series) ran SPSS version 12.0 to perform bivariate correlations. Although other data were collected, this study examines only the scores on the 50-item objective test and the frequency of correct and incorrect uses of ser and estar in short essays about participants living spaces by both current level of Spanish and the number of semesters of Spanish students reported having completed at the time of testtaking. This analysis is broken into two components. In the first analysis course levels were not combined, even where some groups

66 Chrestomathy: Volume 3, 2004 were much smaller than other groups. For the second analysis it was deemed necessary to combine certain groups to compensate for unequal group sizes. Specifically, the 202 and 250 levels were combined to form the 200 level, the 313 and 314 levels were combined to form the 300-1 level, the 361 and 371 levels were combined to form the 300-2 level, the 344 and 381 levels were combined to form the 300-3 level, and the 446 and 458 levels were combined to form the 400 level. Results Scores on the Objective Test (Analysis 1) Students at the different course levels appeared to differ in scores on the 50-item objective test. The 371 level (n=4) had the highest mean at 47.75, while the 202 level (n=160) had the lowest mean at 38.28. Correct Uses of Ser (Analysis 1) Groups also appear to differ in the mean number of times each group correctly uses the verb ser. The 344 level (n=15) had the highest mean at 3.87 correct uses, while the 202 level (n=160) had the lowest mean at 2.36 correct uses. The 250 level (n=6) scored similarly to the 202 level with a mean of 2.33 correct uses, while the 314 level (n=22) scored similarly to the 344 level with a mean of 3.55. Correct Uses of Estar (Analysis 1) The groups do not appear to differ with respect to the number of correct uses of estar. Incorrect Uses of Ser (Analysis 1) Groups do not appear to differ in the number of incorrect uses of ser. Incorrect Uses of Estar (Analysis 1) Groups do not appear to differ with respect to the mean number of incorrect uses of estar.

Egan: Acquisition of Ser and Estar in Undergraduates 67 Summary of Analysis 1 Although the unevenness of the group sizes is problematic for reasons discussed further in the Discussion section, groups seem to differ by level in their scores on the objective test and in the number of times that they use ser correctly. However, no significant differences are apparent between levels in correct uses of estar, incorrect uses of ser, and incorrect uses of estar. Scores on the Objective Test (Analysis 2) There appears to be a difference between levels in scores on the 50-item objective test. The 200 appears to be significantly different from all other levels. The combined 313 and 314 levels seem to differ significantly from the other two groups of 300-level students but not from the 400-level. However, no correlation was found between level and score. Correct Uses of Ser (Analysis 2) The students in the various levels of Spanish do appear to differ in the total number of correct uses of estar. Specifically, a significant difference is expected between the 200-level and the 400-level and between the 200-level and the group consisting of 361 and 371. However, no correlation was found between level and correct uses of ser. Correct Uses of Ser by Level (95% Confidence Interval for Mean) N Mean Devi- Error a tion Lower Upper Mini- Maxi- mum mum No Data 47 2.70 1.284. 187 2.33 3.08 1 6 200 164 2.36 1.365. 107 2.15 2.57 0 8 300-1 70 3.16 1.337. 160 2.84 3.48 1 6 300-2 5 3.40 1.817. 812 1.14 5.66 1 6 300-3 31 3.55 1.362. 245 3.05 4.05 1 6 400 11 3.09. 701. 211 2.62 3.56 2 4 Total 328 2.73 1.395. 077 2.58 2.88 0 8

68 Chrestomathy: Volume 3, 2004 Correct Uses of Estar (Analysis 2) The students in the various levels of Spanish do appear to differ in the total number of correct uses of estar. Specifically, a significant difference is expected between the 200-level and the 400-level and between the 200-level and the group consisting of 361 and 371. However, no correlation was found between level and correct uses of estar. No Data N Correct Uses of Estar by Level (95% Confidence Interval for Mean) Mean Error Lower Upper Deviation Minimum Maximum 47 2.32 1.163. 170 1.98 2.66 0 6 200 164 1.96 1.396. 109 1.75 2.18 0 8 300-1 70 2.46 1.003. 120 2.22 2.70 0 5 300-2 5 3.20 1.304. 583 1.58 4.82 2 5 300-3 31 2.71 1.488. 267 2.16 3.26 0 7 400 11 3.18 1.471. 444 2.19 4.17 2 7 Total 328 2.25 1.334. 074 2.11 2.39 0 8 N Incorrect Uses of Ser by Level (95% Confidence Interval for Mean) Mean Error Lower Upper Deviation Minimum Maximum No 47. 17. 433. 063. 04. 30 Data 200 164. 15. 393. 031. 09. 21 300-1 70. 10. 347. 041. 02. 18 300-2 5. 20. 447. 200 -.36. 76 0 1 300-3 31. 06. 250. 045 -.03. 16 0 1 400 11 0 0 0 0 Total 328. 13. 372. 021. 09. 17

Egan: Acquisition of Ser and Estar in Undergraduates 69 Incorrect Uses of Ser (Analysis 2) The students in the various levels of Spanish do not appear to differ in the total number of incorrect uses of ser. No correlation was found between level and incorrect uses of ser. Incorrect Uses of Estar (Analysis 2) Groups do not appear to differ in the total number of incorrect uses of estar. No correlation was found between level and incorrect uses of estar. Incorrect Uses of Estar by Level (95% Confidence Interval for Mean) Lower Upper Mini- Maxi- N Mean Devi- Error mum mum a tion No 47. 49. 748. 109. 27. 71 Data 200 164. 57. 859. 067. 43. 70 0 4 300-1 70. 29. 515. 062. 16. 41 300-2 5 0 0 0 0 300-3 31. 35. 608. 109. 13. 58 400 11. 27. 647. 195 -.16. 71 Total 328. 46. 753. 042. 38. 54 0 4 Discussion In both analyses, students in various current levels differed in their scores on the objective test. However, scores were not correlated with current level. This could be a reflection of the fact that the 400 level group scored lower than all levels except 200 and 313-314. As one might expect, there does appear to be a relationship between the current course level and scores on the objective test: the higher the level, the higher one is likely to score. Similarly, there seems to exist a relationship between course level and correct uses of ser and estar: the higher one s course level, the more frequently he is likely to use ser correctly. Presumably, students in the higher levels of Spanish

70 Chrestomathy: Volume 3, 2004 have been exposed to more Spanish than students in the lower levels, and the normal acquisition process is taking place. Curiously, this does not appear to be the case with the incorrect uses of ser and estar. One might reasonably assume that as students progress in their Spanish studies, they would make fewer errors. However, this study did not show in either analysis any significant difference between groups in terms of how often they used ser and estar incorrectly. While this seems counter-intuitive, there might be a logical explanation. One possibility is that students in the lower levels of Spanish tend not to use the unfamiliar verbs, which causes them to have few incorrect uses (as well as few correct uses). Meanwhile, more advanced students use these verbs more often than less proficient students, but they tend to use them correctly. This causes them also to have more correct uses but equally few errors, which conceals the difference between lower-level students and high-level students. This also explains how lower- and upper-level students can simultaneously differ in their correct uses of these verbs but not in their incorrect uses of these verbs. Also worthy of notice is the fact that the graph of the mean incorrect uses of ser does not resemble the graph of mean incorrect uses of estar. This seems to suggest that the processes of acquiring these two verbs are not the same. At the lower levels, students seem about equally likely to use ser and estar incorrectly. In response, two possible components might be suggested. First, beginning students tend to overuse estar because many of the first phrases they learn use the verb estar (e.g., Como estás or how are you). However, the verb ser, and in particular its third-person singular (present indicative) form es closely resembles the English word is in both appearance and use. Thus, these students have not developed intuition about which verb is appropriate in a given situation. However, at the higher levels (not including 361-371 due to its having only 5 students), ser is less likely to be used incorrectly, while estar is almost as likely to be used incorrectly as in the lower levels. That is, the upper-level students are overusing estar. Taking a closer look, they are the grammar-related classes (344 and 381) that tend to be making this mistake. Since this topic was most likely covered in

Egan: Acquisition of Ser and Estar in Undergraduates 71 344 prior to the time of test-taking, it is possible that these students have suddenly been made aware of the fact that estar is not as rare as they might have thought and are now overusing the verb as a result, which leads to both more correct uses of estar and more incorrect uses of estar. Limitations Unfortunately, the sizes of groups consisting of the different course levels are too uneven for legitimate comparison, even when appropriate groups are combined. Specifically, the 200-level group (n=164) is more than twice as large as the second largest group, 313-314 (n=70). This group is more than twice as large as the third largest group, 344-381 (n=31), which is more than twice as large as the 400-level group (n=11), which is more than twice as large as 361-371 (n=6). When students answered the question as to how many semesters of Spanish they had taken at the College of Charleston, it is apparent that some answered in the number of hours, while others answered in the number of semester or the number of classes. For example, some students claimed to have had 12 semesters of Spanish even though they were only at the 200-level, which represents at most (assuming no repeats) three completed semesters of Spanish. One student even wrote hours next to her response on her questionnaire. Because we know that some students were responding with the number of hours they had taken, it is unclear for most of the other students whether they meant semesters or hours. Therefore, no meaningful comparisons between groups can be drawn with regard to the number of semesters of Spanish taken. Because of uneven group sizes and other statistical assumptions, statements made about differences and correlations are merely conjectures one would need to run appropriate nonparametric statistical tests to draw any conclusions.