Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education"

Transcription

1 School Psychology Review 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4, pp Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education Mary K. Rothbart and Laura B. Jones University of Oregon Abstract: In recent years, advances in the study of temperament have identified a short list of temperament dimensions. These include positive emotionality/approach, fear, irritability/ frustration, attentional persistence and activity level. In this article, we review research on the first four of these dimensions, briefly linking them to underlying biological systems. We then apply our knowledge of temperament to teachers approaches to children s mastery motivation, fear of novelty, and ego based anxiety. We argue that educators training should include a basic understanding of the development of temperament as well as methods for assessing individual differences in children s emotional reactivity and attentional selfregulation. Recent increases in our understanding of temperament have created new ways for approaching the education of the child. These approaches allow us to appreciate the ways in which children differ from another, and the positive contributions of this variability to the classroom and society. In this article, we begin with some introductory remarks on temperament and schooling. We then review results of studies identifying basic dimensions of temperament that can affect children s exploration, discovery, and learning as well as their discouragement, anger, and avoidance of potential sources of knowledge. We describe the structure of temperament as it has emerged from research during the past two decades, briefly linking this structure to theories of underlying biological systems. The structure of temperament that has emerged includes individual tendencies toward fear, angedfrustration, positive affect and approach, activity level, and effortful or executive attention. These emotional, energetic, and attentional systems have been conserved in evolution and are associated in theories of temperament with underlying neural networks and neurotransmitter activity. We consider implications of individual variability in these tendencies for the child s experiences in school. We discuss ways in which mastery motivation may be related to children s fear and avoidant tendencies, approach, and effortful control of emotion and action. Finally, we review implications of a temperamental understanding of children for the training of educators and the assessment of children. Temperament and Schooling In theories of temperament, early individual differences in emotionality, activity level, and attention are seen as based on a set of brain systems underlying children s reactivity and selfregulation (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). When there is variability in the sensitivity of these systems, the same stimulus does not have the same effect for all: children differ in how easily and intensely they become fearful, frustrated, or positively excited. They differ in capacities for attentional self-regulation. By the time of school entry, temperament affects the nature of the child s adjustment to the requirements and challenges of the educational setting. Experiences of success and failure in the school related to these adjustments influence the child s representations and evaluations of self, school, teachers and peers. This work has been supported with NIMH Grant No awarded to the first author. Address all correspondence concerning this article to Mary K. Rothbart, Department of Psychology, 1227 University of Oregon, Eugene, OR Maryroth@oregon.uoregon.edu. Copyright 1998 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN

2 480 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 Although temperament researchers had originally believed that temperament systems would be in place very early in development and change little with the passage of time (e.g., Buss & Plomin, 1984), we have since learned that temperament systems follow a developmental course (Rothbart, 1989; Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Children s reactive tendencies to both experience and express negative and positive emotions, and their responsivity to events in the environment can be observed very early in life, but children s self-regulatory executive attention develops relatively late and continues to develop throughout the early school years. Because executive attention is involved in the regulation of emotions, some school children will be lacking in controls of emotion and action that other children can demonstrate with ease. The influence of temperament upon personality and schooling and contributions of the school to personality can be considered in numerous ways. Temperament refers to the relative strength of children s emotional reactions and related behaviors as well as their capacities for self-regulation. Emotional reactions and selfregulatory capacities are important in child development. Children differ, however, in the strengths of their temperamental tendencies and capabilities. Some children are at the extremes of temperament; for example, some children tend to approach new situations rapidly and impulsively, are less subject to fear and have less capacity for attentional control than others. Some children are easily overwhelmed by stimulation; some show powerful tendencies to irritability and frustration. Children at the extremes clearly demonstrate the power of temperament in the classroom, and it is important to take these temperament characteristics into account. While children s individual differences influence their own adaptation to the classroom, these characteristics also have an important influence on how students are viewed and treated by their teachers. Thomas and Chess (1977) developed the concept goodness-of-fit to describe how well children s characteristics, capacities and temperament meet the expectations and demands of the environment (Keogh, 1994; Thomas & Chess, 1977; Thomas, Chess, &Birch, 1968). The goodness-of-fit concept provides a framework for thinking about interactions between children and the classroom environment. Maintenance of classroom order is a priority for teachers. When teachers are presented with behavior potentially disruptive to classroom routine, they must make quick decisions about how to manage and respond to this behavior. Poor fit can result when children s characteristics fail to meet teacher s expectations of acceptable behavior. Teachers have a priori ideas about the qualities of a model student (Keogh, 1989); these qualities include temperament variables such as high attention span, adaptability and approach, and low activity and reactivity. Students are viewed as more teachable when they closely match this set of variables, and less teachable the fiuther they are from the set. The quality of the teacher-child interaction also is related to this match. In one study, Martin ( 1989) reported that children distractible and low in attention received more criticism from their teachers. Pullis (1985) discovered that when teachers thought children were capable of, but not practicing, self-control, they were more likely to discipline the children with more punitive and coercive discipline techniques. Increasing teachers understanding of children s individual differences and their relation to adaptations or problems can be extremely helpful in shifting the focus from teachers negative attributions of purposeful misbehavior to active problem-solving. Increasing teachers awareness of how children s temperament dimensions might contribute to the situation can lead to reduced conflict (Pullis, 1985) and to the development of appropriate strategies specific to the temperament dimensions involved. When children feel accepted and respected as individuals, the focus moves from one of accusation to one of support; children feel less compelled to expend energy defending their positions and they are more inclined to consider alternatives for resolving problems. While individual differences play a role in children s initial goodness-of-fit to the classroom environment, especially for children at the extremes of temperament, it also is helpful to consider how these processes underlie adjustment and adaptation to the school environment for all children. For children generally, punishment can lead to inhibition and avoidance; failure can lead to defense or frustration. Positive affect and goal orientation are related to curiosity and energy for most children, and executive attention supports children s flexible response. The dynamics of these systems are important in maintaining and enhancing children s motivation in the classroom,

3 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 48 1 not just for children at the extremes, but for all children, and we consider these dynamics in the next section of this article. Finally, temperament does not account for all aspects of school experience. Children s reactions to success and failure in the classroom are subject to powerful socializing influences in the home and during previous schooling. For example, temperament affects self-evaluations, but children will enter schools with differing degrees of preparation for classroom learning, and with personalities that vary in their vulnerability and resilience to challenge and disappointment. Children whose parents have given unconditional support are more likely to have more stable positive selfevaluations; children whose parents have been rejecting or conditional in their acceptance are likely to be less resilient (Harter, 1998). Dimensions of Treatment Although temperament theory has an ancient history, its application to child study has been relatively recent (see review in Rothbart & Bates, 1998). In the great normative developmental studies of the 1920s and 1930s, Gesell (1928) and Shirley (1 933) identified what Shirley called the early core of personality. Gesell and Shirley argued that although children s emotional reactions change with development (for example, children decrease in their expressed negative emotion with age), children s individuality can nevertheless be observed in such reactions as fear, positive affect, humor, and irritability. Gesell noted that for any set of temperament characteristics, alternative pathways for development also will exist, dependent upon the child s social experiences. To illustrate this principle, he used lhe case of CD, who had maintained her tendencies to positive emotions, beginning at the time that she was nine months old She is now five years of age, and in spite of a varied experience in boarding homes and institutions she has not lost these engaging characteristics. They are part and parcel of her makeup.... It can be predicted with much certainty that she will retain her present emotional equipment when she is an adolescent and an adult. But more than this cannot be predicted in the field of personality. For whether she becomes a delinquent, and she is potentially one, will depend upon her subsequent training, conditioning, and supervision. She is potentially, also, a willing, helpful, productive worker. (Gesell, 1928, cited in Kessen, 1965, p. 223) Parents and educators employ training, conditioning and supervision of children in the hope and expectation of positive developmental outcomes. To do this well, it is helpful when the child s reactive and regulative equipment is taken into account. Researchers with the greatest impact on studies of temperament in childhood are psychiatrists Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas and their colleagues in the New York Longitudinal Study, (NYLS); (Thomas & Chess, 1977, Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig, & Korn, 1963). The NYLS researchers initially conducted a content analysis on mother s reports of their children s behavior during the first six months of life, identifying nine dimensions of temperament variability. The dimensions they identified included children s activity level, threshold for reaction, rhythmicity of eating, sleeping and bowel movement patterns, intensity of response, approach versus withdrawal to new situations, general mood, adaptability to change, distractibility and attention spadpersistence. Recent research however, suggests important revisions of this list (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). In part, revisions of the list are required because temperament develops. The NYLS content analysis was based on individual variability in young infants. The dimensions identified characterize children early in life; laterdeveloping dimensions were not considered. One major area undergoing development is effortfid or executive attention (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996): the orienting and distractibility observed in young infants early months of life is chiefly reactive and automatic. Beginning late in the first year and developing through the grade school years, however, executive attentional control allows increasing self-regulation of behavior and emotion. A second reason why revisions of the NYLS dimensions are needed is that the nine NYLS dimensions, devised for clinical and not psychometric purposes, show considerable conceptual overlap (Rothbart & Mauro, 1990). For example, NYLS mood and adaptability dimensions overlap with each other and with the approach-withdrawal construct. This conceptual overlap is reflected in research findings. Measures of some of the nine dimensions are so strongly intercorrelated that a I

4 482 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 single dimension is indicated (e.g., approachwithdrawal and adaptability); for other dimensions, different aspects of a postulated dimension are not sufficiently related to each other. For example, a child may not be intense in both fear and positive reactions, thereby lowering internal reliability for an intensity scale (see reviews by Martin, Wisenbaker, & Huttunen, 1994; Rothbart & Mauro, 1990). Factor analytic work reviewed by Martin et al. (1 994) and Rothbart and Mauro (1 990) suggests that the dimensions identified by the NYLS can be described by a smaller number of factors. For school-age children, this shorter list of temperament dimensions includes negative emotionality assessed in fear and irritability/ frustration as well as approacwpositive affect, attentional persistence, and activity level (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Two much smaller factors, rhythmicity and threshold, have been found in some studies. In the next section, we briefly review the first four of these dimensions, includmg emotional reactivity dimensions and the later developing executive attentional selfregulatory system. We also briefly refer to links made between these systems and underlying brain systems. The references to brain systems will not be clear to all readers, and can be skipped, but they are important. For readers who desire an introduction to some of the ideas presented here, books by Posner and Raichle (1 994), LeDoux ( 1996), and Panksepp ( 1998) are recommended. Positive AffectIApproach This cluster of temperament dimensions includes positive affect and rapid approach of rewarding objects or events. Individual differences in positive affect can be observed very early and they show considerable stability across the first year of life. Smiling is related to the rapidity of infants approach to novel objects as early as 6 months of age (Rothbart, 1988). Smiling and laughter in the laboratory during infancy also predict activity, approach, and impulsivity at seven years of age as reported by children s mothers (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, 1995). In our research assessing temperament in children 3 to 7 years of age, using the Children s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ; Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 1997), we found activity level to be related to a broad factor that also includes approach and positive affect and we have called this factor Surgency or Extraversion. Gray (1 987) described a similar Behavioral Activation System (BAS), which responds to signals of reward. It involves the recruiting of dopamine systems facilitating action. Depue and Iacono (1989) also described a doparnine-related Behavioral Facilitation System (BFS). Blockage of this system is seen to lead to imtative and aggressive behavior to remove the obstacle or threat. A similar approach system is Panksepp s ( 1986a) Expectancy-Foraging System, which includes motor and autonomic responses to emotional states such as desire, curiosity, anticipatory eagerness, and locomotion. All of these models posit neural systems linked to positive affect and related to facilitated behavior, particularly in the form of approach (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). As discussed below, the approach system is of central importance to schooling, in its associations with curiosity, eagerness, and energy toward goal satisfaction related to the construct of mastery motivation. When it is not controlled, however, the child may show failure of self-regulation in the classroom. Fear Fearfulness can be defined as a tendency to negative affect and inhibition or withdrawal in response to novelty, challenge, or signals of punishment. This dimension has been called withdrawal in the NYLS studies (Thomas & Chess, 1977), fearfulness (Rothbart, 1989) and behavioral inhibition (Kagan, 1994). Individual differences in fearfulness to novelty can be seen by the last quarter of the first year of life, and fear reactions in older infants are predictable from four-month-olds negative affect and activity in response to stimulation at four months of age (Kagan, 1998). Individual differences in children behavioral inhibition and shyness show considerable stability with time, and infants fear reactions in the laboratory during the first year of life are predictive of their mothers reports of children s fear and shyness at age seven years (Rothbart et al., 1995). Of all the temperament systems, fear has probably been the most studied by neuroscientists (e.g., Davis, 1992; LeDoux, 1996; review by Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). Fear responses are rapidly recruited in the amygdala, part of the brain s limbic system. Gray (1 982) described fear or anxiety reactions in terms of a Behavioral Inhibition System or BIS. In Gray s

5 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 483 model, brain circuits underlying the BIS include hippocampus, subiculum, septum and related structures. The BIS opposes the behavioral approach of the BAS. Because many situations involve cues related to both reward and punishment, reward cues would trigger the BAS, punishment cues the BIS. Persons with stronger BAS tendencies will then be more likely to act in a given situation and be seen as extraverted; those with stronger BIS to become shy, inhibited or introverted. A fear reaction includes many response levels (see Rothbart et al., 1994 for review), including arousal, recruitment of attention toward the fear object and possible escape routes, autonomic nervous system effects (heart rate and respiratory increases, sweaty palms), and somatic nervous system reactions (muscle tension, arrest of ongoing behavior, tendency to startle). Influences of fear or anxiety in disrupting thinking, e.g., in children s test anxiety, have been an object of interest of school psychologists for many years (e.g., Sarason, Lighthall, Davidson, Waite, & Ruebush, 1960). Irritability/Frustration Irritability/frustration is defined as affect in response to failure in goal attainment, removal of reward, or blockage of progress toward a goal. In some factor analyses of temperament data on school-age children, this dimension forms part of a cluster of Negative Emotionality, combined with fearfulness, tendencies to discomfort, and sadness; in other studies, it emerges as a separate factor (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Infant research, however, uniformly differentiates these two forms of negative affect. In Gray s (1982) theoretical model, the amygdala, hypothalamus, and midbrain support aggressive and defensive behavior in response to frustration and form a fighvflight system. Early fiustration reactions of infants in the laboratory are predictive of children s reactions to frustration (and their levels of aggression) as reported by their mothers when the children are seven years of age (Rothbart et al., 1995). Like fear, frustration has important implications for school situations associated with failure, aggression, and defensive tendencies. Attention Span/ Effortful Control Attentional processes show considerable development from infancy through the early school years (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). Infants demonstrate reactive orienting that, when sustained, appears to be related to interest in an object. This orienting system is subject to habituation, when repeated events no longer evoke as strong a reaction, but it also supports learning, when an event indicates where something interesting or important will appear next (Posner & Raichle, 1994). The posterior orienting network, including cortical, midbrain and thalamic areas, underlies early orienting (Posner & Raichle, 1994). Children differ in both their latency to orient and in their duration of orienting to novelty (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). Individual differences in duration of orienting in infancy are related to caregiver reports of smiling and laughter and vocal activity (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). By late in the first year of life, a second, more anterior brain system is developing that will allow deployment of attention in the service of longerterm goals as well as planning and persistence in the face of distraction (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). We have called this the executive attention system, and it supports effortful control, that is, the ability to inhibit a dominant response to perform a subdominant response. Executive attention has been the subject of considerable recent study by neuropsychologists and cognitive psychologists. Executive or effortful attention is related to the activation of midline frontal brain areas, including the anterior cingulate and adjacent structures (Posner & Raichle, 1994). Effortful control constructs have become extremely important in understanding influences of temperament on behavior. Until recently, almost all of the major theories of temperament have focused on its more reactive aspects related to reward, punishment, and arousal to stimulation. Thus, people have been seen to be at the mercy of their temperamental tendencies to approach or avoid a situation or stimulus, given reward or punishment cues. More extraverted individuals were expected to be sensitive to reward and to show tendencies to rapid approach; more fearful or introverted individuals, sensitive to punishment, showing inhibition or withdrawal from excitement (Gray, 1975). Systems of effortfbl control, however, allow the approach of situations in the face of immediate cues for punishment., and avoidance of situations in the face of immediate cues for reward. The programming of effortful control is critical to socialization. The work of Kochanska (1995) indicates that the

6 484 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 development of conscience is related to temperamental individual differences in effortful control. The home and the school are two of the major sites for programming attention, effort, and conscience. Using these dimensions of temperament variability as a starting point, we now consider issues that relate temperament to the classroom. Applying Temperament Systems to the Classroom Rothbart, Ahadi, and Hershey (1 994) described some of the basic ways in which temperament can be seen to affect children s social experience, and we would expect these to apply to the classroom as well as to the home. First, individual differences in temperament mean that the same environment will be processed differently by different children. Some children will be more easily overwhelmed by intense levels of stimulation (e.g., noise, activity) than others; our data indicate that these children also are likely to be more fearful (Rothbart et al., 1997). The reaction we are referring to is not equivalent to fear, however; it is a feeling of discomfort that may make processing comitive material more difficult. Other children have stronger tendencies to positive affect and potentially out-of-control action; these children may become especially excited about upcoming positive events, and they are likely to be more subject to contagious excitement. These temperamental reactions, in turn, will form the basis for children s affective memories and evaluations of the classroom and their classroom habits. For some children, a teacher s actions that might be expected to gain the child s focus of attention also will not be sufficient, and these children may fail to be influenced by what the teacher is saying and doing. Teachers can directly address some of these tendencies. Fearful children, for example, can benefit by gradual introduction of new information and novel situations, until they are comfortable with them. With higher comfort levels, inhibition of behavioral response is less likely, permitting the child to have a relatively positive experience with novel situations. For children highly sensitive to overstimulation, loud and boisterous activity can be stressful and fatiguing, and quiet places allowing them recover from excitement are helpful. Children who are very active may require motor outlets in opportunities for exercise and movement. Motor activities (pointing, reaching, moving fkom one location to another, retrieving objects) also can be used as an adjunct to teaching. Temperamental processes also are strongly implicated in the nature of social learning. Gray (1975, 1982) described individuals with high behavioral activation systems (BAS) as more subject to reward; individuals with high behavioral inhibition systems (BIS) as more subject to punishment. This has important implications for the classroom. Much of what adults require of children involves the inhibition of actions the child might prefer to be doing, so that for children with a strong BIS, compliance will be much easier than for children with,a strong BAS and a weak BIS. For the latter children, it may be more important to stress what the child can do in a given situation rather than what a child should not be doing. On the other hand, children with a strong BIS can become more easily discouraged and may benefit from frequent but accurate feedback about their performance, to counteract their tendencies to discouragement. Attentional switching also may be so difficult for some children that both rewards and punishments may have little effect. One temperament characteristic also can be used to moderate or control another. Children higher in positive affectlapproach will be more likely to become excited about upcoming events, but unless these reactions are controlled by the BIS (fear or inhibition) or by attentional capacities, they are likely to become excited about topics that are not part of the teacher s agenda. When an outgoing chiid is deficient in executive attentional control, it may be especially dificult to shift the child s enthusiasms in the direction the teacher has chosen. Fearfulness or behavioral inhibition to punishment or challenge (Kagan, 1998) works in opposition to approach, with more inhibited children more likely to be quiet and reserved, and to resist impulsive action. Although educators often attempt to motivate children through positive means alone, in fact, classroom rules are often supported by children s fear of the results of infraction. Fear of not receiving potential rewards or the possibility of poor grades also is a motivating force in children. Forthe child not prone to temperamentally based fear or school anxiety, these influences may be less effective, and attentional control may be much more important. Both fear (the BIS) and effortful control also

7 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 485 may moderate children s risky, impulsive and aggressive behavior. For example, models by Lytton (1990) and Quay (Quay, Routh, & Shapiro, 1987) implicate temperament in the development of aggressive and conduct problems. Quay et al. (1987) argued that aggressive children are likely to have lower levels of the BIS as well as strongly reactive reward or approach systems (BAS). Rothbart et al. (1994) found that infants with higher activity and positive affect (BAS) in the laboratory and stronger anger/ frustration reactions were more likely to engage in aggressive behaviors at age seven; children who had been more fearful and inhibited (BIS) were less likely to behave aggressively; these latter children also showed more empathy to others. Children who tend to act quickly and impulsively, or who explode with frustration, need practice in attentional control. Cognitivebehavioral approaches for training rule-based self-control in children with attentional deficits have often not been effective (Cherkes- Julkowski, Sharp, & Stolzenberg, 1997). After the fact, this is not too surprising. These strategies often subject children to intensified cognitive demands as they attempt to invoke and apply general rules for their behavior that make compliance even more difficult. Cherkes-Julkowski et al. (1997) suggested that a master-apprentice cooperative approach to learning can be helpful in training self-regulation skills. In this approach, the teacher initially, has the executive function of organizing, regulating, and monitoring the process as a whole. It is through the joint efforts of repeated action and intersubjectivity that apprentices internalize the actions of the other (Cherkes-Julkowski et al., 1997, p. 80). Apprenticeship approaches require careful task analysis. This analysis can be further implemented by cooperative goal-directed learning using teaching aides, who can help to pace children who may work too fast or too slowly because of self-regulation problems, including anxiety about their work. Masterapprentice activity can serve important socialemotional goals for children, providing adult acceptance and support in the context of requiring effortful performance. Enlisting families and organizing volunteers (e.g., middle and high school students, retired adults, and even children from upper grades within the same school) will increase children s opportunities for these valuable one-on-one experiences. While it is hard to miss the excitement and enthusiasm expressed by children high in approach and positive affect, or the dramatic emotional displays of more highly reactive children, it also is important to identify and attend to children who are trying very hard, but for whom the process of learning does not generate such a noticeable effect. It is easy to overlook the efforts of these more subdued children and consequently, they may not experience the usual rewards of the classroom. This could lead to discouragement. Attention to working with discouraged children is helpful even though they may not be actively stirring up excitement or disrupting the classroom. By rewarding effort it may be possible to decrease children s discouragement. Rewarding children s efforts puts the focus on the process of learning. With this focus, students are less likely to compare their performance to that of others, and are more likely to concentrate on their own performance goals. Mastery Motivation We now illustrate the application of temperament ideas to an important system in support of education, namely, mastery motivation. Mastery motivation is initially closely linked to the basic emotional-motivational systems seen in young children, and hence to variability in functioning of the dimensions identified with temperament. It is further linked to children s experiences of reward and punishment in mastery-related situations. Finally, however, with the passage of time, mastery motivation becomes increasingly affected by the evaluations of others and selfevaluations reflected in ego involvement and personality. Schooling takes advantage of basic dimensions of temperament in the creation of learning environments for children. One of the most important of these is positive affect and approach. The positive affecdapproach system is an early support for what has been called mastery motivation. Messer described mastery motivation as, ba psychological force that stimulates an individual to attempt independently, in a focused and persistent manner, to solve problems and master a skill or task that is moderately challenging to him or her (Messer, 1995, p. 319). McCall(l995) used the term disposition rather than force. He argued that mastery motivation is inborn, but influenced substantially by the environment. Barrett and Morgan (1995) agreed

8 486 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 that mastery motivation is intrinsic to the individual. Instrumental aspects of mastery motivation include task persistence, preference for challenge or novelty, and an inclination to control one s environment. Positive affect and approach tendencies are not the only contributors to children s mastery motivation. Tendencies to fear, frustration, and effortful control are all likely additional contributors. We have previously discussed three types of selection processes linking temperament and learning: children s temperamentally based response tendencies, their experience of the emotions and their attentional capacities (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997). Response tendencies regulate the type of information children are exposed to. Even during infancy, children act to select the level of stimulation they will experience, using self-soothing and selfstimulating behavior, or through their direction of attention (Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). As noted, positive affect/approach is related to exploration and stimulus seeking, and to duration of interest. The continuing power of interest or positive involvement to influence achievement is suggested by a meta-analysis of the studies involving children in grades 5 through 12 (Schiefele, Krapp, & Winteler, 1992). In a review of 121 studies conducted in 18 different countries, Schiefele et al. found that interest accounted for 10% of the variability in children s achievement. Interest also was more strongly related to achievement in boys than in girls. In our research with college students, we have found that self-reported higher positive affect is related to higher scores on the personality dimension of openness to experience, a likely contributor to mastery motivation (Evans & Rothbart, 1998). Children also differ in their direct emotional responses to events and related learning. Fearful children and children who are easily frustrated are more prone to associate negative affect with events, while children higher in approach experience more positive affect (Rothbart, 1989). These emotional responses further influence children s fbture reactions to challenge and the child s tendency to approach or avoid challenge. Temperament variability affects the types of information an individual stores in memory. Persons high in anxiety form stronger short-term memory representations for attended negative words (Reed & Derryberry, 1995) and these representations can influence how a child views himself or others (Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994). If children are fearful, their estimation of their abilities and likelihood of their success in a given situation also may be affected, as may be their perceptions about whether novel people or objects are a source of safety or of threat (Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994). Fearfbl children who rely primarily on avoidant strategies may find temporary relief fiom their anxiety, but their ability to learn about perceived situations of threat will be limited. They may not learn they can effectively cope in these situations and may persist in representing the self as vulnerable and ineffective (Cortez & Bugental, 1995). With development, however, children s capacity for effortful control increases. Attentional control can strongly influence sources of input: children are increasingly able to shift attention away from threatening stimuli and. internal feelings of inefficacy; at the same time they can more readily focus on positive information, which can lead to improved self-evaluations relating to feelings of efficacy and success. Finally, they can overcome reactive tendencies and persist in a task, even in the midst of negative consequences (Denybeny & Rothbart, 1997). Attention is not the only route to more positive experiences for vulnerable children. It is important to remember that even the most vulnerable and negative child also has capacities for positive experiences. Accentuating the positive in the child s experience can serve to increase mastery motivation. Thinking about temperament in this way, we can consider interactions among systems that can either facilitate or inhibit children s mastery motivation. Children high in approach and low in fear may launch into new situations with zeal; this behavior is useful in that the children are continually exposed to situations with the potential for reward, but the lack of fear controls can lead to impulsive behavior. On the other hand, strong fear and weak behavioral activation can lead to overregulation of approach; children may avoid novel situations, resulting in missed opportunities for the positive experiences of mastery. For these children, accentuating the positive is important. Mastery motivation will be sustained by children s experiences of reward or nonpunishment in achievement situations (Harter, 1980). With age, however, children s responses move from taking direct pleasure in mastering tasks to added concerns about the results of their efforts and the evaluation of others based on these results. Affect is still critically important to

9 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 487 mastery motivation, but it is now at least partially mediated by children s views of how others view their performance, and by children s related egoinvolvement, self-evaluation and sense of competence (Harter, 1980). Harter observes that the initial purity of affective response to mastery becomes diluted by the necessity to consider the reactions of significant others in the environment. Ego-Involvement and Mastery The development of ego involvement provides an interesting perspective on children s evolving reactions to success and failure, reward and punishment. Toward the middle of the second year, children begin to show signs of selfconsciousness about their appearance and behavior (Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979). They increasingly express pride in mastery and shame in failure to achieve a goal (Lewis & Feiring, 1989; Stipek, Recchia, & McClintic, 1992). Now, children are more likely to complete tasks, to pause after completing a task, and to refuse help (Jennings, 1993), suggesting early links between ego and success. Lewis, Alessandri, and Sullivan (1992) found that 33- to 36-month-old children showed more pride upon success in a difficult task compared to completion of an easy task. They also showed less shame upon failure in a difficult task compared to failure on an easy task. Children at this age are beginning to recognize that exerting effort on a challenging task is important, and that they should persist until the task is complete (Barrett & Morgan, 1995). Increased self-awareness also supports the emergence of later developing shyness. Buss ( 1985) argued that self-conscious shyness occurs when children have attained an advanced, cognitive self (Buss, 1985, p. 43). This selfconscious evaluation is not so heavily dependent upon temperament. Children who have been criticized and punished by others may come to demonstrate later appearing shyness even though they earlier showed little shyness (Asendorpf, 1990). It may be useful to distinguish between early appearing temperamental fearfulness or shyness and ego-related anxiety. Temperament tendencies to fear are seen in children s inhibition of excitement and approach of new situations and challenges. In addition, however, low or vulnerable self evaluation can lead children with a wide range of temperamental endowments to become anxious about the possibility of failure andor to resist evidence that they have failed (cf. Ausubel, 1996; Ausubel, Sullivan, & Ives, 1980). Evaluative reactions may nevertheless be affected by temperamental fearfulness. Harter (1980), for example, reported rudimentary signs of fearful children s decreased interest in challenging tasks and behavioral withdrawal when they are scrutinized and evaluated by others. In our view, children s ego concerns reflect the values of their society. In the United States, ego values exist for autonomous achievement, forthrightness and consistency between public and private selves (Harter, 1998). Ego values have traditionally varied for girls and boys, with individual success more important for boys and social approval and physical attractiveness more important for girls. As children s representations of self develop, their vulnerability and anxiety about failure in these valued areas increases (Harter, 1998). Children s temperamental susceptibility to fear will be a contributor to these reactions, but at least equally important will be children s ego needs for successful performance. Anxiety is related to early experiences in the family. Children whose feelings of self-worth depend chiefly upon their individual performance will be more anxious about the possibility of failure than children who achieve vicarious success from parental acceptance (Ausubel, 1996). Temperamental tendencies to fearfulness will contribute to anxiety reactions, but under ego pressures, even a temperamentally positive and approaching child can become vulnerable to anxiety about the possibility of failure. In addition, ego-involved children will be subject to the frustration, defensiveness, avoidance, and depression related to avoiding feelings of failure and decreased self-evaluation (Harter, 1998). Asendorpf ( 1990) conducted an important longitudinal study of children s shyness and behavioral inhibition in a classroom setting. In his research, children who showed fear of strangers (early appearing shyness) were inhibited in the classroom early in the year, but by the end of the year were likely to have made an adjustment to the class setting. Other children, however, became more inhibited during the course of the school year and increasingly isolated from others. Asendorpf suggests that this later developing shyness occurs for children who have behaved in ways that led to rejection from their peers. In Asendorpf and van Aken s (1994) follow ups of the children, early appearing shyness (stranger fear) was not the major

10 488 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 predictor of later self-esteem; the children likely to develop lower self-esteem were those showing later developing shyness. It is important to note, however, that Asendorpf s research was conducted in German classes where children remained in the same class from year to year, allowing stronger peer influence. Cross-cultural research also suggests that the value of outgoing versus shy behavior will differ from one cultural group to another. In the United States, early appearing shyness may create more problems of adjustment than it would in Germany. However, Asendorpf s findings suggest that punishment related concerns may be at least as important as initial temperament in the development of behavior problems with selfesteem. Teachers can be alert to peer rejection in the classroom and work to create opportunities for these children to become more involved with their peers. Closely monitored small group activity in which the teacher or aide observes the group at play, models appropriate strategies for initiating peer contact, and facilitates cooperative play is one helpful way to ease children into peer relationships and bolster their confidence in social situations. Cicchetti and Tucker ( 1994) argue that early incompetence tends to promote later incompetence, because the person reaches successive stages of development with inadequate resources. Children high in fear and/or low in self-evaluation may thus come to avoid achievement situations, resulting in relative inexperience and possible feelings of inadequacy. With the development of self-awareness, the child may experience feelings of shame connected with feelings of inadequacy, leading to an even stronger fear or anxiety and avoidance in response to novel or challenging situations. This developmental progression, however, is not without recourse. Changes in the external and internal environment may lead to improvements in an individual s ability to master developmental changes and to redirect the developmental trajectory (Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994). Repeatzd opportunities for mastery in novel situations might alter the child s perception of novel experiences as threatening and bad to a perception of the experiences as safe and positive. The emergence of effortful control is another important internal element to mastery. Using effort, children now will have an increased capacity to modulate reactive systems. In older children, two kinds of academic achievement goals have been identified, one including intrinsic mastery motivation and task involvement, the other extrinsic motivation, performance evaluation, and ego involvement. In a review of the relations between these two orientations and cognitive engagement, Pintrich, Marx, and Boyle (1993) reported that, a focus on mastery or learning goals can result in deeper cognitive processing on academic tasks than a focus on the self (ego-involved) or a focus on performance (grades, besting others), which seems to result in more surface processing and less overall cognitive engagement (p. 173). In turn, mastery motivation is affected by school structure: children are more oriented toward mastery when the goals they are directed toward are meaningful and challenging, when the children themselves have some control of the direction of their effort, and when evaluations are not highly focused on external rewards, competition or social comparison. When performance is focused on reward and competition, children tend to be more concerned about doing better than others or pleasing the teacher, than on understanding the content of what is taught (review by Pintrich et al., 1993). External standards do, however, have their place in the classroom. Although it may appear that external standards interfere with the development of intrinsic motivation, standards in the classroom actually serve to maintain order in addition to providing important learning goals for children to pursue. An optimal learning environment is one in which these external standards serve as a scaffold for students as they work to cultivate their own approach to leamirig. Teachers bring vast knowledge about how to meet these standards to the classroom. This knowledge, coupled with an increased understanding of children s individual differences permits the development of approaches that best match students needs, thereby stimulating children s intrinsic motivation. What fosters the development of intrinsic motivation? Recall that the definition of mastery motivation centers on an individual s attempts to solve challenging problems. Children s individual differences contribute to their unique experience of challenge, adjustment and mastery in school, but the classroom environment will have a substantial impact on this experience. Providing extracurricular activities and multiple opportunities in the classroom for young people with a wide variety of temperament characteristics to learn and to excel also promote feelings of

11 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 489 mastery. Following this approach, almost all children can find a school activity they are good at. In addition, many children will develop multiple interests, so that if one learning pursuit is not going well, another will be available to shore up the child s self-evaluation and maintain motivation for mastery. Education can do many things to encourage students to find joy in learning, to welcome new opportunities, and to follow through on tasks. Temperament: Training and Assessment In this section, we apply topics we have discussed to the training of educators and school psychologists and to the use of broad-based assessment in the classroom. Because temperament variability in emotion, motivation and selfregulation strongly influence children s learning and failure to learn, it becomes necessary for the educator to follow the advice of educational psychologists to teach children, not subject matter (Sarason, 1993, p. 124). To do this, educators should understand children s temperament and personality characteristics as well as they understand their cognitive-processing capacities. This means that programs for college and university training of educators should include state-of-the-art knowledge of child temperament and development and their relation to the classroom. In the creation and implementation of this curriculum, collaborations between educators and developmental psychologists would have much to offer. A second application of temperament ideas is to assessment. As in the assessment of cognitive skills, a multidimensional approach to temperament-related assessment is helpful. Information about temperament can be obtained from the parent, so that the teacher or school psychologist can learn about aspects of the child s emotional reactivity and attentional self-regulation as they are shown outside of the classroom. Measures such as the Children s Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart et al., 1997) for children from 3 to 8 years of age or McClowry, Hegvik, and Teglasi s (1993) measure for children from 8 to 12 years of age are useful for obtaining information about temperament from the parents. Another important source of information is the classroom teacher. In assessing temperament, Keogh s Teacher Temperament Questionnaire can assist teachers with identifying children s temperamental tendencies (Pullis, 1989; Pullis & Cadwell, 1985). Assessment of classroom structure also is essential (Keogh, 1994). Keogh argued that traditional assessment focuses upon the child while ignoring critical information about contributions of the school environment to the child s adaptations (Keogh & Speece, 1996). Children high in activity, for example, may have difficulty sitting for long periods of time; highly reactive children may be bothered by noise and crowding (Rothbart & Jones, in press). In assessment, one important distinction is between the children s early appearing fearfulness and their evaluation anxiety based on ego concerns. Some shy and fearful children will be slow to adapt to new situations, including testing, and they need time and familiarity with the setting to do their best. Other problems are more related to evaluation concerns. Variations in administration of test items can be used to examine effects of ego involvement and evaluation anxiety (Smith, 1988). Smith described an apparently anxious 10-year-old who is given accurate information that she or he had responded to some of the questions at above a 16-year level; this information led to increased confidence, allowing the child to correctly perform items she or he had previously failed. Other assessment variations can investigate sources of inattentive children s errors, for example, by looking for improvement when the child s attention is directed to relevant features of a display, or when competing responses are reduced, or when background stimulation is increased or decreased (Smith, 1988). Sarason et al. (1960) noted that we often forget what a powerful figure a teacher is in the lives of children. All children benefit from a classroom atmosphere created by the teacher that fosters their desire to learn, and provides exposure to a challenging and meaningful cumculum. They benefit from a place where efforts are encouraged and rewarded; an environment where children collaborate with peers, rather than compete with them, and a climate in which children are accepted and supported regardless of their intellectual and temperament characteristics. A Zen story may apply to this last point, One day Banzan was walking through a market when he overheard a customer ask a butcher for the best piece of meat he had. Every piece in my shop is the best, the butcher said. Hearing the butcher s explanation, Banzan became enlightened (Schiller, 1997). Acceptance benefits all children, helping them achieve classroom skills by taking on new

12 490 School Psychology Review, 1998, Vol. 27, No. 4 challenges, adapting to changing classroom demands, and persisting in their efforts to succeed. References Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and a twofactor model. Development Psychology, 26, Asendorpf, j. B., & van Aken, M. A. (1994). Traits and relationship status: Stranger versus peer goup inhibition and test intelligence versus peer group competence as early predictors of later self-esteem. Child Development, 65(6), Ausubel, D. P. (1996). Ego development and psychoputhologv. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Ausubel D. P., Sullivan, E. & Ives, S. (1980). Theory and problems of child development (3rd ed.). New York: Grune & Stratton. Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancy and toddlerhood: A conceptualization and review. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and 51-93). Norwood, NJ Ablex. Buss, A. H. (1985). Two kinds of shyness. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and 65-75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament; Early developing personaliry waits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, K. (1994). Development and selfregulatory structures of the mind. Development and Psychopathology, 6, Cherkes-Julkowski, M., Sharp, S.,& Stolzenberg, J. (1997). Rethinking attention deficit disorders. Cambridge, MA:. Brookline. Cortez, V. L., & Bugental, D. B. (1995). Priming of perceived control in young children as a buffer against fearinducing events. Child Development, 66, Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 15, Depue, R. A,, & Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, Denyberry D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, Evans, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Relationships bewen self-report temperament measures and personality. Unpublished manuscript. Gesell, A. (1928). infmcy and human growth. New York Macmillan. Gray, J. A. (1975). Elements ofa?wo process theory of learning. New York Academic Press. Gray, J. A. ( 1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford, ENG: Oxford University Press. Gray, J. A. (1987). Perspectives on anxiety and impulsivity: A commentary. Journal of Research in Personaliry, 21, Harter, S. (1980). The development of competence motivation in the mastery of cognitive and physical skills: Is there still a place for joy? Psychology of Motor Behavior and Sport, Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psycholod: Vol. 3. Social, emotional andpersonalitydevelopment (5th ed., pp ). New York: Wiley. Jennings, K. D. ( 1993). Mastery motivation and the formation of self-concept from infancy through early childhood. In D. J. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: development, measurement, and social processes (pp ). London: Routledge. Kagan, J. ( 1994). Galen sprophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic. Kagan, J.(1998).Biologyandthechild. In W. Damon(Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp ). New York Wiley. Keogh, B. K. (1989). Applying temperament research to school. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in ). New York Wiley. Keogh, B. K. (1994). Tempevent and teachers views of teachability. In W. B.-Cary & S. C. McDevitt (Eds.), Prevention and early intervention: Individual diferences as risk factors for the mental health of children (pp ). New York Brunner/Mazel. Keogh, B. K., & Speece, D. L. ( 1996). Learning disabilities within the context of schooling. In D. L. Speece, & B. K. Keogh (Eds.), Research on classroom ecologies: implications for inclusion of children with learning 1-14). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Kessen, W. (1965). The child. New Yoik Wiley. Kochanska, G. (1995). A longitudinal study of the roots of preschooler s conscience: Commi?ed compliance and emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster. Lewis, M., Alessandri, S.,& Sullivan, M. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a function of children s gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self: New York Plenum. Lewis, M., & Feiring, C. (1989). Infant, mother, and motherinfant interaction behavior and subsequent attachment. Child Development, 60, Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, Martin, R. P. (1989). Activity level, distractibility, and persistence: Critical characteristics in early schooling. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in ). New Yoik Wiley. Martin, R. P., Wisenbaker, J., & Huttunen, M. (1994). Review of factor analytic studies of temperament based on the Thomas-Chess structural model: Implications for the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, Jr., G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McCall, R. B. ( 1995). On definitions and measuresof mastery motivation. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery morivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and applications (pp ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. McClowry, S. G., Hegvik, R., & Teglasi, H. (1993). An examination of the construct validity of the Middle Childhood Temperament Questionnaire. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39,

13 Temperament, Self-Regulation, and Education 49 1 Messer, D. J. (1995). Mastery motivation: Past, present and future. In R H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins. conceptualizations, and applications. Advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 12, pp ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Panksepp, J. ( 1986). The anatomy of emotions. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emorion: Theory, research and experience: Val. 3. Biological foundations of emotions (pp ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Panksepp, J. (1998). Aflective neuroscience. New York: Oxford. Pintrich, P. R., Marx, R. W., & Boyle, R. A. (1993). Beyond cold conceptual change: The role of motivational beliefs and classroom contextual factors in the process of conceptual change. Review of Educational Research, 63, Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library. Pullis, M. (1985). Students' temperament characteristics and their impact on decisions by resource and mainstream teachers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 8, Pullis, M. (1989). Goodness-of-fit in classroom relationships. In W. B. Carey & S. C. McDevitt (Eds.), Clinical and educafional applications of remperament ). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. Pullis, M., & Cadwell, J. (1985). Temperament as a factor in the assessment of children educationally at risk. The Journal of Special Education, 19, Quay, H. C., Routh, D. K., & Shapiro, S. K. (1987). Psychopathology of childhood: From description to validation. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, Reed, M. A., & Denybeny, D. (1995). Temperament and attention to positive and negative trait information. Personality and individual diferences, 18, Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Temperament and the development of inhibited approach. Child Development, 59, Rothbart, M. K. ( 1989). Temperament and development. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in ). Chichester, ENG: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament and the development of personality. Journal of Abnormal psycho lo^, 103, Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S., Hershey, K., & Fisher, P. (1997). Invesrigafions of temperament at 3-7 years: The children S behavior questionnaire. Manuscript submitted for publication. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp ). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., Denyberry, D., & Hershey, K. (1995). Stability of temperament in childhood: Loboratory infant assessment to parent rqort at seven years. Unpublished manuscript. Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences at the inteeace of biology and behavior (pp ). Washington, Dc: American Psychological Association. Rothbart, M. K., & Jones, L. B. (in press). Temperament: Developmental perspectives. In R. Gallimore, C. Bemheimer, D. MacMillan, D. Speece, & S. Vaugh (Eds.), Developmentalperspectiveson children w'rh high incidence disabilities: Papers in honor of Barbara Keogh. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rothbart, M. K., & Mauro, J. A. (1990). Questionnaire approaches to the study of infant temperament. In J. W. Fagen & J. Colombo (Eds.), Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability and ). Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum. Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Artention in ear& development: Themes and variations. New York Oxford University Press. Sarason, S. B. (1993). The case for change: Rethinking rhe preparation of educafors. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Sarason, S. B., Lighthall, F. F., Davidson, K. S., Waite, R. R., & Ruebush, B. K. (1960). Anxiety in elementary school children. New York: Wiley. Schiefele U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest learning ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schiller, D. (1997). The litfle Zen calendar, Jan. 23. New York Workman. Shirley, M. M. (1933). Thefirst two years: A study of25 babies. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Smith, C. R. (1988). Ecological approaches in reading assessment. In C. N. Hedley, & J. S. Hicks, (Eds.), Reading and the special learner (pp ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Selfevaluation in young children. Monographs ofthe Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (Monograph 226), 100. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: BrunnedMazel. Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. G. (1968). Temperament and behavior disorders in children. New York: New York University Press. Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch. H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S. (1963). Behavioral individuality in early Childhood. New York: New York University Press. Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., & Korn, S. (1963). Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New Yo& New York University Press. Mary K. Rothbart, PhD, is Professor of Psychology at the University of Oregon. She is currently on a Senior Scientist award from NIH, allowing her to study the development of temperament and attention. Laura B. Jones is a graduate student studying developmental psychology at the University of Oregon. She is currently on an emotion training grant from NIH to study the development of attention and its influence on emotional expression.

Development Through the Lifespan. Psychosocial Stages During Infancy and Toddlerhood. First Appearance of Basic Emotions

Development Through the Lifespan. Psychosocial Stages During Infancy and Toddlerhood. First Appearance of Basic Emotions Development Through the Lifespan Chapter 6 Emotional and Social Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited

More information

This historical document is derived from a 1990 APA presidential task force (revised in 1997).

This historical document is derived from a 1990 APA presidential task force (revised in 1997). LEARNER-CENTERED PSYCHOLOGICAL PRINCIPLES: A Framework for School Reform & Redesign TABLE OF CONTENTS: Background Learner-Centered Principles Prepared by the Learner-Centered Principles Work Group of the

More information

Temperament and Its Impact on Child Development: Comments on Rothbart, Kagan, Eisenberg, and Schermerhorn and Bates

Temperament and Its Impact on Child Development: Comments on Rothbart, Kagan, Eisenberg, and Schermerhorn and Bates TEMPERAMENT Temperament and Its Impact on Child Development: Comments on Rothbart, Kagan, Eisenberg, and Schermerhorn and Bates Susan D. Calkins, PhD University of North Carolina, USA April 2012,, Rev.

More information

Executive Function in Context: The Role of Stress and Vulnerability and Opportunities for Intervention. Stephanie M. Jones

Executive Function in Context: The Role of Stress and Vulnerability and Opportunities for Intervention. Stephanie M. Jones Ending Poverty: Integrating Behavioral and Social Sciences Executive Function in Context: The Role of Stress and Vulnerability and Opportunities for Intervention Stephanie M. Jones Harvard Graduate School

More information

Motivation. Motivation as defined by Sage is the direction and intensity of one s effort.

Motivation. Motivation as defined by Sage is the direction and intensity of one s effort. Motivation In Sport Motivation Motivation as defined by Sage is the direction and intensity of one s effort. Pitfalls and Dangers of Motivating? Adopting specific motivational strategies for all situations

More information

EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL CONSEQUENCES OF HEAD INJURY

EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL CONSEQUENCES OF HEAD INJURY Traumatic brain injury EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL CONSEQUENCES OF HEAD INJURY Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a common neurological condition that can have significant emotional and cognitive consequences.

More information

The Dance of Attachment

The Dance of Attachment The Dance of Attachment The parenting challenge for carers fostering or adopting children with attachment difficulties Kim S. Golding Dancing the Same Steps Child signals attachment needs clearly Sensitive

More information

Guide to Using Results

Guide to Using Results Guide to Using Results Contact Information For assistance, call ACT Engage Customer Service at 319.337.1893, 8:30 a.m. 5:00 p.m., central time, Monday through Friday, or email engage@act.org. Resources

More information

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions Virtual Child Written Project Assignment Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions Virtual Child Report (Assignment) 1: Infants and Toddlers (20 points) Choose 7 or 8 questions whose total point

More information

Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011

Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011 Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011 Principles of Prevention Prevention programs should enhance protective factors and reverse or reduce risk factors (Hawkins et al. 2002). The risk of becoming a drug

More information

Using Individual Behaviour Support Plans

Using Individual Behaviour Support Plans Using Individual Behaviour Support Plans An individual behaviour support plan, which documents supports and strategies based on students unique and individual characteristics, will benefit students with

More information

www.projectlearnet.org WHAT IS COGNITIVE INTERVENTION/REHABILITATION?

www.projectlearnet.org WHAT IS COGNITIVE INTERVENTION/REHABILITATION? Tutorial: Cognitive Intervention/Rehabilitation (See Tutorials on Cognition; Transfer/Generalization; Instructional Routines; Attention; Memory and Memory Problems; Organization; Problem Solving; Concrete

More information

Emotional Quotient. Michael Sample. CEO Sample Co. 5-22-2013. Your Address Here Your Phone Number Here Your Email Address Here

Emotional Quotient. Michael Sample. CEO Sample Co. 5-22-2013. Your Address Here Your Phone Number Here Your Email Address Here Emotional Quotient CEO Sample Co. 5-22-2013 Introduction The Emotional Quotient report looks at a person's emotional intelligence, which is the ability to sense, understand and effectively apply the power

More information

A Guide for Enabling Scouts with Cognitive Impairments

A Guide for Enabling Scouts with Cognitive Impairments A Guide for Enabling Scouts with Cognitive Impairments What cognitive impairments are discussed in this manual? Autism Spectrum Disorder Attention Deficit Hyper Activity Disorder Depression Down Syndrome

More information

How To Improve A Child'S Learning Experience

How To Improve A Child'S Learning Experience Effective teaching and classroom management is about whole child - and whole school development for knowledge, skills and human values During the past years as an outcome of the UN Study on Violence against

More information

Executive Functions: Developing an Independent Learner NOVEMBER 12, 2014 KATHLEEN BRUNSWICK, MA JEFFREY A. DETESO, PH.D.

Executive Functions: Developing an Independent Learner NOVEMBER 12, 2014 KATHLEEN BRUNSWICK, MA JEFFREY A. DETESO, PH.D. Executive Functions: Developing an Independent Learner NOVEMBER 12, 2014 KATHLEEN BRUNSWICK, MA JEFFREY A. DETESO, PH.D. What do Independent Learners Look Like? Independent Learner Characteristics 1) Curious

More information

Types of Psychology. Alex Thompson. Psychology Class. Professor Phelps

Types of Psychology. Alex Thompson. Psychology Class. Professor Phelps Running Head: PSYCHOLOGY 1 Types of Psychology Alex Thompson Psychology Class Professor Phelps March 4, 2014 PSYCHOLOGY 2 Types of Psychology Developmental psychology Developmental psychology entails the

More information

Helping your child with Non-verbal Learning Disability

Helping your child with Non-verbal Learning Disability Helping your child with Non-verbal Learning Disability What is non-verbal learning disability? Non-verbal learning disability (NVLD) is a term used to describe a pattern of strengths and weaknesses in

More information

TEXAS RISING STAR WEBINAR SERIES: CURRICULUM AND EARLY LEARNING GUIDELINES RECORDED OCTOBER 29, 2015 NOTES

TEXAS RISING STAR WEBINAR SERIES: CURRICULUM AND EARLY LEARNING GUIDELINES RECORDED OCTOBER 29, 2015 NOTES TEXAS RISING STAR WEBINAR SERIES: CURRICULUM AND EARLY LEARNING GUIDELINES RECORDED OCTOBER 29, 2015 NOTES The topics that will be addressed during these webinars include: 1. The General Overview: Curriculum

More information

EMOTIONAL DISTURBANCE

EMOTIONAL DISTURBANCE I. DEFINITION "Emotional disturbance" means a condition exhibiting one or more of the following characteristics over a long period of time and to a marked degree that adversely affects a child's educational

More information

Intervention Strategies to Engage Students and Parents Struggling with School Anxiety School Refusal Patrick McGrath Ph.D Jackie Rhew MA, CADC, LPC

Intervention Strategies to Engage Students and Parents Struggling with School Anxiety School Refusal Patrick McGrath Ph.D Jackie Rhew MA, CADC, LPC Intervention Strategies to Engage Students and Parents Struggling with School Anxiety School Refusal Patrick McGrath Ph.D Jackie Rhew MA, CADC, LPC School Anxiety / School Refusal Program Alexian Brothers

More information

Classical vs. Operant Conditioning

Classical vs. Operant Conditioning Classical vs. Operant Conditioning Operant conditioning (R S RF ) A voluntary response (R) is followed by a reinforcing stimulus (S RF ) The voluntary response is more likely to be emitted by the organism.

More information

Brain Development. Genetic make-up... is not the major determiner

Brain Development. Genetic make-up... is not the major determiner Brain Development Presented by: Linda Alsop SKI-HI Institute Utah State University Genetic make-up... is not the major determiner Early experiences are so powerful that they can completely change the way

More information

AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY An Introduction to MODULE - I 1 AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY As human beings our curiosity drives us to know the reasons behind various events happening around us. Whenever we meet somebody or see someone

More information

Bullying. Take Action Against. stealing money. switching seats in the classroom. spreading rumors. pushing & tripping

Bullying. Take Action Against. stealing money. switching seats in the classroom. spreading rumors. pushing & tripping switching seats in the classroom stealing money Take Action Against Bullying spreading rumors pushing & tripping U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH & HUMAN SERVICES Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration

More information

THIRD MODULE DETERMINERS AND CONDITIONERS OF NON PRESCRIPTIVE DRUGS AND CONSUME.

THIRD MODULE DETERMINERS AND CONDITIONERS OF NON PRESCRIPTIVE DRUGS AND CONSUME. THIRD MODULE DETERMINERS AND CONDITIONERS OF NON PRESCRIPTIVE DRUGS AND CONSUME. INTRODUCTION This module presents different factors which influence in the life of people who consume drugs, there upon

More information

Executive Function Capacities:

Executive Function Capacities: Executive Function Capacities: It s not not knowing what to do; it s knowing to do what you know. Lisa A. Perkins, Psy. D. Westport Public Schools Questions about Your Executive Function Capacities? How

More information

why happiness is good for business

why happiness is good for business why happiness is good for business Martyn Newman Ph.D. Randstad consulting psychologist In today s business world, driven by competitive advantage, success and wealth, happiness can easily be relegated

More information

Using the Second Step: Social-Emotional Skills for Early Learning and Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA-P2) Preschool Program Together

Using the Second Step: Social-Emotional Skills for Early Learning and Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA-P2) Preschool Program Together Using the Second Step: Social-Emotional Skills for Early Learning and Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA-P2) Preschool Program Together The Second Step program and DECA Preschool Program are both

More information

Cognitive Remediation of Brain Injury

Cognitive Remediation of Brain Injury Cognitive Remediation of Brain Injury Amanda Sacks, PhD, ABPP-CN Weill Cornell Medical College Purpose 1. Outline possible cognitive deficits resulting from brain injury 2. Explain the role of cognitive

More information

Case Formulation in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy. What is Case Formulation? Rationale 12/2/2009

Case Formulation in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy. What is Case Formulation? Rationale 12/2/2009 Case Formulation in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy What is Case Formulation? A set of hypotheses regarding what variables serve as causes, triggers, or maintaining factors for a person s problems Description

More information

APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula

APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula http://www.apa.org/ed/natlstandards.html I. METHODS DOMAIN Standard Area IA: Introduction and Research Methods CONTENT STANDARD IA-1: Contemporary

More information

Child Development. Caseworker Core Training Module VII: Child Development: Implications for Family-Centered Child Protective Services

Child Development. Caseworker Core Training Module VII: Child Development: Implications for Family-Centered Child Protective Services Child Development P R E - T R A I N I N G A S S I G N M E N T Caseworker Core Training Module VII: Child Development: Implications for Family-Centered Child Protective Services Developed by the Institute

More information

1of 5. Parental Resilience. Protective & Promotive Factors

1of 5. Parental Resilience. Protective & Promotive Factors Parental Resilience 1of 5 Being a parent can be a very rewarding and joyful experience. But being a parent can also have its share of stress. Parenting stress is caused by the pressures (stressors) that

More information

Post Traumatic Stress Disorder & Substance Misuse

Post Traumatic Stress Disorder & Substance Misuse Post Traumatic Stress Disorder & Substance Misuse Produced and Presented by Dr Derek Lee Consultant Chartered Clinical Psychologist Famous Sufferers. Samuel Pepys following the Great Fire of London:..much

More information

Healing the Invisible Wound. Recovery and Rehabilitation from a Post Traumatic. Stress Injury. By Dr. Amy Menna

Healing the Invisible Wound. Recovery and Rehabilitation from a Post Traumatic. Stress Injury. By Dr. Amy Menna Healing the Invisible Wound Recovery and Rehabilitation from a Post Traumatic Stress Injury By Dr. Amy Menna Post Traumatic Stress Disorder can affect anyone. It is a term used to describe a reaction to

More information

Understanding your ENGAGE Results

Understanding your ENGAGE Results Understanding your ENGAGE Results 2010 by ACT, Inc. All rights reserved. 18349 What is ENGAGE College? ENGAGE College is a survey from ACT that will help you identify your strengths and needs for several

More information

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254 High School Psychology Kansas Course Code # 04254 The American Psychological Association defines Psychology as the study of the mind and behavior. The discipline embraces all aspects of the human experience

More information

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress Chapter Fourteen Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress EMOTIONS! Emotions generally include a Physical component, and a Subjective component or quality, and a Valence Emotions a product of Evolution?

More information

Kids Have Stress Too! Especially at Back to School Time As a Parent, You Can Help!

Kids Have Stress Too! Especially at Back to School Time As a Parent, You Can Help! 1 Kids Have Stress Too! Especially at Back to School Time As a Parent, You Can Help! Stress can infect and affect the physical, emotional, intellectual and academic well being of children. It can interfere

More information

Executive Skills Questionnaire. Peg Dawson & Richard Guare

Executive Skills Questionnaire. Peg Dawson & Richard Guare Executive Skills Questionnaire Peg Dawson & Richard Guare Step I: Read each item below and then rate that item based on the extent to which you agree or disagree with how well it describes you. Use the

More information

It can also be linked to someone s frustration at not being able to express themselves or perform at the level they previously expected.

It can also be linked to someone s frustration at not being able to express themselves or perform at the level they previously expected. The emotional and behavioural effects of Brian Injury can be the most difficult to understand and treat. Many of the people who sustain a brain injury are left with some form of emotional or behavioural

More information

Emotional Intelligence (EI) and Human Synergistics Circumplex Model By: Eric Klisz, M.A. Organizational Development Specialist

Emotional Intelligence (EI) and Human Synergistics Circumplex Model By: Eric Klisz, M.A. Organizational Development Specialist Emotional Intelligence (EI) and Human Synergistics Circumplex Model By: Eric Klisz, M.A. Organizational Development Specialist The following crosswalk is designed to compare and contrast the theoretical

More information

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE Reference: Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.

More information

Anxiety and Education Impact, Recognition & Management Strategies

Anxiety and Education Impact, Recognition & Management Strategies Anxiety and Education Impact, Recognition & Management Strategies Dr Amanda Gamble Centre for Emotional Health (formerly MUARU) Macquarie University, Sydney. WHY SHOULD I BE CONCERNED? 1 Prevalence of

More information

WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD

WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is an anxiety disorder that can occur following the experience or witnessing of a

More information

OTC Abuse. Dr. Eman Said Abd-Elkhalek Lecturer of Pharmacology & Toxicology Faculty of Pharmacy Mansoura University

OTC Abuse. Dr. Eman Said Abd-Elkhalek Lecturer of Pharmacology & Toxicology Faculty of Pharmacy Mansoura University OTC Abuse Dr. Eman Said Abd-Elkhalek Lecturer of Pharmacology & Toxicology Faculty of Pharmacy Mansoura University Opiates Abuse Opioids are a group of natural, partially synthetic, or synthetic drugs

More information

Returning to School After a Concussion: A Fact Sheet for School Professionals

Returning to School After a Concussion: A Fact Sheet for School Professionals Returning to School After a Concussion: A Fact Sheet for School Professionals Assess the situation Be alert for signs and symptoms Contact a health care professional What is a Concussion? A concussion

More information

Approaches to learning (ATL) across the IB continuum

Approaches to learning (ATL) across the IB continuum Approaches to learning (ATL) across the IB continuum Through approaches to learning in IB programmes, students develop skills that have relevance across the curriculum that help them learn how to learn.

More information

Handout: Risk. Predisposing factors in children include: Genetic Influences

Handout: Risk. Predisposing factors in children include: Genetic Influences Handout: Risk The more risk factors to which a child is exposed the greater their vulnerability to mental health problems. Risk does not cause mental health problems but it is cumulative and does predispose

More information

The Georgia Early Care and Education Professional Development Competencies

The Georgia Early Care and Education Professional Development Competencies Page 1 The Georgia Early Care and Education Professional Development Competencies Early Care and Education Professional School-Age Care Professional Program Administrator Trainer Technical Assistance Provider

More information

The Social Cognitive perspective and Albert Bandura

The Social Cognitive perspective and Albert Bandura LP 11D Social Cog/Trait 1 The Social Cognitive perspective and Albert Bandura For more information on Albert Bandura and the Social Cognitive Perspective, see Chapter 5: Learning and Chapter 8: Motivation

More information

Arkansas State PIRC/ Center for Effective Parenting

Arkansas State PIRC/ Center for Effective Parenting Increasing Your Child s Motivation to Learn In order to be successful in school and to learn, students must stay involved in the learning process. This requires students to do many different activities

More information

Role Plays for Teacher Classroom Management

Role Plays for Teacher Classroom Management Role Plays for Teacher Classroom Management Collaboration, Workshop 1 After Vignette #4 Making connection with quiet child Practice making a connection with a child who is quiet, shy, or sad. Think about

More information

DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY

DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY COURSE DESCRIPTION AP Psychology engages students in a rigorous appraisal of many facets of our current understanding of psychology. The course is based on the

More information

INDIVIDUAL CHANGE Learning and the process of change in what ways can models of

INDIVIDUAL CHANGE Learning and the process of change in what ways can models of INDIVIDUAL CHANGE Learning and the process of change in what ways can models of learning help us understand individual change? The behavioural approach to change how can we change people s behaviour? The

More information

EARLY CHILDHOOD ENVIRONMENT RATING SCALE REVISED (ECERS-R) Statements of Developmentally Appropriate Practice

EARLY CHILDHOOD ENVIRONMENT RATING SCALE REVISED (ECERS-R) Statements of Developmentally Appropriate Practice EARLY CHILDHOOD ENVIRONMENT RATING SCALE REVISED (ECERS-R) Statements of Developmentally Appropriate Practice SPACE AND FURNISHINGS 1. Indoor space Children need sufficient space that is well lit and has

More information

Treatment Planning. The Key to Effective Client Documentation. Adapted from OFMQ s 2002 provider training.

Treatment Planning. The Key to Effective Client Documentation. Adapted from OFMQ s 2002 provider training. Treatment Planning The Key to Effective Client Documentation Adapted from OFMQ s 2002 provider training. 1 Models Medical Model Diagnosis Psych Eval Problems Symptoms Psycho/Social/Behavioral Model Diagnosis

More information

Singapore Kids and Video games: Findings of a 3-year longitudinal study

Singapore Kids and Video games: Findings of a 3-year longitudinal study Singapore Kids and Video games: Findings of a 3-year longitudinal study Dr Angeline Khoo Associate Professor Psychological Studies, National Institute of Education Definition of Pathological Video Gaming

More information

Session 2. Brain Development; Ages and Stages; Comfort and Calming. Nature vs. Nurture

Session 2. Brain Development; Ages and Stages; Comfort and Calming. Nature vs. Nurture Session 2 Brain Development; Ages and Stages; Comfort and Calming Nature vs. Nurture The debate of whether children are more influenced by their genetics (nature) or the way they are parented (nurture)

More information

The MSPA Rubric for Evaluation of School Psychologists

The MSPA Rubric for Evaluation of School Psychologists The MSPA Rubric for Evaluation of School Psychologists The Massachusetts School Psychologists Association (MSPA) has developed this rubric for the evaluation of school psychologists for adoption, in part

More information

Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders

Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders S H A N A M. H A T Z O P O U L O S G E O R G E W A S H I N G T O N U N I V E R S I T Y S P E D 2 0 1 S U M M E R 2 0 1 0 Overview of Emotional and Behavioral

More information

USVH Disease of the Week #1: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

USVH Disease of the Week #1: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) USVH Disease of the Week #1: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Effects of Traumatic Experiences A National Center for PTSD Fact Sheet By: Eve B. Carlson, Ph.D. and Josef Ruzek, Ph.D. When people find

More information

A STATE CALL TO ACTION: Working to End Child Abuse and Neglect In Massachusetts

A STATE CALL TO ACTION: Working to End Child Abuse and Neglect In Massachusetts A STATE CALL TO ACTION: Working to End Child Abuse and Neglect In Massachusetts Massachusetts Citizens for Children A Massachusetts Kids Count Report, April 2001 CHAPTER 15: The Role of Schools in the

More information

SELF-REGULATION. Keys to Understanding and Help

SELF-REGULATION. Keys to Understanding and Help SELF-REGULATION Students with learning disabilities and/or Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (AD/HD) often have difficulty with managing their behaviour. They need to recognize, channel and manage

More information

Compensation Strategies for Common Challenges after Traumatic Brain Injury

Compensation Strategies for Common Challenges after Traumatic Brain Injury Compensation Strategies for Common Challenges after Traumatic Brain Injury Annette Pearson MS, LADC, CBIS Vinland National Center apearson@vinlandcenter.org 1 This is any one of us, with some area(s) of

More information

Family Engagement and Ongoing Child Assessment

Family Engagement and Ongoing Child Assessment Family Engagement and Ongoing Child Assessment The partnership between parents and Head Start staff is fundamental to children s current and future success and their readiness for school. This relationship

More information

RESTORATIVE TECHNIQUES IN COGNITIVE REHABILITATION: PROGRAM DESIGN AND CLINICAL BENEFITS

RESTORATIVE TECHNIQUES IN COGNITIVE REHABILITATION: PROGRAM DESIGN AND CLINICAL BENEFITS RESTORATIVE TECHNIQUES IN COGNITIVE REHABILITATION: PROGRAM DESIGN AND CLINICAL BENEFITS In the treatment of traumatic brain injury, cognitive rehabilitation is an intervention that seeks to improve cognitive

More information

The Executive Teacher Program

The Executive Teacher Program The Executive Teacher Program Introduction Adam J. Cox, Ph.D. Can We Afford To Wait? There is a deep chasm between new science in psychology, neurology, and psychiatry, and the people who can most benefit

More information

GUIDELINES FOR THE IEP TEAM DATA COLLECTION &

GUIDELINES FOR THE IEP TEAM DATA COLLECTION & GUIDELINES FOR THE IEP TEAM DATA COLLECTION & Progress Monitoring Decisions about the effectiveness of an intervention must be based on data, not guesswork. Frequent, repeated measures of progress toward

More information

Executive Function and Student Success Karen Boutelle Associate Director karenboutelle@landmark.edu

Executive Function and Student Success Karen Boutelle Associate Director karenboutelle@landmark.edu Executive Function and Student Success Karen Boutelle Associate Director karenboutelle@landmark.edu What do successful students do? Learn academic content And This workshop will help you: Explore and develop

More information

Down Syndrome Students and their School Placement

Down Syndrome Students and their School Placement Down Syndrome Students and their School Placement Ashley Gobel 11/1/2010 EDEC 420 1 Abstract Introduction Down syndrome is one of the most common genetic disorders with reports of one infant in every 600-800

More information

What Does It Mean for Students to Be Engaged?

What Does It Mean for Students to Be Engaged? 1 What Does It Mean for Students to Be Engaged? Teachers are constantly working to connect their students to school and to learning because they know that engagement is crucial to school success. It may

More information

How To Understand Internet Addiction In The Netherlands

How To Understand Internet Addiction In The Netherlands Internet addiction in the Netherlands a brief overview BELSPO Brussels March 19, 2012 The colleagues Tim Schoenmakers Tony van Rooij IVO research on internet addiction Some publications: Meerkerk, G.,

More information

Aristotle described 3 types of Friendship. 1. Utility 2. Pleasure 3. True Friendship

Aristotle described 3 types of Friendship. 1. Utility 2. Pleasure 3. True Friendship Aristotle described 3 types of Friendship 1. Utility 2. Pleasure 3. True Friendship At what Age do children have the cognitive and emotional abilities required to have a friendship? Children s perceptions

More information

Checklist: Timeline for Assessment (Sample) Early Childhood Classes: Oaklawn School

Checklist: Timeline for Assessment (Sample) Early Childhood Classes: Oaklawn School Sept. 1 14 Sept. 15 30 Oct.1 15 Oct.16 31 Nov. 1 15 Checklist: Timeline for Assessment (Sample) Early Childhood Classes: Oaklawn School Social Attributes Checklist (Existing/Ready-made) Williams s Motor

More information

NUCDF. So what do I do now? Today I will talk about: KEY POINTS: FUN

NUCDF. So what do I do now? Today I will talk about: KEY POINTS: FUN So what do I do now? Neuropsychological evaluation results in Urea Cycle disorders and what to do about them Susan Waisbren, PhD Lead Psychologist, Longitudinal Study Boston Children s Hospital & Harvard

More information

General Psychology 201A Syllabus PCC, Cascade Campus, Fall 2006

General Psychology 201A Syllabus PCC, Cascade Campus, Fall 2006 General Psychology 201A Syllabus PCC, Cascade Campus, Fall 2006 This syllabus is a written contract between each student and myself, your instructor. Please read it carefully and ask me if anything needs

More information

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Dr. Sare Akdag, RPsych

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Dr. Sare Akdag, RPsych Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Dr. Sare Akdag, RPsych Most people living with epilepsy do not experience serious problems with their thinking. However, there are aspects of thinking that can be affected

More information

CORE-INFO: Emotional neglect and emotional abuse in pre-school children

CORE-INFO: Emotional neglect and emotional abuse in pre-school children CORE-INFO: Emotional neglect and emotional abuse in pre-school children Introduction This leaflet summarises what is currently known about children aged less than six years who have been emotionally neglected

More information

Adolescent Development: What Do Our Students Need to Succeed? Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Spectrum Services

Adolescent Development: What Do Our Students Need to Succeed? Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Spectrum Services Adolescent Development: What Do Our Students Need to Succeed? Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Spectrum Services Important Areas of Adolescent Development Social Executive functioning Academic Personality Independence

More information

Case Study: Jane Dhillon. 2. Why do I think Jane has a non-verbal learning disability?

Case Study: Jane Dhillon. 2. Why do I think Jane has a non-verbal learning disability? Case Study: Jane Dhillon 1. Background information on Jane Dhillon 2. Why do I think Jane has a non-verbal learning disability? Three issues in this course that relate to Jane: 3. Memory 4. Peer relationships

More information

Emotionally Disturbed. Questions from Parents

Emotionally Disturbed. Questions from Parents 1 Emotionally Disturbed Questions from Parents Characteristics that may be reflective of ED:* an inability to learn which cannot be explained by intellectual, sensory, or health factors. an inability to

More information

Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06

Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06 Course Information: HCS Curriculum: Social Studies 6 12 Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06 Course Title: Psychology, IB Grade Level: 11-12 Course Description: This course

More information

The Role of Executive Functions in Childhood Learning and Behavior. George McCloskey. Plain Talk About Reading February 9-11, 2015 New Orleans

The Role of Executive Functions in Childhood Learning and Behavior. George McCloskey. Plain Talk About Reading February 9-11, 2015 New Orleans The Role of in Childhood Learning and Behavior George McCloskey Plain Talk About Reading February 9-11, 2015 New Orleans About the Presenter George McCloskey George McCloskey, Ph.D., is a professor and

More information

A GUIDE TO MAKING CHILD-FOCUSED PARENTING TIME DECISIONS

A GUIDE TO MAKING CHILD-FOCUSED PARENTING TIME DECISIONS A GUIDE TO MAKING CHILD-FOCUSED PARENTING TIME DECISIONS 2 Prepared by The Court Services Advisory Committee of the Maine District Court This booklet is based on information from the Minnesota Supreme

More information

Standards for Certification in Early Childhood Education [26.110-26.270]

Standards for Certification in Early Childhood Education [26.110-26.270] I.B. SPECIFIC TEACHING FIELDS Standards for Certification in Early Childhood Education [26.110-26.270] STANDARD 1 Curriculum The competent early childhood teacher understands and demonstrates the central

More information

Contact Information. Rebecca.cain@state.sd.us Phone 773-3678

Contact Information. Rebecca.cain@state.sd.us Phone 773-3678 Contact Information Rebecca.cain@state.sd.us Phone 773-3678 Learning Behavior/Changing Behavior Students learn any behavior in the same way they learn to read through instruction/modeling, practice, feedback,

More information

Registered Charity No. 5365

Registered Charity No. 5365 THE MULTIPLE SCLEROSIS SOCIETY OF IRELAND Dartmouth House, Grand Parade, Dublin 6. Telephone: (01) 269 4599. Fax: (01) 269 3746 MS Helpline: 1850 233 233 E-mail: mscontact@ms-society.ie www.ms-society.ie

More information

How does mental illness interfere with school performance?

How does mental illness interfere with school performance? How does mental illness interfere with school performance? Mental illnesses may interfere with functioning in different ways. Often, the person themselves or the professionals working with them can describe

More information

HELPING CHILDREN COPE WITH STRESS

HELPING CHILDREN COPE WITH STRESS CENTER FOR EFFECTIVE PARENTING HELPING CHILDREN COPE WITH STRESS Stress is a natural and normal part of children's lives. The process of growing up carries with it quite a bit of stress. Children begin

More information

School Refusal Behavior: Children Who Can't or Won't Go To School

School Refusal Behavior: Children Who Can't or Won't Go To School Questions from chapter 1 School Refusal Behavior: Children Who Can't or Won't Go To School 1) A child-motivated absence is referred to by all the following EXCEPT a) Truancy b) School withdrawal c) School

More information

Psychiatrists should be aware of the signs of Asperger s Syndrome as they appear in adolescents and adults if diagnostic errors are to be avoided.

Psychiatrists should be aware of the signs of Asperger s Syndrome as they appear in adolescents and adults if diagnostic errors are to be avoided. INFORMATION SHEET Age Group: Sheet Title: Adults Depression or Mental Health Problems People with Asperger s Syndrome are particularly vulnerable to mental health problems such as anxiety and depression,

More information

Assessing families and treating trauma in substance abusing families

Assessing families and treating trauma in substance abusing families Children, Trauma and the impact of Substance abuse Day One Outpatient (874-1045) Amy Stevenson LCPC CCS (amys@day-one.org) Don Burke LCPC CCS (donb@day-one.org) Assessing families and treating trauma in

More information

CALIFORNIA S TEACHING PERFORMANCE EXPECTATIONS (TPE)

CALIFORNIA S TEACHING PERFORMANCE EXPECTATIONS (TPE) CALIFORNIA S TEACHING PERFORMANCE EXPECTATIONS (TPE) The Teaching Performance Expectations describe the set of knowledge, skills, and abilities that California expects of each candidate for a Multiple

More information

Conducting Emotionally Difficult Conversations. John Banja, PhD Center For Ethics Emory University jbanja@emory.edu

Conducting Emotionally Difficult Conversations. John Banja, PhD Center For Ethics Emory University jbanja@emory.edu Conducting Emotionally Difficult Conversations John Banja, PhD Center For Ethics Emory University jbanja@emory.edu How To Break Bad News Robert Buckman (with contributions by Yvonne Kason). Baltimore:

More information

FLORIDA EARLY LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENTAL STANDARDS: Birth to Five

FLORIDA EARLY LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENTAL STANDARDS: Birth to Five FLORIDA EARLY LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENTAL STANDARDS: Birth to Five The Florida Early Learning and Developmental Standards were created by using multiple resources, of which, one was the Alaska Guidelines.

More information

Child s Developmental Stages: A Challenge to Relevancy and Curriculum Development in Children s Church

Child s Developmental Stages: A Challenge to Relevancy and Curriculum Development in Children s Church Child s Developmental Stages: A Challenge to Relevancy and Curriculum Development in Children s Church DR. K. A. KORB FACULTY OF EDUCATION U NIVERSITY OF JOS Let the little children come to me, and do

More information