Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages"

Transcription

1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1998, Vol. 4, No. 4, X/98/$3.00 Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages Brad J. Bushman Iowa State University Three experiments tested whether television violence impairs memory for commercials. Participants saw commercials embedded in violent or nonviolent film clips. The film clips were similar on other dimensions that might influence commercial memory (e.g., arousal and involvement). After viewing a film clip, participants completed several recall and recognition memory tests. In all experiments, participants who saw a violent film clip had poorer memory for commercials than did participants who saw a nonviolent film clip. In Experiment 3, participants also reported their mood after viewing a film clip but before completing the memory tests. Anger mediated between television violence and commercial memory. Television violence increased anger, and anger, in turn, impaired memory for commercials. These results suggest that sponsoring violent programs might not be a profitable venture for advertisers. Since its introduction to American society, television has become an integral part of nearly every home. It is estimated that there are more television sets in the United States than there are toilets. Over 98% of American homes have at least one television set (American Psychological Association, 1993). In the United States, children spend more time watching television than they spend at school, and adults spend more time watching television than they spend on any other activity except sleeping and working (Huston et al., 1992). Much of what is shown on television is violent. The National Television Violence Study I would like to thank Len Berkowitz, Bill Hoyt, Rick Gibbons, Gary Wells, Jim Weaver, and Dolf Zillmann for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article, Dan Russell for his assistance with the Linear Structural Relations analyses, and Ken Koehler for his assistance with the log-linear analyses. I also extend appreciation to Eric Seelau, Angela Stack, and Stacey Swaim for their help with the s and to Joy Cart, Andrea Determan, Shawn Peters, David Laidig, Lori McGonigal, Sarah Peters, Angela Stack, and Dominic Starr for serving as experimenters. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Brad J. Bushman, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa Electronic mall may be sent to bushman@iastate.edu. (1996, 1997) conducted a detailed content analysis of about 4,500 hr of programming on cable and broadcast television. The results showed that about 60% of the programs contained violence. By the time the average child graduates from elementary school, he or she will have seen more than 8,000 murders and more than 100,000 other assorted acts of violence on television (Huston et al., 1992). Surveys indicate that most Americans believe that there is too much violence on television (e.g., Fischer, 1994; Zipperer, 1994). In one survey (TV Guide, 1992), for example, people were asked to say in their own words what made them angry about television programming. The most common complaint was "too much violence." If most people object to the amount of violence shown on television, then why is there so much violence portrayed on television? Television industry leaders answer this question by claiming that their programs merely reflect the violence that already exists in society. For example, the television movie producer Jim Green said, "Murder happens. It happened a lot in Shakespeare" (Drevitch, 1994, p. 12). The amount of violence shown on television, however, far exceeds the amount of violence found in the real world. Even in reality-based police television programs, rio- 291

2 292 BUSHMAN lence is grossly overemphasized (e.g., Oliver, 1994). A second possible answer to this question is that advertisers assume violent programs will attract larger audiences and will therefore gain more exposure for their products. However, the available scientific evidence does not support the idea that gratuitous displays of violence will draw more viewers. Television violence neither increases nor decreases the popularity of a program in the United States (e.g., Diener & DeFour, 1978; Diener & Woody, 1981; Sprafkin, Rubinstein, & Stone, 1977). 1 Alternatively, advertisers might want to target certain audiences who have a preference for violent programming. Regardless of the reason for the excessive amount of violence on television, one important question is whether violent content influences memory for commercial messages. A commercial may be interesting, enjoyable, or persuasive, but it may not be effective if the potential buyer cannot remember the brand advertised or the commercial message. Unfortunately, the question of whether television violence influences memory for commercials is difficult to answer because of a dearth of empirical evidence on the topic. The few studies that have been conducted on this topic have confounded violence with other variables that influence memory for commercials, such as arousal. Many violent programs are action-adventure type programs. Previous research has shown that arousing programs impair attention to, as well as processing and storage of, commercial messages (e.g., Mundorf, Zillmann, & Drew, 1991; Pavelchak, Antil, & Munch, 1988; Singh & Churchill, 1987). The present research is interested in determining whether violent programs influence memory for commercial messages when the arousal level of programs is held constant. There are theoretical reasons for predicting that emotional responses other than arousal mediate the effect of television violence on memory for commercial messages. There is a large body of research evidence indicating that mood can affect memory (e.g., Bower, 1981; Burke, Heuer, & Reisberg, 1992; Christiansen, 1992; Mayer, McCormick, & Strong, 1995). Previous research has shown that violent programs put viewers in a bad mood and especially make them angry (e.g., Anderson, 1997; Bushman, 1995; Bushman & Geen, 1990; Hansen & Hansen, 1990). There are at least two reasons why anger might interfere with memory for commercials. First, the mood state induced by a television program can prime or activate similarly toned material stored in memory. Berkowitz (1984) has suggested that "the aggressive ideas suggested by a violent movie can prime other semantically related thoughts, heightening the chances that viewers will have other aggressive ideas in this period" (p. 411). Berkowitz bases this priming hypothesis on the concept of spreading activation within a network in memory (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975): Thoughts send out radiating activation along associative pathways, thereby activating other related thoughts. In this way, ideas about aggression that are not identical to those observed in the media may be elicited by the latter. In addition, thoughts are linked, along the same sort of associative lines, not only to other thoughts but also to emotional reactions (Bower, 1981; Isen, Clark, Shalker, & Karp, 1978). Thus, the anger induced by a violent program could activate other hostility-related ideas that interfere with the elaboration of the information contained in the commercials, rendering the commercials less accessible. Mood management theory provides a second reason why anger might interfere with memory for commercial messages (Isen, 1984; Parrott, 1993; Schwarz & Clore, 1983). Viewers in a program-induced negative mood should engage in processes to terminate or improve their negative mood, whereas viewers in a programinduced positive mood should engage in processes to prolong their positive mood. Because mood repair processes require more effort than do mood maintenance processes, viewers in a negative mood might focus more attention on themselves and less attention on their environment, including the commercials (Wood, Saltzberg, & Goldsamt, 1990). Thus, anger induced by a violent television program could lead viewers to focus more attention on repairing their mood than on the commercials, rendering the commercials less accessible. The present research does not, however, explicitly test these theories. The first two experiments in this article tested 1Similar results have been reported in countries other than the United States, such as Finland (Mustonen, 1997; Mustonen & Pulkkinen, 1993) and Britain (Wober, 1988).

3 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 293 whether television violence impairs memory for commercial messages. The third experiment tested whether program-induced anger mediates the effect of television violence on memory for commercial messages. Experiment 1 Experiment 1 tested the effects of television violence on recall of brand names and commercial messages. Male and female college students were randomly assigned to watch either a violent or nonviolent film clip that contained two commercials. The violent and nonviolent film clips were preselected because they did not differ on measures of self-reported arousal (i.e., exciting, boring, and arousing ratings) or on measures of physiological arousal (i.e., systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, mean arterial pressure, and heart rate). Immediately after viewing the, participants recalled brand names of products in the commercials and recalled details in the commercial messages. After completing the recall measures, participants reported the number of hours per week they spent watching various types of television programs, including violent programs. This measure was taken to ensure that violent content of films rather than habitual exposure to televised violence was affecting responses to commercials. Participants also reported whether they had seen the film from which the clip had been taken and whether they had seen the commercials imbedded in the film clip. Participants who saw the nonviolent video were expected to have greater recall of brands and commercial messages than were participants who saw the violent video. Both men and women were included in Experiment 1 to increase the generalizability of the results, but no sex differences were predicted for either dependent variable. Participants Method Participants were 200 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology classes (100 men and 100 women) who received extra course credit in exchange for their voluntary participation. Videotapes The s for Experiment 1 were preselected to be differentially violent but equally exciting and arousing. The fouowing 2 s were chosen from a pool of 52 tapes that were each about 15-min long. Each tape was recorded from a commercial or cable television channel. Karate Kid III (violent). In a karate tournament, a young man defeats an arrogant opponent who repeatedly violates the rules by fighting dirty. Prior to the tournament, the opponent had insulted the young man, kicked the young man's girlfriend in the stomach, and destroyed the young man's property. Gorillas in the Mist (nonviolent). A scientist observes gorillas in their natural habitat. At first, the gorillas do not trust the scientist. In time, however, the gorillas begin to trust the scientist and they allow her to interact with them. Although this film clip was intense and emotionally moving, it contained no violence. A total of 2,080 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology classes acted as judges. Each was rated by 40 judges (20 men and 20 women). The judges filled out a set of 11-point rating scales indicating the degree to which the video was exciting, boring, arousing, and violent. Some other rating dimensions were added as fillers. The scales were anchored at one end by 0 (not at all) and at the other end by 10 (extremely). Exciting, boring, and arousing ratings were used to measure the arousal content of the tapes, and violence ratings were used to measure the violent content of the tapes. Judges also reported whether they had seen the movie from which the clip was taken; this measure was used as a covariate in all analyses. A cover sheet explained that the questionnaires were part of a media evaluation study being conducted jointly with the Department of Journalism and Mass Communication. There were no differences between the Karate Kid III and Gorillas in the Mist s on any of the self-reported arousal measures (see Table 1). As expected, the Karate Kid III was judged to be more violent (M = 7.2, SE = 0.42) than the Gorillas in the Mist (M = 2.2, SE = 0.39), F(1, 77) = 76.89, p <.05, d = In previous research using these two s (Bushman, 1995), higher levels of anger

4 294 BUSHMAN Table 1 Comparison of the Violent and Nonviolent Videotapes Used in Experiments 1 and 2 on Self.Report and Physiological Measures of Arousal Violent Nonviolent Measure M SE M SE Self-reported arousal Exciting Boring Arousing Physiological arousal Systolic blood pressure Diastolic blood pressure Mean arterial pressure Heart rate Note. Means were adjusted for whether judges had seen the film from which the was taken. Ratings were made using an 11-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). The violent video was taken from Karate Kid III and the nonviolent video was taken from Gorillas in the Mist (see video descriptions in tex0. N = 40 (20 men and 20 women) for self-reported arousal measures. N = 296 (148 men and 148 women) for physiological arousal measures. and aggressive behavior were found in participants who saw the Karate Kid III than in participants who saw the Gorillas in the Mist. The s did not differ, however, in their effects on systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, mean arterial pressure, and heart rate (see Table 1). The cardiovascular measures are consistent with the self-report measures of arousal content (see Table 1). Each consisted of three segments (each about 5 rain long) that maintained a coherent story line. One commercial was inserted between the first and second film segments and the other commercial was inserted between the second and third film segments. The commercials were for Krazy Glue and Wisk laundry detergent. Both commercials advertised products with broad market appeal, were 30 s long, and were several years old. The Krazy Glue commercial depicts a man hanging in the air from a construction beam, kicking his legs and trying to get down. The man is wearing a helmet that is fastened to the construction beam with Krazy Glue. In the foreground is a table containing several fragile objects the man fixed using Krazy Glue. A woman is shown breaking the objects on the table, apparently because she is mad at the man for breaking a promise he had made to her. The commercial ends with the announcer stating, "Krazy Glue, for things that are broken, except promises." The Wisk commercial shows people getting their clothes dirty in a variety of ways (e.g., children are shown jumping in mud puddles, chasing pigs in the dirt, and spilling food and beverages on their clothes). Meanwhile, the jingle "Tisk, Tisk, Tisk, Wisk, Wisk" plays in the background. A piece of white fabric, stained with the letters TSK, is shown coming clean after it is washed with Wisk detergent. The commercial ends with the announcer stating, "Advanced action Wisk dissolves adhesive bonds that lock dirt and stains onto your clothes. To clean a whole wash load of risks." The order of the commercials was counterbalanced so that half of the participants saw the Wisk commercial first and the Krazy Glue commercial second, whereas the other half saw the commercials in the reverse order. To enhance the realism of the, a network identification message was inserted prior to each commercial. Procedure Participants were tested individually. They were told that the study was part of a National Consumer Research Project to evaluate clips of films that had recently been shown on television. Participants were randomly assigned to watch either the violent or nonviolent d film clip. Immediately after the showing of the, participants listed the names of brands advertised in the commercials and listed everything they could remember about the commercial messages. After completing the recall measures, participants reported the number of hours per week they spent watching various types of television programs, including violent programs. They also reported whether they had seen the film from which the clip had been taken and whether they had seen the commercials imbedded within the film clip. Finally, participants were questioned for suspicion and fully debriefed. No participant expressed suspicion about the study.

5 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 295 Results Order, Commercial, and Sex Effects The presentation order of the two commercials did not influence either dependent variable. There were no differences between the two commercials on either dependent variable. There also were no sex main effects or interactions for either dependent variable. Thus, the data for the two orders, two commercials, and two sexes were combined for subsequent analyses. Exposure Effects Habitual exposure to televised violence and previous exposure to the film and commercials shown in the were not related to either one of the dependent variables, either alone or interacting with film content. The exposure variables were therefore excluded from the analyses. Reliability of Coding Recall Measures Two independent raters who were unaware of the conditions and experimental hypotheses coded the free recall test scores. Because the intraclass correlation coefficient (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) was quite large (ricc =.86), scores from the two raters were averaged for subsequent analyses. Effects of Television Violence on Commercial Recall Brand name recall. Participants who watched the nonviolent video recalled more brands than did participants who watched the violent, F(1, 198) = 7.85, p <.05, d = 0.40 (see Table 2). 2 Commercial message recall. On the free recall test, participants who watched the nonviolent video recalled more details in the commercials than did participants who watched the violent, F(1,198) = 10.98, p <.05, d = 0.47 (see Table 2). Discussion As expected, television violence impaired memory for commercials. Participants who watched the violent video recalled fewer brand Table 2 Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages (Experiment 1) Violent Nonviolent Measure M SE M SE Brand name recall Commercial message recall Note. Brand name recall scores could range from 0 (no brands recalled) to 2 (both brands recalled). Commercial message recall scores were continuous (scores ranged from 0 to 22). names and commercial message details than did participants who watched the nonviolent. It is unlikely that these memory deficts were due to differential arousal levels produced by the films. The violent and nonviolent s used in Experiment 1 were preselected because they did not differ on self-report or physiological measures of arousal (see Table 1). Experiment 2 Experiment 2 was conducted to replicate and extend the results of Experiment 1. Experiment 2 included an additional memory measure--visual brand recognition. A brand choice decision made in a store may need only a recognition level of learning because the choices may be reviewed at the time a decision is made (Singh & Rothschild, 1983). Even when a consumer makes a shopping list of products in the home, the final brand selection generally occurs in the store (Betmaan, 1979). Experiment 2 also included some manipulation check items. It is well known that active rehearsal facilitates encoding and later retrieval of stimuli (Craik & Lockhart, 1972), including commercial messages. Previous research has shown that if viewers are caught up in an involving television program, they are more likely to rehearse the program than the commercial message (Bryant & Comisky, 1978). Commercial interruptions in high involvement programs might also distract and annoy viewers (Worchel, Hardy, & Hurley, 2Although brand recall scores only ranged from 0 to 2, log-linear analyses yielded the same pattern of results, X2(2, N = 200) = 8.04, p <.05.

6 296 BUSHMAN 1976). One cannot rule out involvement level as an alternative explanation of the results of Experiment 1 because the s were not rated on an involvement dimension. In Expe~ent 2, participants rated how involving the film clip was and how distracting and annoying the commercials were. In Experiment 2, male and female college students were randomly assigned to violent or nonviolent conditions. Immediately after viewing the, participants recalled brand names, recalled commercial message details, and recognized advertised brands from competitor brands. Participants reported how involving the film clip was and how distracting and annoying the commercials were. Scores on all three dependent variables (i.e., brand recognition, brand recall, and commercial message recall) were expected to be higher in the nonviolent condition than in the violent condition. Participants Method Participants were 200 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology classes (100 men and 100 women) who received extra course credit in exchange for their voluntary participation. Procedure Participants were tested in small groups, but they worked independently on tasks. The cover story and s were the same as those used in Experiment 1. Immediately after the showing of the, participants were given an unexpected free recall test. Participants listed the names of brands advertised and listed everything they could remember about the commercial messages. Participants then completed a visual brand recognition test. Participants were told that the film clip contained two commercials, one for a brand of glue and the other for a brand of laundry detergent. Participants were shown slides of 6 glue brands (Krazy Glue and 5 distractor brands) and 6 laundry detergent brands (Wisk and 5 distractor brands) and were asked to write the brands advertised in the commercials. The order of the products and the order of brands within each product were randomized for each session. The slides were shown at the rate of one slide every 5 s. Next, participants reported the number of hours per week they spent watching various types of television programs, including violent programs. Participants also reported whether they had seen the film from which the clip had been taken and whether they had seen the commercials imbedded within the film clip. Participants also completed the following manipulation check items: (a) "The film clip was involving," (b) "The laundry detergent commercial was distracting," (c) "The laundry detergent commercial was annoying," (d) "The glue commercial was distracting," and (e) "The glue commercial was annoying." The statements were rated using a 10-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 10 (strongly agree). The first statement was included to test whether the Karate Kid III and Gorillas in the Mist s differed in their involvement level. The remaining four statements were included to test whether commercial interruptions were more distracting and annoying when they were embedded in the violent film clip than when they were embedded in the nonviolent film clip. Finally, participants were questioned for suspicion and fully debriefed. No participant expressed suspicion about the study. Results Order, Commercial, and Sex Effects The presentation order of the two commercials did not influence any of the dependent variables or manipulation check items. There were no differences between the two commercials on any of the dependent variables or manipulation check items. There also were no sex main effects or interactions for any of the dependent variables or manipulation check items. Thus, the data for the two orders, two commercials, and two sexes were combined for subsequent analyses. Exposure Effects Habitual exposure to televised violence and previous exposure to the film and commercials shown in the were not significantly related to any of the dependent variables or manipulation check items, either alone or interacting with film content. The exposure variables were therefore excluded from the analyses.

7 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 297 Film Clip Differences in Involvement Ratings There was no difference between the violent and nonviolent s in involvement ratings, F(1, 198) = 1.44, p >.05 (see Table 3). Thus, any effects of media violence on commercial recall cannot be attributed to differential levels of viewer involvement in the violent and nonviolent conditions. Film clip involvement ratings also did not influence any of the dependent variables, either alone or interacting with film content. The film involvement variable was therefore excluded from the analyses. Effects of Television Violence on Distracting and Annoying Ratings of Commercials There were no differences between the violent and nonviolent s in ratings of how distracting and annoying the commercials were, Fs(1, 198) = 1.39 and 1.98, respectively, ps >.05 (see Table 3). Thus, commercial interruptions did not distract and annoy viewers more in the violent condition than in the nonviolent conditions. Distracting and annoying ratings of commercials also did not influence any of the dependent variables, either alone or interacting with film content. The distracting and annoying variables were therefore excluded from the analyses. Reliability of Coding Message Recall Two independent raters who were unaware of conditions and experimental hypotheses coded Table 3 Effects of Television Violence on Manipulation Check Items (Experiment 2) Violent Nonviolent Measure M SE M SE Film was involving Commercials were annoying Commercials were distracting Note. Ratings were made using a 10-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 10 (strongly agree). Table 4 Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages (Experiment 2) Violent Nonviolent Measure M SE M SE Brand name recognition Brand name recall Commercial message recall Note. Brand name recognition and recall scores could range from 0 to 2. Commercial message recall scores were continuous (scores ranged from 1 to 17). the free recall data. Because the intraclass correlation coefficient (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) was large (ricc =.95), scores from the two raters were averaged for subsequent analyses. Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages Brand name recognition. Participants who watched the nonviolent video recognized more brands from the slides than did participants who watched the violent, F(1, 198) = 6.48, p <.05, d = 0.36 (see Table 4). 3 Brand name recall. Participants who watched the nonviolent video recalled more brands than did participants who watched the violent, F(1, 198) = 15.70,p <.05, d = 0.56 (see Table 4). 4 Commercial message recall. Although the effect was in the predicted direction, content did not significantly influence recall of commercial message details, F(1, 198) = 3.17, p >.05, d = 0.25 (see Table 4). Discussion Experiment 2 replicated and extended the results from Experiment 1. Participants who watched the violent video recalled fewer brand 3Although brand recognition scores only ranged from 0 to 2, log-linear analyses yielded the same pattern of results. However, the effect was not quite significant, 2(2, N = 200) = 5.76, p <.06. 4Although brand recall scores only ranged from 0 to 2, log-linear analyses yielded the same pattern of results, 2(2, N = 200) = 12.63, p <.05.

8 298 BUSHMAN names than did participants who had watched the nonviolent. Participants who watched the violent video, in comparison to those who watched the nonviolent video, were less likely to recognize the advertised brands from the competitor brands. The effect was in the same direction for recall of commercial message details, although it was not quite significant. It is unlikely that these memory deficits were due to involvement level because the violent and nonviolent s did not differ in terms of how involving they were judged to be. In addition, the amount of violence in the program did not influence perceptions of how distracting and annoying the commercials were. Experiment 3 The primary purpose of Experiment 3 was to determine whether anger mediates the effect of television violence on memory for commercial messages. Arousal and positive affect also were tested as potential mediators. Testing both anger and positive affect allows one to determine whether anger impairs memory for commercials or whether positive affect improves memory for commercials. Arousal was also tested to determine whether generalized arousal or emotions specific to aggression had a stronger mediating influence on the relation between television violence and memory for commercials. A second purpose of Experiment 3 was to replicate the results from Experiments 1 and 2, using a broader set of programs and commercials. Although the violent and nonviolent s used in Experiments 1 and 2 appear to be similar on a number of important dimensions (e.g., self-reported arousal, cardiovascular arousal, and involvement level), only one exemplar of each type of was used. Thus, it is possible that differences between the violent and nonviolent s are due to features of the s other than the amount of violence they contain. In Experiment 3, four exemplars of each type of were used. Although no differences were found between the two commercials used in Experiments 1 and 2, a different commercial also was used in Experiment 3 to increase the generalizability of results. In Experiment 3, male and female college students were randomly assigned to watch either a violent or nonviolent film clip that contained two commercials. There were four violent film clips and four nonviolent film clips that were preselected to be similar on measures of selfreported arousal (i.e., exciting, boring, and arousing ratings). Immediately after viewing the, participants reported their mood, recalled brand names, recalled commercial message details, and recognized advertised brands from competitor brands. They also rated the on several dimensions. Scores on all three measures of commercial memory (i.e., brand recognition, brand recall, and commercial message recall) were expected to be higher among people who saw a nonviolent video than among people who saw a violent video. Structural equation analysis was used to test whether anger mediates the effect of television violence on commercial memory. Television violence was expected to increase anger levels in viewers, and anger, in turn, was expected to decrease commercial memory. This prediction follows from theories of the relation between mood and memory. The anger induced by violent programming could render commercials less accessible because violent programs activate other aggressive thoughts that interfere with the elaboration of the information contained in the commercials or because viewers focus more attention on repairing their negative mood than on the commercials. Arousal and positive affect were not expected to mediate the effect of television violence on commercial memory. Participants Me~od Participants were 320 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology classes (160 men and 160 women) who received extra course credit in exchange for their voluntary participation. Videotapes From the pool of 52 tapes used to select s for Experiments 1 and 2, 8 s were selected (4 violent and 4 nonviolent) for use in Experiment 3. 5 The videos were preselected to 5The eight s were taken from the following films: Cobra (male-on-male violence), Die Hard

9 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 299 Table 5 Comparison of the Nonviolent and Violent Videotapes Used in Experiment 3 on Various Rating Dimensions Selection procedure Experiment proper Violent Nonviolent Violent Nonviolent s s s s Measure M SE M SE M SE M SE Arousal content Exciting Boring ,20 Arousing ,20 Involving Violent Note. The means were adjusted for whether or not judges had seen the movie from which the segment was taken. The nonviolent videos were taken from Awakenings, Chariots of Fire, Field of Dreams, and Never Cry Wolf, and the violent videos were taken from Cobra, Die Hard, Single White Female, and The Hand That Rocks the Cradle. There were no sex differences in ratings for any. be equally exciting but differentially violent. As can be seen in Table 5, the violent and nonviolent s did not differ on any measure of arousal content. As expected, the violent s were judged to be more violent than the nonviolent s, F(1, 317) = 2,124.49, p <.05, d = There were no sex differences in ratings. The commercials embedded in the film clips were for Krazy Glue and Plax mouth rinse. Both commercials advertised products with broad market appeal, were 30-s long, and were several years old. The Krazy Glue commercial, also used in Experiments 1 and 2, was described earlier. The Plax commercial depicted a man brushing his teeth vigorously. The message stated that plaque was the number one dental problem and that even brushing well could not remove all plaque. The commercial ended by having the announcer state, "Rinsing with Plax before brushing removes 300% more plaque than brushing alone." Procedure Participants were tested in small groups, but they worked independently on tasks. The cover (male-on-male violence), Single White Female (femaleon-female violence), The Hand That Rocks the Cradle (female-on-female violence), Awakenings (nonviolent), Chariots of Fire (nonviolent), Field of Dreams (nonviolent), and Never Cry Wolf(nonviolent). story was the same one used in Experiments 1 and 2. Immediately after viewing the, participants completed a mood form that measured anger and positive affect. The anger measure consisted of 15 adjectives (e.g., angry, annoyed, and furious) from the Hostility subscale of the revised Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist (Zuckerman & Lubin, 1985). The positive affect measure consisted of 10 adjectives (e.g., alert, determined, and enthusiastic) from the Positive Affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clarke, & Tellegen, 1988). Watson and his colleagues defined positive affect as a state of "high energy, full concentration, and pleasurable engagement" (p. 1063). All adjectives were rated along a 5-point Likert-type scale, where 1 = very slightly or not at all, 2 = a little, 3 = moderately, 4 = quite a bit, and 5 = extremely. Participants were told to "Indicate to what extent you feel this way right now, that is, at the present moment." The alpha coefficients for the measures of anger and positive affect were.88 and.89, respectively. After completing the mood form, participants rated how exciting, boring, arousing, involving, and violent the film was. Several other rating dimensions were added as fillers. The ratings were made along a 10-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). Participants then listed the brand names of products shown in the commercials and listed everything they could remember about the commercial

10 300 BUSHMAN messages. Next, participants completed a visual recognition test. Participants were told that the film clip contained two commercials, one for a brand of glue and the other for a brand of mouth rinse. Participants were shown slides of 6 glue brands (Krazy Glue and 5 distractor brands) and 6 mouth rinses (Plax and 5 distractor brands) and were asked to write the brands of the products shown in the commercials. The order of the products and the order of brands within each product were randomized for each session. The slides were shown at the rate of one slide every 5 s. Next, participants reported the number of hours per week they spent watching various types of television programs, including violent programs. Participants also reported whether they had seen the film from which the clip had been taken and whether they had seen the commercials imbedded within the film clip. Finally, participants were questioned for suspicion and fully debriefed. No participant expressed suspicion about the study. Results Order, Commercial, Videotape, and Sex Effects The presentation order of the two commercials did not influence any of the dependent variables. There were no differences between the two commercials on any of the dependent variables. There were no differences between the four violent s or between the four nonviolent s on any of the dependent variables. There also were no sex main effects or interactions for any of the dependent variables. Thus, the data for the two orders, two commercials, four violent s, four nonviolent s, and two sexes were combined for subsequent analyses. Exposure Effects Habitual exposure to televised violence and previous exposure to the film and commercials shown in the were not significantly related to any of the commercial memory measures, either alone or interacting with film content. These exposure variables were therefore excluded from the analyses. Film Clip Differences in Arousing, Exciting, Boring, and Involving Ratings An analysis of covariance was used to compare arousing, exciting, boring, involving, and violent ratings for the violent and nonviolent s. Whether participants had seen the movie from which the clip was taken was used as a covariate. There was no difference between the violent and nonviolent s in arousing and involving ratings, Fs(1, 317) = 0.00 and 0.79, respectively, ps >.05 (see Table 5). In contrast with the selection procedure findings, the violent s were judged to be more exciting and less boring than the nonviolent s, F(1, 317) = 9.61, p <.05, d = 0.35 and F(1,317) = 6.74,p <.05, d = 0.29 (see Table 5). Of course, the violent s were judged to be more violent than the nonviolent s, F(1, 317) = 1,738.27, p <.05, d = 4.68 (see Table 5). However, none of the ratings influenced any of the dependent variables, either alone or interacting with film content. The ratings were therefore excluded from subsequent analyses. Reliability of Coding Message Recall Two independent raters who were unaware of conditions and experimental hypotheses coded the free recall data. Because the intraclass correlation coefficient (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) was fairly high (ricc=.80), scores from the two raters were averaged for subsequent analyses. Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages Brand name recognition. Participants who watched a nonviolent video recognized more brands from the slides than did participants who watched a violent, F(1,318) = 5.80, p <.05, d = 0.27 (see Table 6). 6 Brand name recall. Participants who watched a nonviolent video recalled more brand names than did participants who watched a violent 6 Although brand recognition scores only ranged from 0 to 2, log-linear analyses yielded the same pattern of results, X2(2, N = 320) = 6.19, p <.05.

11 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 301 Table 6 Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages (Experiment 3) Violent Nonviolent Measure M SE M SE Brand name recognition Brand name recall Commercial message recall Note. Brand name recognition and recall scores could range from 0 to 2. Commercial message recall scores were continuous (scores ranged from 1 to 18)., F(1, 318) = 4.50, p <.05, d = 0.24 (see Table 6). 7 Commercial message recall. Participants who watched a nonviolent video recalled more commercial details than did participants who watched a violent, F(1,318) = 6.01, p <.05, d = Anger as a Mediator in the Effect of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages Linear Structural Relations (LISREL) VIII (Jtreskog & Stirbom, 1993) was used to test whether anger mediates the effect of television violence on memory for commercial messages. LISREL has at least two advantages over causal modeling procedures based on ordinary leastsquares regression. First, it permits the removal of the effects of random measurement error that tend to attenuate path coefficients. Second, it provides a chi-square test of the overall goodness of fit of the model or the extent to which the hypothesized model is able to account for relations among the measured variables. In specifying the model, an indicator variable was used to represent television violence (violent film clip = 1 and nonviolent film clip = 0). The anger variable consisted of the sum of participants' scores for the 15 hostile adjectives on the mood form. In order to reduce the number of parameters being estimated, coefficient alpha (.88) was subtracted from 1.00 to estimate the error variance in the anger variable. Commercial memory was treated as a latent variable, operationalized using three measured variables: (a) brand name recognition, (b) brand name recall, and (c) commercial message recall. The variance--eovariance matrix used in the LISREL analysis is given in Table 7. The hypothesized mediation model contained a path from television violence to anger and another path from anger to commercial memory (see Figure 1). The mediation model fit the data extremely well, X2(5, N = 320) = 10.60, p >.05, goodness of fit index (GFI) =.99, comparative fit index (CFI) =.98, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) =.059. As can be seen in Figure 1, television violence increased anger, z = 6.64, p <.05, and anger, in turn, impaired memory for commercial messages, z = -4.71, p <.05. An alternative model that included a direct path from television violence to commercial memory was tested. Because the mediation model is nested within this alternative model, a chisquare difference test can be used to compare the two models. The difference test was nonsignificant, z(1, N = 320) = 0.08, p >.05. Thus, television violence did not have a direct effect on commercial memory. Other Mediators in the Effects of Television Violence on Memory for Commercial Messages LISREL also was used to test whether arousal or positive affect mediate the effect of television violence on memory for commercial messages. Each analysis is described in turn below. Arousal. In specifying this model, an indicator variable was used to represent television violence (violent film clip = 1 and nonviolent film clip = 0). Arousal was treated as latent variable, operationalized using three measured variables: (a) arousing film ratings, (b) exciting film ratings, and (c) boring film ratings. Commercial memory was treated as a latent variable, operationalized using three measured variables: (a) brand name recognition, (b) brand name recall, and (c) commercial message recall. The 7Although brand recall scores only ranged from 0 to 2, log-linear analyses yielded the same pattern of results. However, the effect was not significant, X2(2, N = 320) = 4.48, p >.05.

12 302 BUSHMAN Table 7 Variance-Covariance Matrix for Linear Structural Relations Analyses Measure Anger " * -.26* --.24*.36* 2. Arousing film rating " -.31".42* Exciting film rating " 1.48" * 4. Boring film rating " * 5. Positive affect " 6. Brand name recognition *.32* --.13" 7. Brand name recall * --.12" 8. Commercial message recall " 9. Television violence Note. Variances are on the diagonal and covariances are below the diagonal. Correlations are reported above the diagonal for descriptive purposes. *p <.05. variance-covariance matrix used in the LISREL analysis is given in Table 7. The mediation model contained a path from television violence to arousal and another path from arousal to commercial memory (see Figure 2). The mediation model fit the data extremely well, 2(13, N = 320) = 20.44, p >.05, GFI =.98, CFI =.98, RMSEA =.042. As can be seen in Figure 2, television violence increased arousal, z = 4.20, p <.05. Arousal did not, however, impair memory for commercial messages, z = -0.80, p >.05. The alternative model that included a direct path from television violence to commercial memory was tested. This alternative model did not fit the data better than the mediation model, 2(1, N = 320) = 3.48, p >.05. Once again, television violence did not have a direct effect on commercial memory. Positive affect. In specifying this model, an indicator variable was used to represent television violence (violent film clip - 1 and nonviolent film clip = 0). The positive affect variable consisted of the sum of participants' scores for the 10 positive affect adjectives on the mood form. In order to reduce the number of parameters being estimated, coefficient alpha (.89) was subtracted from 1.00 to estimate the error variance in the positive affect variable. Commercial memory was treated as a latent variable, operationalized using three measured variables: (a) brand name recognition, (b) brand name recall, and (c) commercial message recall. The variance-covariance matrix used in the LISREL analysis is given in Table 7. The mediation model contained a path from television violence to positive affect and another path from positive affect to commercial memory g=.e~ if=.15 ~=.6o Brand name I Brand name I Commercial reco0n,ion I I,'.ca,, II r.essa0ereoa,, Television I.3S*,-.1 vi lence I "l Anger I -.33*.~/" ~'~'1'2~'~.~*' ~ Figure 1. Anger as a mediator between television violence and memory of commercial messages. *p <.05.

13 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 303 ~'=.65 ~'=.15 ~'=.60 Brand name ] I Brand name Commercial recognition recall message recall Television violence.26" ~ -.05 /",.,u,,,,,,=,,.,,=,, \ Arousing [ I Exciting film rating film rating Boring [ film rating T 1 T =.79 C =.24 =.52 Figure 2. Arousal as a mediator between television violence and memory of commercial messages. *p <.05. (see Figure 3). The mediation model fit the data extremely well, X2(5, N = 320) = 7.69, p >.05, GFI =.99, CFI =.99, RMSEA =.041. As can be seen in Figure 3, television violence decreased positive affect, z = -3.01, p <.05. Positive affect did not, however, facilitate memory for commercial messages, z = 1.69, p >.05. An alternative model that included a direct path from television violence to commercial memory was tested. This alternative model did not fit the data better than the mediation model, X2(1, N = 320) = 1.82, p >.05, As was found in the previous models, television violence did not have a direct effect on commercial memory. In summary, the LISREL analyses suggest that anger is a more important mediator of the effect of television violence on commercial memory than either arousal or positive affect. Although all of the models fit the data extremely well, only anger was significantly related to commercial memory. Anger explained 11% of the variation in commercial memory, positive affect explained ~' =.68 (=.08 (=.63 Brand name recognition II- -" I I recall message recall Television [ -.76*,,., violence I "- I (R 2 =.58) I Positive affect I Figure 3. Positive affect as a mediator between television violence and memory of commercial messages. *p <.05.

14 304 BUSHMAN only 2% of the variation in commercial memory, and arousal explained none of the variation in commercial memory. Discussion As expected, television violence impaired memory for commercial messages. Participants who watched a violent video had lower scores on all three memory measures (i.e., brand name recognition, brand name recall, and commercial message recall) than did participants who watched a nonviolent video. These findings replicate those obtained in Experiments 1 and 2 using a broader set of film clips and commercial messages. The primary advantage of Experiment 3 is that it provides some insight about why television violence impairs memory for commercial messages. It is unlikely that the observed memory deficits were due to differential arousal produced by the films. The films did not differ in terms of how arousing and involving they were, and none of the film ratings influenced memory for commercials. Further, arousal was not related to commercial memory (see Figure 2). It also is unlikely that the observed memory deficits were due to positive affect. Although television violence decreased positive affect, positive affect was not related to commercial memory (see Figure 3). The observed memory deficits were due to anger. Television violence increased anger, and anger, in turn, decreased memory for commercial messages (see Figure 1). Television violence had an indirect rather than a direct effect on commercial memory. General Discussion In previous research examining the effect of violent programming on commercial memory, program violence has been confounded with other variables that could influence memory, such as arousal. The present research attempted to disentangle these variables by using programs selected to be differentially violent but equally arousing. The present findings suggest that violent programming impairs memory for commercial messages even when the level of program arousal is held constant or is statistically controlled. The negative effects of television violence on memory for commercial messages is at least partly due to the anger induced by the violent films. Information in commercials is retained to the extent that the viewers rehearse this information in their minds. The angry mood that was induced by the violent media might have activated other hostility-related ideas that interfered with rehearsal of commercial messages. Participants who saw a violent film clip might also have tried to repair their angry moods. During the time that advertisers hope viewers are absorbing their commercial messages, viewers may actually be focusing on themselves, trying to calm the anger brought on by what they have just seen on the screen. It should be noted that the experiments in this article did not directly test the effect of television violence on purchasing behavior. However, to the extent that a positive purchasing decision is based on the reception of information in commercials, impaired information acquisition and retrieval can only decrease commercial effectiveness (Mundorf et al., 1991). It also should be noted that the experiments in this article did not test the effect of television violence on delayed memory for commercial messages. The commercials used in the present research advertised products with broad market appeal and were relatively neutral in content. No attempt was made to manipulate whether the ads were consistent or inconsistent with the content of the program. One might predict that hostility based ads would be more memorable if placed in violent than in nonviolent programs. Current research in our laboratory is in the process of testing this hypothesis. The participants in the present research were all college students, and college students do differ from other people in some respects (e.g., Sears, 1986). Therefore, an important question is whether these findings generalize to other groups of people. It is unlikely that memory processes differ between college students and people of most other ages who do not have brain damage, but college students may differ from other people on nonmemory processes. College students may be heavier (or lighter) consumers of violent programming than people of other ages. Habitual exposure to television violence did not, however, influence the results in any of the experiments

15 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 305 reported in this article. Because the effect of television violence on commercial memory in Experiment 3 was mediated by anger, another potential obstacle to generalizing the results of the present research would be a differential propensity on the part of college students (relative to other groups) to react to violent material with anger. Although I know of no research documenting that college students are more (or less) likely than other people to feel anger when watching violent programming, the present findings suggest that if such differences in anger response were found, they would lead to corresponding group differences in memory for advertising content. In summary, group differences in the effects of television violence on commercial memory, if they exist, could be either smaller or larger than those obtained for college students in the present research. There is little reason to believe that the present findings are limited to college students. Television violence probably impairs memory for commercial messages in most viewers. Consider the magnitude of the effect of television violence on memory for commercials. The average effect-size estimate for the three experiments reported in this article was 0.34, with a 95% confidence interval ranging from 0.19 to 0.49 (for a reference on combining correlated effect-size estimates, see Gleser & Olkin, 1994). It is interesting to note that television violence has a similar sized effect on aggressive behavior (Hearold, 1986; Paik & Comstock, 1994; Wood, Wong, & Chachere, 1991). Thus, the effects reported in this article are by no means trivial. Several years ago, a spokesperson for the J. Walter Thompson Company stated, "The more we probe the issue, the more we are convinced that sponsorship of television violence is potentially bad business, as well as a social risk" ("Lousy Frames for Beautiful Pictures," 1977, p. 56). The data from the present experiments are consistent with this conclusion. There are at least three reasons why advertisers might want to think twice about sponsoring violent programs. First, most Americans are upset by the excessive amount of violence shown on television (e.g., Fischer, 1994; TV Guide, 1992; Zipperer, 1994). Second, television violence has a negative impact on society because it increases aggressive behavior in viewers (e.g., Hearold, 1986; Huston et al., 1992; National Institute of Mental Health, 1982; Paik & Comstock, 1994; Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, 1972; Wood et al., 1991). Third, violent programs decrease viewers' memories of brand names and commercial messages. Thus, sponsoring violent programs might not be a profitable venture for advertisers. References American Psychological Association. (1993). Violence and youth: Psychology's response. Washington, DC: Author. Anderson, C. A. (1997). Effects of violent movies and trait hostility on hostile feelings and aggressive thoughts. Aggressive Behavior, 23, Berkowitz, L. (1984). Some effects of thoughts on anti- and prosocial influences of media effects: A cognitive-neoassociation analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 95, Bettman, J. R. (1979). Memory factors in consumer choices: A review. Journal of Marketing, 43, Bower, G. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, Bryant, J., & Comisky, P. W. (1978). The effect of positioning a message within differentially cognitively involving portions of a television segment on recall of the message. Human Communications Research, 5, Burke, A., Heuer, E, & Reisberg, D. (1992). Remembering emotional events. Memory and Cognition, 20, Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effects of violent media on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, Bushman, B. J., & Geen, R. G. (1990). Role of cognitive-emotional mediators and individual differences in the effects of media violence on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, Christiansen, S.-A. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, Collins, A., & Loftus, E. (1975). A spreadingactivation theory of semantic memory. Psychological Review, 82, Craik, E I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for understanding memory

16 306 BUSHMAN research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, Diener, E., & DeFour, D. (1978). Does television violence enhance program popularity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, Diener, E., & Woody, L. W. (1981). Television violence, conflict, realism, and action: A study in viewer liking. Communication Research, 8, Drevitch, G. (1994, February 11). Murder, she saw. Scholastic Update, Fischer, R. L. (1994, July). Is it possible to regulate television violence? USA Today, Gleser, L. J., & Olkin, I. (1994). Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp ). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1990). The influence of sex and violence on the appeal of rock music videos. Communication Research, 17, Hearold, S. (1986). A synthesis of 1043 effects of television on social behavior. In G. Comstock (Ed.), Public communication and behavior (Vol 1, pp ). New York: Academic Press. Huston, A. C., Donnerstein, E., Fairchild, H., Feshbach, N. D., Katz, P. A., Murray, J. P., Rubinstein, E. A., Wilcox, B. L., & Zuckerman, D. (1992). Big world, small screen: The role of television in American society. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer, Jr. & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Isen, A. M., Clark, M., Shalker, T. E., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, J6reskog, K. G., & S6rbom, D. (1993). Windows LISREL Chicago: Scientific Software. Lousy frames for beautiful pictures: Are they changing? (1977, May 23). Advertising Age, 48, 56. Mayer, J. D., McCormick, L. J., & Strong, S. E. (1995). Mood-congruent memory and natural moods: New evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, Mundorf, N., Zillmann, D., & Drew, D. (1991). Effects of disturbing televised events on the acquisition of information from subsequently presented commercials. Journal of Advertising, 20, Mustonen, A. (1997). Nature of screen violence and its relation to program popularity. Aggressive Behavior, 23, Mustonen, A., & Pulkkinen, L. (1993). Aggression in television programs in Finland. Aggressive Behavior, 19, National Institute of Mental Health. (1982). Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific progress and implications for the eighties (Vol. 1), Summary Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. National Television Violence Study. (1996). National television violence study (Vol. 1). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. National Television Violence Study. (1997). National television violence study (Vol. 2). Studio City, CA: Mediascope. Oliver, M. B. (1994). Portrayals of crime, race, and aggression in "reality-based" police shows: A content analysis. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 38, Paik, H., & Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of television violence on antisocial behavior: A metaanalysis. Communication Research, 21, Parrott, G. W. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad moods. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Pavelchak, M. A., Antil, J. H., & Munch, J. M. (1988). The Super Bowl: An investigation into the relationship among program context, emotional experience, and ad recall. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychotogy's view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, Singh, S. N., & Churchill, G. A. (1987). Arousal and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Advertising, 16, 4-10, 40. Singh, S. N., & Rothschild, M. L. (1983). Recognition as a measure of learning from television commercials. Journal of Marketing Research, 20, Sprafkin, J. N., Rubinstein, E. A., & Stone, A. (1977).

17 TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND MEMORY FOR COMMERCIALS 307 The content analysis of four television diets (Occasional Paper 77-3). Stony Brook, NY: Brookdale International Institute. Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior. (1972). Television and growing up: The impact of televised violence. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. TV Guide. (1992, October 10-16). TV Guide poll: Would you give up TV for a million bucks? TV Guide, 40, 10-13, 15, 17. Watson, D., Clarke, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Wober, M. (1988). The extent to which viewers watch violence-containing programs. Current Psychology: Research and Reviews, 7, Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, Wood, W., Wong, E Y., & Chachere, J. G. (1991). Effects of media violence on viewers' aggression in unconstrained social interaction. Psychological Bulletin, 109, Worchel, S., Hardy, T. W., & Hurley, R. (1976). The effects of commercial interruption of violent and nonviolent films on viewers' subsequent aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, Zipperer, J. (1994, February 7). Violence foes take aim: Advertisers and affiliates caught in the crossfire. Christianity Today, Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1985). Manual for the MAACL-R: The Multiple Affective Adjective Checklist Revised. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Received September 15, 1997 Revision received April 17, 1998 Accepted April 28, 1998 New Editors Appointed, The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association announces the appointment of three new editors for 6-year terms beginning in As of January 1, 1999, manuscripts should be directed as follows: For Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, submit manuscripts to Warren K. Bickel, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, 38 Fletcher Place, Burlington, VT For the Journal of Counseling Psychology, submit manuscripts to Jo-Ida C. Hansen, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN For the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, submit manuscripts to David A. Rosenbaum, PhD, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, 642 Moore Building, University Park, PA Manuscript submission patterns make the precise date of completion of the 1999 volumes uncertain. Current editors, Charles R. Schuster, PhD; Clara E. Hill, PhD; and Thomas H. Carr, PhD, respectively, will receive and consider manuscripts through December 31, Should 1999 volumes be completed before that date, manuscripts will be redirected to the new editors for consideration in 2000 volumes.

The Effect of Interactive Program Loyalty Banners on Television Advertising Avoidance

The Effect of Interactive Program Loyalty Banners on Television Advertising Avoidance Page 1 of 7 ANZMAC 2009 The Effect of Interactive Program Loyalty Banners on Television Advertising Avoidance Steve Dix, Curtin University of Technology Steve Bellman, Interactive Television Research Institute

More information

How To Find Out How Fast A Car Is Going

How To Find Out How Fast A Car Is Going JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 13, 585-589 (1974) Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction: An Example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory ~ ELIZABETH F. LOFTUS AND JOHN C. PALMER

More information

A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics

A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics Dept. of Social Welfare, BaekSeok Culture University, ijoo65@hanmail.net Abstract

More information

Student Mood And Teaching Evaluation Ratings

Student Mood And Teaching Evaluation Ratings Student Mood And Teaching Evaluation Ratings Mary C. LaForge, Clemson University Abstract When student ratings are used for summative evaluation purposes, there is a need to ensure that the information

More information

A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents in the Klang Valley, Malaysia

A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents in the Klang Valley, Malaysia 2011 International Conference on Humanities, Society and Culture IPEDR Vol.20 (2011) (2011) IACSIT Press, Singapore A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents

More information

An Evaluation of Cinema Advertising Effectiveness

An Evaluation of Cinema Advertising Effectiveness An Evaluation of Cinema Advertising Effectiveness Jason Dunnett and Janet Hoek The purpose of this study was to explore the effectiveness of cinema advertising. Specifically: To quantify the proportion

More information

Video Game Effects on Children & Adolescents

Video Game Effects on Children & Adolescents Video Game Effects on Children & Adolescents Craig A. Anderson, Distinguished Professor Director, Center for the Study of Violence! Iowa State University Basic Science Department of Psychology Public Policy!

More information

Scientific Methods II: Correlational Research

Scientific Methods II: Correlational Research Scientific Methods II: Correlational Research EXAMPLES "MARRIAGE SLOWS CANCER DEATHS Evidence that married people have a better chance of surviving cancer than do singles means that the unmarried might

More information

Longitudinal Research on Game Effects and Implications for Public Policy Munich, Germany, November 20, 2008

Longitudinal Research on Game Effects and Implications for Public Policy Munich, Germany, November 20, 2008 Douglas A. Gentile, Ph.D. Director, Media Research Lab, Iowa State University Director of Research, National Institute on Media and the Family Research Fellow, Institute on Science and Society Associate

More information

Merged Structural Equation Model of Online Retailer's Customer Preference and Stickiness

Merged Structural Equation Model of Online Retailer's Customer Preference and Stickiness Merged Structural Equation Model of Online Retailer's Customer Preference and Stickiness Sri Hastuti Kurniawan Wayne State University, 226 Knapp Building, 87 E. Ferry, Detroit, MI 48202, USA af7804@wayne.edu

More information

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data John Reece Discipline of Psychology School of Health Sciences Impetus for This Research Oh, can you study psychology

More information

The Effects of Moderate Aerobic Exercise on Memory Retention and Recall

The Effects of Moderate Aerobic Exercise on Memory Retention and Recall The Effects of Moderate Aerobic Exercise on Memory Retention and Recall Lab 603 Group 1 Kailey Fritz, Emily Drakas, Naureen Rashid, Terry Schmitt, Graham King Medical Sciences Center University of Wisconsin-Madison

More information

Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1

Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1 Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1 Careers in Psychology 1. Which of the following psychological professionals must always have a medical degree? a. psychologist b. psychiatric social worker c. psychiatrist d.

More information

9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Single Factor design. Single design experiment. Experimental design. Textbook Chapters

9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Single Factor design. Single design experiment. Experimental design. Textbook Chapters 9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition Fall 2009 Single factor design Textbook Chapters Chapter 5: Types of variables Chapter 8: Controls Chapter 7: Validity Chapter 11: Single factor design Single design

More information

Picture Memory Improves with Longer On Time and Off Time

Picture Memory Improves with Longer On Time and Off Time Journal ol Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 197S, Vol. 104, No. 2, 114-118 Picture Memory mproves with Longer On Time and Off Time Barbara Tversky and Tracy Sherman The Hebrew University

More information

Scientific Method. 2. Design Study. 1. Ask Question. Questionnaire. Descriptive Research Study. 6: Share Findings. 1: Ask Question.

Scientific Method. 2. Design Study. 1. Ask Question. Questionnaire. Descriptive Research Study. 6: Share Findings. 1: Ask Question. Descriptive Research Study Investigation of Positive and Negative Affect of UniJos PhD Students toward their PhD Research Project : Ask Question : Design Study Scientific Method 6: Share Findings. Reach

More information

Effect of Video Games on Children s Aggressive Behavior and Pro-social Behavior: A Panel Study with Elementary School Students.

Effect of Video Games on Children s Aggressive Behavior and Pro-social Behavior: A Panel Study with Elementary School Students. Effect of Video Games on Children s Aggressive Behavior and Pro-social Behavior: A Panel Study with Elementary School Students. Nobuko Ihori Ochanomizu University n_ihori07@hotmail.com Akira Sakamoto Ochanomizu

More information

DEPTH OF PROCESSING PICTURES OF FACES AND RECOGNITION MEMORY 1

DEPTH OF PROCESSING PICTURES OF FACES AND RECOGNITION MEMORY 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology 1974, Vol. 103, No. 4, 7S1-7S7 DEPTH OF PROCESSING PICTURES OF FACES AND RECOGNITION MEMORY 1 GORDON H. BOWER 2 AND MARTIN B. KARLIN Stanford University These studies

More information

MAGNT Research Report (ISSN. 1444-8939) Vol.2 (Special Issue) PP: 213-220

MAGNT Research Report (ISSN. 1444-8939) Vol.2 (Special Issue) PP: 213-220 Studying the Factors Influencing the Relational Behaviors of Sales Department Staff (Case Study: The Companies Distributing Medicine, Food and Hygienic and Cosmetic Products in Arak City) Aram Haghdin

More information

Procrastination in Online Courses: Performance and Attitudinal Differences

Procrastination in Online Courses: Performance and Attitudinal Differences Procrastination in Online Courses: Performance and Attitudinal Differences Greg C Elvers Donald J. Polzella Ken Graetz University of Dayton This study investigated the relation between dilatory behaviors

More information

A Study in Learning Styles of Construction Management Students. Amit Bandyopadhyay, Ph.D., PE, F.ASCE State University of New York -FSC

A Study in Learning Styles of Construction Management Students. Amit Bandyopadhyay, Ph.D., PE, F.ASCE State University of New York -FSC A Study in Learning Styles of Construction Management Students Amit Bandyopadhyay, Ph.D., PE, F.ASCE State University of New York -FSC Abstract Students take in and process information in different ways.

More information

CASE STUDY. The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE ENGAGEMENT and VIDEO CONTENT POPULARITY DECEMBER 2011 INTRODUCTION KEY FINDINGS

CASE STUDY. The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE ENGAGEMENT and VIDEO CONTENT POPULARITY DECEMBER 2011 INTRODUCTION KEY FINDINGS CASE STUDY DECEMBER 2011 TABLE OF CONTENTS Situation Analysis... 2 Research Hypothesis... 2 Case Study Context... 2 Methodology...3-8 Business Implications...7-8 The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE

More information

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Interaction: memory experiment

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Interaction: memory experiment 9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science Fall 25 Lecture 6 Factorial Design: Complex design Aude Oliva Ben Balas, Charles Kemp Interaction: memory experiment Goal: In an experiment, you compare the explicit

More information

Effective Advertising and its Influence on Consumer Buying Behavior

Effective Advertising and its Influence on Consumer Buying Behavior Information Management and Business Review Vol. 4, No. 3, pp. 114-119, Mar 2012 (ISSN 2220-3796) Effective Advertising and its Influence on Consumer Buying Behavior * Ghulam Shabbir Khan Niazi 1, Javaria

More information

Video Game Violence and Public Policy

Video Game Violence and Public Policy Video Game Violence and Public Policy David Walsh, Ph.D. National Institute on Media and the Family Video games will turn 30 years old in 2002. The industry that started with Pong has become a multi-billion

More information

Person Perception in the College Classroom: Accounting for Taste in Students Evaluations of Teaching Effectiveness

Person Perception in the College Classroom: Accounting for Taste in Students Evaluations of Teaching Effectiveness Person Perception in the College Classroom: Accounting for Taste in Students Evaluations of Teaching Effectiveness Jennifer Gross, 1 Brian Lakey, and Kristin Edinger Grand Valley State University Edward

More information

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES.

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. 277 CHAPTER VI COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. This chapter contains a full discussion of customer loyalty comparisons between private and public insurance companies

More information

Impact of attendance policies on course attendance among college students

Impact of attendance policies on course attendance among college students Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, Vol. 8, No. 3, October 2008. pp. 29-35. Impact of attendance policies on course attendance among college students Tiffany Chenneville 1 and Cary Jordan

More information

The Effect of Neighborhood Watch. Programs on Neighborhood Crime in. Medford Oregon

The Effect of Neighborhood Watch. Programs on Neighborhood Crime in. Medford Oregon The Effect of Neighborhood Watch Programs on Neighborhood Crime in Medford Oregon Economics 419: Honors Thesis - Draft May 25th, 2014 Abstract: Over the past decade Medford Oregon has expanded their Neighborhood

More information

The Relation between Positive Brand Emotions and Recall Duygu Aydın, Selcuk University, Turkey

The Relation between Positive Brand Emotions and Recall Duygu Aydın, Selcuk University, Turkey The Relation between Positive Brand Emotions and Recall Duygu Aydın, Selcuk University, Turkey Abstract Brand memory and ad memory is a kind of mental storage that consumers apply to while making decisions

More information

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan ABSTRACT The focus of this research is to determine the impact of

More information

Sport Celebrity Influence on Young Adult Consumers. Keywords: Advertising, Execution, Strategy, Celebrity

Sport Celebrity Influence on Young Adult Consumers. Keywords: Advertising, Execution, Strategy, Celebrity Page 1 of 9 ANZMAC 2009 Sport Celebrity Influence on Young Adult Consumers Steve Dix, Curtin University of Technology Email: Steve.Dix@cbs.curtin.edu.au The paper investigates how sports celebrities can

More information

Checklist Of What Works In Print, Radio, TV, Direct Mail and Outdoor Ads

Checklist Of What Works In Print, Radio, TV, Direct Mail and Outdoor Ads IN PRINT 1. Use simple layouts. Checklist Of What Works In Print, Radio, TV, Direct Mail and Outdoor Ads by Tom Egelhoff One big picture works better than several small pictures. Avoid cluttered pages.

More information

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION 行 政 院 國 家 科 學 委 員 會 專 題 研 究 計 畫 成 果 報 告 線 上 購 物 介 面 成 份 在 行 銷 訊 息 的 思 考 可 能 性 模 式 中 所 扮 演 角 色 之 研 究 The Role of Online Shopping s Interface Components in Elaboration of the Marketing Message 計 畫 編 號 :

More information

Impact. How to choose the right campaign for maximum effect. RKM Research and Communications, Inc., Portsmouth, NH. All Rights Reserved.

Impact. How to choose the right campaign for maximum effect. RKM Research and Communications, Inc., Portsmouth, NH. All Rights Reserved. Impact How to choose the right campaign for maximum effect RKM Research and Communications, Inc., Portsmouth, NH. All Rights Reserved. Executive summary Advertisers depend on traditional methods of testing

More information

Selective Exposure to Television Programmes and Advertising Effectiveness

Selective Exposure to Television Programmes and Advertising Effectiveness APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 17: 593 606 (2003) Published online in Wiley InterScience 24 April 2003 (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.892 Selective Exposure to Television

More information

Premium Advertising Sweden UK France Germany

Premium Advertising Sweden UK France Germany Premium Advertising Sweden UK France Germany On behalf of Widespace 05.11.2015 Content Study design Management Summary Sample Results Total Sweden UK France Germany Contact 2 Study design Study characteristics

More information

Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Arguments for and Against Measuring Effectiveness. Chapter 18 Measuring the Effectiveness of the Promotional Program

Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Arguments for and Against Measuring Effectiveness. Chapter 18 Measuring the Effectiveness of the Promotional Program Chapter 18 Measuring the Effectiveness of the Promotional Program Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill

More information

A BEHAVIORAL VIEW OF LEARNING

A BEHAVIORAL VIEW OF LEARNING Chapter 10 Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning: The Story of Dogs and Little Albert A BEHAVIORAL VIEW OF LEARNING As you read below you may come to think that behavioral learning theories seem

More information

An Empirical Study on the Effects of Software Characteristics on Corporate Performance

An Empirical Study on the Effects of Software Characteristics on Corporate Performance , pp.61-66 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/astl.2014.48.12 An Empirical Study on the Effects of Software Characteristics on Corporate Moon-Jong Choi 1, Won-Seok Kang 1 and Geun-A Kim 2 1 DGIST, 333 Techno Jungang

More information

THE RELATIVE ROLE OF IMPACT AND RESONANCE IN DETERMINING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF RADIO ADVERTISING. Steen Lundsteen, Copenhagen Business School, Denmark

THE RELATIVE ROLE OF IMPACT AND RESONANCE IN DETERMINING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF RADIO ADVERTISING. Steen Lundsteen, Copenhagen Business School, Denmark THE RELATIVE ROLE OF IMPACT AND RESONANCE IN DETERMINING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF RADIO ADVERTISING Steen Lundsteen, Copenhagen Business School, Denmark Flemming Hansen, Copenhagen Business School, Denmark

More information

Media Studies / 6th 7th Grade

Media Studies / 6th 7th Grade Media Studies / 6th 7th Grade MARY ENGLER HAGEN FYI I have set up these lessons/unit on media studies for a 6th or 7th grade class. I have not intended to make these lessons a surface study only of media,

More information

Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88)

Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88) Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88) Introduction The National Educational Longitudinal Survey (NELS:88) followed students from 8 th grade in 1988 to 10 th grade in

More information

Learning and Memory Adult Development. Lifespan Development Applied Psychology. Multi-Cultural Psychology Child Development

Learning and Memory Adult Development. Lifespan Development Applied Psychology. Multi-Cultural Psychology Child Development GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 201 FALL 2010 Instructor: Eric Kim MWF: 9:00-9:50 / DTC 302 Office Hours: TuTh 12:00-12:50 (main campus) TuTh 1:00-2:20 / CEN 402 MWF 10:00-10:50 (Downtown Center) TuTh 3:00-4:20 / DTC

More information

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING *Zahra Gholami Ghareh Shiran 1, Ghodsi Ahghar 2, Afshin Ahramiyan 3, Afsaneh Boostan

More information

DEVELOPING A LOW COST BRAIN INJURY REHABILITATION PROGRAM: GUIDELINES FOR FAMILY MEMBERS

DEVELOPING A LOW COST BRAIN INJURY REHABILITATION PROGRAM: GUIDELINES FOR FAMILY MEMBERS DEVELOPING A LOW COST BRAIN INJURY REHABILITATION PROGRAM: GUIDELINES FOR FAMILY MEMBERS The day you waited for with such mixed feelings finally arrived: your family member was discharged from rehabilitation

More information

Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology

Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology INTRODUCTION Previous studies (e.g., Jiang & Benbasat, 2005; Algharabat & Dennis, 2010 a, b, c) regarding online

More information

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology 1.Obtaining Knowledge 1. Correlation 2. Causation 2.Hypothesis Generation & Measures 3.Looking into

More information

THE ROLE OF PERFORMANCE IN THE STRESS RE-APPRAISAL PROCESS

THE ROLE OF PERFORMANCE IN THE STRESS RE-APPRAISAL PROCESS Indiana University Undergraduate Journal of Cognitive Science 6 (2011) Copyright 2011 IUCJS. All rights reserved. THE ROLE OF PERFORMANCE IN THE STRESS RE-APPRAISAL PROCESS Leslie Rith-Najarian Department

More information

ANALYSIS: General Statistics

ANALYSIS: General Statistics HYPOTHESIS: Current research shows that an effective social media campaign will promote the business and its brand along with its products and services while encouraging the audience and target market

More information

Principles Of Research Design

Principles Of Research Design 186 Part 3 / Research Designs, Settings, and Procedures Chapter 13 Principles Of Research Design Research designs can be classified into three broad categories, according to the amount of control the researcher

More information

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life Final report December 2012 Context Sport England s 2012-17 strategy aims to help people and communities across the country transform our sporting culture, so that

More information

Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression

Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression Slide 1 Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression There are several different measures of statistical association used for understanding the quantitative relationship

More information

Principles of Measuring Advertising Effectiveness

Principles of Measuring Advertising Effectiveness Print article Close window Principles of Measuring Advertising Effectiveness By David Olson Testing the effectiveness of advertising is a big business. With the cost of producing a commercial at $500,000

More information

Copyright subsists in all papers and content posted on this site.

Copyright subsists in all papers and content posted on this site. Student First Name: Raed Student Second Name: Algharabat Copyright subsists in all papers and content posted on this site. Further copying or distribution by any means without prior permission is prohibited,

More information

SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT. A Thesis

SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT. A Thesis SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Chapter 9. Two-Sample Tests. Effect Sizes and Power Paired t Test Calculation

Chapter 9. Two-Sample Tests. Effect Sizes and Power Paired t Test Calculation Chapter 9 Two-Sample Tests Paired t Test (Correlated Groups t Test) Effect Sizes and Power Paired t Test Calculation Summary Independent t Test Chapter 9 Homework Power and Two-Sample Tests: Paired Versus

More information

Online Customer Experience

Online Customer Experience Online Customer Experience What is the experience and how is it difference for frequent and infrequent purchasers, age cohorts and gender groups? Prepared by Jillian Martin and Gary Mortimer for Coles

More information

An Empirical Study on the Influence of Perceived Credibility of Online Consumer Reviews

An Empirical Study on the Influence of Perceived Credibility of Online Consumer Reviews An Empirical Study on the Influence of Perceived Credibility of Online Consumer Reviews GUO Guoqing 1, CHEN Kai 2, HE Fei 3 1. School of Business, Renmin University of China, 100872 2. School of Economics

More information

Marketing Mix Modelling and Big Data P. M Cain

Marketing Mix Modelling and Big Data P. M Cain 1) Introduction Marketing Mix Modelling and Big Data P. M Cain Big data is generally defined in terms of the volume and variety of structured and unstructured information. Whereas structured data is stored

More information

McGraw-Hill/Irwin 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., All Rights Reserved.

McGraw-Hill/Irwin 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., All Rights Reserved. 19 Measuring the Effectiveness of the Promotional Program McGraw-Hill/Irwin 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., All Rights Reserved. Pros and Cons of Measuring Advertising Effectiveness Reasons to Measure

More information

Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding?

Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding? Memory & Cognition 1977, Vol. 5 (2),252-256 Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding? PAUL R. D'AGOSTINO Gettysburg CoUege, Gettysburg, Pennsylvania 17925

More information

THE ART OF EFFECTIVE ADVERTISING

THE ART OF EFFECTIVE ADVERTISING FEATURED INSIGHTS DELIVERING CONSUMER CLARITY THE ART OF EFFECTIVE ADVERTISING THE 3-C FRAMEWORK OF CONTENT, CONTEXT AND CONSUMER Only one out of five advertising campaigns on television turn out efficient

More information

Overview of Factor Analysis

Overview of Factor Analysis Overview of Factor Analysis Jamie DeCoster Department of Psychology University of Alabama 348 Gordon Palmer Hall Box 870348 Tuscaloosa, AL 35487-0348 Phone: (205) 348-4431 Fax: (205) 348-8648 August 1,

More information

Alcohol and drug abuse

Alcohol and drug abuse Alcohol and drug abuse This chapter explores how alcohol abuse affects our families, relationships, and communities, as well as the health risks associated with drug and alcohol abuse. 1. Alcohol abuse

More information

Does Trust Matter to Develop Customer Loyalty in Online Business?

Does Trust Matter to Develop Customer Loyalty in Online Business? Does Trust Matter to Develop Customer Loyalty in Online Business? Pattarawan Prasarnphanich, Ph.D. Department of Information Systems, City University of Hong Kong Email: pprasarn@cityu.edu.hk Abstract

More information

SAMPLE REPORT. To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.psychcorp.com

SAMPLE REPORT. To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.psychcorp.com Scoring Assistant with Report Writer SAMPLE REPORT To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.psychcorp.com In Canada, call 1-800-387-7278 In United Kingdom, call +44 (0) 1865 888188 In

More information

Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination

Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 4, No. 3, April 2009, pp. 235 247 Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination Todd J. Thorsteinson University of

More information

The Influence of Arousal on Advertising Effectiveness

The Influence of Arousal on Advertising Effectiveness The Influence of Arousal on Advertising Effectiveness D. Belanche 1, C. Flavián 2, A. Pérez-Rueda 3 Departamento de Dirección de Marketing e Investigación de Mercados, Universidad de Zaragoza, Zaragoza,

More information

The Effect of Perceived Value on Customer Loyalty in a Low-Priced Cosmetic Brand of South Korea: The Moderating Effect of Gender

The Effect of Perceived Value on Customer Loyalty in a Low-Priced Cosmetic Brand of South Korea: The Moderating Effect of Gender , pp.40-44 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/astl.2015.114.08 The Effect of Perceived Value on Customer Loyalty in a Low-Priced Cosmetic Brand of South Korea: The Moderating Effect of Gender Ki-Han Chung 1, Ji-Eun

More information

Comparing User Engagement across Seven Interactive and Social-Media Ad Types.

Comparing User Engagement across Seven Interactive and Social-Media Ad Types. Comparing User Engagement across Seven Interactive and Social-Media Ad Types. A Collaborative Study by ` Executive Summary. Increases in social networking and interactive advertising technology have led

More information

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE IMPACTS OF VIDEO AND ONLINE GAMES ON ADOLESCENTS

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE IMPACTS OF VIDEO AND ONLINE GAMES ON ADOLESCENTS Aug. 21. Vol., No. ISSN 23-193 212-21 IJSK & K.A.J. All rights reserved AN INVESTIGATION OF THE IMPACTS OF VIDEO AND ONLINE GAMES ON ADOLESCENTS 1 AMOS O OLAGUNJU, 2 NHIA YANG 1 Professor, Computer Science

More information

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Factorial Design

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Factorial Design 9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science Fall 25 Lecture 4a Factorial Design: Interaction Aude Oliva Ben Balas, Charles Kemp Factorial Design Two or more factors in such a way that all the possible combinations

More information

Viewing a Crime Drama

Viewing a Crime Drama LESSON PLAN Level: Grades 9 11 About the Author: Adapted, with permission, from a unit developed by Mark Zamparo, an Ottawa-based media educator Viewing a Crime Drama Overview In this lesson students explore

More information

Reliability and validity, the topics of this and the next chapter, are twins and

Reliability and validity, the topics of this and the next chapter, are twins and Research Skills for Psychology Majors: Everything You Need to Know to Get Started Reliability Reliability and validity, the topics of this and the next chapter, are twins and cannot be completely separated.

More information

Sports betting advertising lesson plans

Sports betting advertising lesson plans Sports betting advertising lesson plans This resource will help secondary students to discuss the definition of sport and then critically deconstruct the portrayal of sports betting as a normalised part

More information

INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION

INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION Yafang Tsai, Department of Health Policy and Management, Chung-Shan Medical University, Taiwan, (886)-4-24730022 ext.12127, avon611@gmail.com

More information

Lesson Plan for Media Literacy

Lesson Plan for Media Literacy Constructions Beliefs & Values Audience Intent Form Lesson Plan for Media Literacy Text Production Audience Lesson Focus: What is the focus of the lesson? How will I teach it? The focus of this shared

More information

Do Supplemental Online Recorded Lectures Help Students Learn Microeconomics?*

Do Supplemental Online Recorded Lectures Help Students Learn Microeconomics?* Do Supplemental Online Recorded Lectures Help Students Learn Microeconomics?* Jennjou Chen and Tsui-Fang Lin Abstract With the increasing popularity of information technology in higher education, it has

More information

Click Here: The State of Online Advertising. New insights into the beliefs of consumers and professional marketers. October 2012

Click Here: The State of Online Advertising. New insights into the beliefs of consumers and professional marketers. October 2012 Click Here: The State of Online Advertising New insights into the beliefs of consumers and professional marketers October 2012 Methodology Research firm Edelman Berland conducted an online survey of 1,250

More information

RESIDENTIAL CUSTOMER WATER EFFICIENCY SURVEY (November, 2011) Executive Summary

RESIDENTIAL CUSTOMER WATER EFFICIENCY SURVEY (November, 2011) Executive Summary RESIDENTIAL CUSTOMER WATER EFFICIENCY SURVEY (November, 2011) Executive Summary The Survey Sample This statistically-projectable survey includes 400 telephone interviews conducted among a random sampling

More information

Research of Female Consumer Behavior in Cosmetics Market Case Study of Female Consumers in Hsinchu Area Taiwan

Research of Female Consumer Behavior in Cosmetics Market Case Study of Female Consumers in Hsinchu Area Taiwan usiness, 2010, 2, 348-353 doi:10.4236/ib.2010.24045 Published Online December 2010 (http://www.scirp.org/journal/ib) Research of Female Consumer Behavior in Cosmetics Market Case Study of Female Consumers

More information

Media: Participants learn about the media s influence on our lives; participants identify how

Media: Participants learn about the media s influence on our lives; participants identify how Media: Participants learn about the media s influence on our lives; participants identify how the media influences their attitudes; participants brainstorm ways to combat the media s negative influence.

More information

The Impact of Consumer Interactions in Social Networking Sites on Brand Perception

The Impact of Consumer Interactions in Social Networking Sites on Brand Perception IBIMA Publishing Journal of Internet and e-business Studies http://www.ibimapublishing.com/journals/jiebs/jiebs.html Vol. 2015 (2015), Article ID 197131, 7 pages DOI: 10.5171/2015.197131 Research Article

More information

The Influence of Stressful Life Events of College Students on Subjective Well-Being: The Mediation Effect of the Operational Effectiveness

The Influence of Stressful Life Events of College Students on Subjective Well-Being: The Mediation Effect of the Operational Effectiveness Open Journal of Social Sciences, 2016, 4, 70-76 Published Online June 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/jss http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jss.2016.46008 The Influence of Stressful Life Events of

More information

Using Survey Research to Evaluate Communications Campaigns

Using Survey Research to Evaluate Communications Campaigns Using Survey Research to Evaluate Communications Campaigns Prepared for the Communications Consortium Media Center Belden Russonello & Stewart November 2004 Introduction When should a non-profit communications

More information

Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1

Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1 Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1 Theme 1: Introduction and Research Methods Topic 1: Introduction 1.1.1 Define psychology, and list and explain its goals. 1.1.2 Describe and compare

More information

THE ROLE OF MEDIA VIOLENCE IN VIOLENT BEHAVIOR

THE ROLE OF MEDIA VIOLENCE IN VIOLENT BEHAVIOR Annu. Rev. Public Health 2006. 27:393 415 doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144640 Copyright c 2006 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved THE ROLE OF MEDIA VIOLENCE IN VIOLENT BEHAVIOR L. Rowell

More information

A Study on Satisfaction of Movie Viewers Watching Movies on Smartphones

A Study on Satisfaction of Movie Viewers Watching Movies on Smartphones , pp.78-83 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/astl.2014.67.19 A Study on Satisfaction of Movie Viewers Watching Movies on Smartphones Hyungjoon Kim 1, Seongwon Park 2* and Hyelin Yang 2 1 Office of International

More information

PARTIAL LEAST SQUARES IS TO LISREL AS PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS ANALYSIS IS TO COMMON FACTOR ANALYSIS. Wynne W. Chin University of Calgary, CANADA

PARTIAL LEAST SQUARES IS TO LISREL AS PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS ANALYSIS IS TO COMMON FACTOR ANALYSIS. Wynne W. Chin University of Calgary, CANADA PARTIAL LEAST SQUARES IS TO LISREL AS PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS ANALYSIS IS TO COMMON FACTOR ANALYSIS. Wynne W. Chin University of Calgary, CANADA ABSTRACT The decision of whether to use PLS instead of a covariance

More information

Self Assessment: Substance Abuse

Self Assessment: Substance Abuse Self Assessment: Substance Abuse Please respond TRUE (T) or FALSE (F) to the following items as they apply to you. Part 1 I use or have used alcohol or drugs for recreational purposes. I use alcohol despite

More information

9. Sampling Distributions

9. Sampling Distributions 9. Sampling Distributions Prerequisites none A. Introduction B. Sampling Distribution of the Mean C. Sampling Distribution of Difference Between Means D. Sampling Distribution of Pearson's r E. Sampling

More information

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts Gender Stereotypes Associated 1 Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts Lacey D. Seefeldt Undergraduate Student, Psychology Keywords: Altruism, Gender Stereotypes, Vignette Abstract Possible

More information

Scientific Methods in Psychology

Scientific Methods in Psychology Scientific Methods in Psychology Why do research? To demonstrate that psychology is a science! Psychology would like to have the same academic standing as other sciences like biology, chemistry, astronomy,

More information

Violent Video Games: The Effects of Gender Match on Levels of Aggression

Violent Video Games: The Effects of Gender Match on Levels of Aggression Violent Video Games: The Effects of Gender Match on Levels of Aggression McNair Scholars Journal Volume 11 by Sunny Lee Faculty Mentor: Dr. Kimberly Roberts abstract This study examined the relationship

More information

How To Teach A Sociological Course

How To Teach A Sociological Course Course: Introduction to Sociology Kirkwood Community College CASTLE Case Study Analysis Questions Introduction to Sociology Jeff Sherman May 4, 2004 Learning Outcomes: Evaluate the techniques of different

More information

EXPLAIN THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ADVERTISING USING THE AIDA MODEL

EXPLAIN THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ADVERTISING USING THE AIDA MODEL EXPLAIN THE EFFECTIVENESS OF ADVERTISING USING THE AIDA MODEL SAHAR GHARIBI a DR.SEYED YAHYAH SEYED DANESH b DR.KAMBIZ SHAHRODI c Abstract The main objective of this research, Explain the effectiveness

More information

Alcohol Marketing, the Alcohol Industry and Their Impact on Binge Drinking by Adolescents

Alcohol Marketing, the Alcohol Industry and Their Impact on Binge Drinking by Adolescents Alcohol Marketing, the Alcohol Industry and Their Impact on Binge Drinking by Adolescents Thomas F. Babor, PhD, MPH The University of Connecticut School of Medicine Department of Community Medicine and

More information

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1 Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida The critical reader of a research report expects the writer to

More information