Consumers often interact more effectively with physically. Consumer Reactions to Attractive Service Providers: Approach or Avoid?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Consumers often interact more effectively with physically. Consumer Reactions to Attractive Service Providers: Approach or Avoid?"

Transcription

1 Consumer Reactions to Attractive Service Providers: Approach or Avoid? LISA C. WAN ROBERT S. WYER JR INTRODUCTION Consumers often interact more effectively with physically attractive service providers, resulting in greater consumer satisfaction and stronger purchase intentions Lisa C. Wan is assistant professor of School of Hotel and Tourism Management, the Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong Robert S. Wyer Jr. is visiting professor of marketing, the Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong The authors gratefully acknowledge the helpful input of the editor, associate editor, and three reviewers. The authors also gratefully acknowledge Anthony Cheng, Kendra Tang, and a group of student helpers from the Chinese University of Hong Kong for their assistance in data collection. In addition, the authors would like to thank 228IN Toy Store and KINNETEC Company for their help in data collection for field experiments. This research was supported in part by Grants GRF , , and from the Research Grants Council, Hong Kong. Supplemental materials are included in the online-only version of this article. Darren Dahl served as editor, and Andrea Morales served as associate editor for this article. Advance Access publication August 27, 2015 Attractive service providers are often assumed to elicit favorable consumer reactions and to increase purchase intentions. However, this may not always be true. A pilot study and five field and laboratory experiments show that when a selfpresentation concern is made salient, consumers react less positively to highly attractive providers than to less attractive ones. This concern can be influenced by chronic social anxiety or can be aroused by unrelated experiences that consumers have before being exposed to the service interactions. In addition, it can be activated by the type of product being sold, that is, a product that is likely to cause embarrassment. Thus the attractiveness of a service provider can have either positive or negative effects on consumers reactions to a consumption experience and their consequent purchase intentions, depending on the type of product under consideration. These effects occur when the service provider is both of the opposite sex and the same sex. However, self-presentation concerns when an opposite-sex provider is attractive are driven by sexual motives, whereas these self-presentation concerns when a same-sex target is attractive are stimulated by social comparison processes. Keywords: attractive service providers, self-presentation concern, social anxiety, embarrassing consumption, social comparison processes (e.g., Ahearne, Gruen, and Jarvis 1999). Indeed, consumers sometimes evaluate a product more favorably when it has simply been touched by an attractive service provider (Argo, Dahl, and Morales 2008). The assumption that physically attractive salespersons are particularly effective is pervasive. The present research qualifies this assumption, however. We find that attractive service providers can sometimes lead consumers to have self-presentation concerns (that is, concerns about their ability to make a good impression on others; Leary et al. 1994). Research on impression management (Leary and Kowalski 1990) suggests that people are generally motivated to make a favorable impression on attractive others and that their inability to accomplish can create anxiety (Leary 1986; Schlenker and Leary 1982). This suggests that when a provider s physical attractiveness elicits self-presentation concerns, it might have a detrimental effect on their purchase decisions. We predicted that when consumers self-presentation concerns are heightened, they often avoid interacting with physically attractive providers and so the providers are relatively ineffective. As we discuss presently, the processes that underlie this effect are likely to depend on whether the VC The Author Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Journal of Consumer Research, Inc. All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oup.com Vol DOI: /jcr/ucv

2 WAN AND WYER 579 consumer and provider are the same or the opposite sex. That is, the self-presentation concerns that are stimulated by exposure to an attractive opposite-sex provider are sexually driven, whereas the concerns activated by an attractive same-sex target are a result of social comparison processes. Nevertheless, their effects on provider effectiveness are similar. A provider s physical attractiveness is obviously not sufficient to elicit concerns about the impression one forms; other conditions are necessary as well. Self-presentation concerns can be influenced by chronic individual differences in social anxiety (Arkin, Appelman, and Burger 1980). However, they can also be activated by unrelated experiences that occur prior to a social interaction and by features of the interaction itself. In the latter regard, the consumption of some products (e.g., condoms, weight-loss pills) is likely to cause embarrassment (see Dahl, Manchanda, and Argo 2001). These factors are likely to combine with a salesperson s physical attractiveness to activate consumers concerns about their self-presentation, leading them to avoid interacting with the salesperson and decreasing their likelihood of making a purchase. To our knowledge, our research is the first to distinguish between conditions in which a service provider s physical attractiveness increases the favorableness of consumers reactions to a service encounter and the conditions in which it decreases the favorableness of these reactions. We investigated these conditions in six studies. An observational study provided initial evidence that individuals social anxiety influences their self-presentation concerns and their reactions to attractive opposite-sex service providers in an actual consumption setting. Four laboratory studies confirmed the implications of this study, showing that consumers avoidance of an attractive opposite-sex provider depends on their self-presentation concerns and that this is true regardless of whether these concerns are chronic (study 1), induced in an unrelated situation (study 2), or activated by characteristics of the consumption situation itself (studies 3 and 4). A fifth experiment demonstrated similar effects when the attractive service provider was of the same sex rather than the opposite sex. CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND The Effects of Physical Attractiveness The positive impact of physical attractiveness on social interactions is well established (e.g., Berscheid and Walster 1974; Dion, Berscheid, and Walster 1972; Eagly et al. 1991). The beautiful is good stereotype is pervasive. Thus physically attractive people are perceived to have more desirable personalities, more favorable attributes, and more satisfying life experiences. People think that attractive individuals are more persuasive, have more influence on others, and are better liked (Sigall and Aronson 1969). They also perceive attractive individuals to be more intellectually competent, popular, and sociable (Eagly et al. 1991). They therefore find interacting with attractive individuals to be an aesthetically pleasant experience (Dion et al. 1972). Consumer research appears to confirm implications of these findings. Studies of service marketing have found that attractive service providers are often better liked and produce greater consumer satisfaction and purchase intentions (e.g., Ahearne et al. 1999; DeShields, Kara, and Kaynak 1996; Koernig and Page 2002; Reingen and Kernan 1993). An attractive salesperson can sometimes increase evaluations of a product by simply touching it (Argo et al. 2008). The impact of a provider s attractiveness decreases over time and repeated exposure (Ahearne et al. 1999). Nevertheless, past research perpetuates the view that attractive service providers increase the aesthetic pleasantness of a consumption experience and have a positive effect on first-time purchases. However, little if any previous research has investigated the conditions in which the physical attractiveness of a service provider might decrease the favorableness of consumers reactions to a service encounter. A field study by Dabbs and Stokes (1975) is nonetheless suggestive. They found that both male and female pedestrians walked further away from an attractive woman than from an unattractive one. This was apparently because they were nervous about approaching an attractive person. These findings suggest that individuals often have self-presentation concerns when they anticipate interacting with an attractive service provider and consequently may avoid doing so. Moreover, these avoidance reactions may be evident regardless of whether the provider is of the same or opposite sex. However, the factors that mediate these reactions may differ. That is, self-presentation concerns about interacting with an attractive opposite-sex provider are likely to stem from anxiety about creating a poor impression on a sexually appealing other. In contrast, the self-presentation concerns activated by an attractive same-sex provider may elicit social comparison processes that create jealousy and dislike of the person who gives rise to these feelings. The next section elaborates these differences. Self-Presentation and Opposite-Sex Providers The impressions that others form of a person have implications for how the person is treated (Goffman 1959). Consequently, people attempt to control the impressions they convey (Leary and Kowalski 1990). Impression management has two components. First, impression motivation concerns the desire to make a favorable impression on others and occurs when people are concerned about how the others will evaluate them (Leary 1996; Leary and Kowalski 1990). The second, impression construction,

3 580 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH concerns the strategies that people use to influence others impressions of them. We consider each component in turn. Impression Motivation. Individuals motivations to present themselves in a favorable light when they encounter an attractive person of the opposite sex may be influenced in part by the person s physical attractiveness. When a person is not particularly attractive, individuals may not always think about the impression they make. When the person is attractive, however, self-presentation concerns are more likely to come into play (Schlenker 1980). As noted earlier, physically attractive persons are attributed generally favorable personal qualities (Berscheid and Walster 1974) and are preferred as dating partners (Walster, Aronson, and Abrahams 1966). These persons can therefore spontaneously activate self-presentation concerns that are sexually driven. Evidence of this motivation in opposite-sex interactions is suggested in a study by Snyder, Tanke, and Berscheid (1977). They found that when male participants believed that the woman with whom they were talking in a telephone conversation was physically attractive, they communicated in a warmer and more personable way that elicited the qualities they expected the woman to have. In a quite different situation, Dong and Wyer (2014) found that when individuals conversed with an opposite-sex partner that they believed to be of average attractiveness, they were motivated to form an impression of the partner and paid particular attention to things the partner said during the conversation. When they believed their partner to be physically attractive, however, they were more concerned about creating a good impression on the other rather than forming an impression of the other. Thus they were more attentive to things they personally said. Impression Construction. When individuals want to make a good impression on someone and feel able to do so, they may be motivated to interact with this person. The strategies they use to attain this objective can involve selfdescriptions, nonverbal behaviors, and often deception. Thus, for example, female participants eat less snack food in the presence of an attractive male in order to enhance their apparent femininity (Mori, Chaiken, and Pliner 1987). Both male and female participants are more apt to lie about their personal attributes to get a date if the prospect is physically attractive than if (s)he is less so (Tyler and Feldman 2004). Moreover, males display luxury possessions to impress attractive women (Griskevicius et al. 2007). Situations can arise, however, when individuals are motivated to make a favorable impression on someone but believe they are unable to attain this objective. In this case, they could expect that interacting with the person will be aversive and might try to avoid doing so. Evidence of this is suggested in a study by Bernstein et al. (1983). They invited male participants to participate in a movie-rating exercise and offered them a choice of two TV monitors to watch. Two chairs were in front of each monitor about 3 inches apart, and either a highly attractive or relatively unattractive woman was seated in front of one of the monitors. Some participants were told that different movies would be shown on the two monitors, whereas others were told that the same movie would be shown on both. When the woman was highly attractive, they were more likely to sit beside her when different movies were being shown on the monitors than when the same movie was shown. This was apparently because they had an excuse for affiliating in the former condition (i.e., their preference for one movie over the other). If this excuse was unavailable, the men s fear about being negatively evaluated led them to avoid the attractive female. Although purchasing a product or service can give consumers a clear excuse to affiliate with an attractive salesperson of the opposite sex, they may nevertheless avoid doing so if their self-presentation concerns are activated. As noted earlier, however, another s physical attractiveness is unlikely to be a sufficient condition for activating concerns about one s self-presentation. Both individual differences and situational factors can also come into play. Some individuals have chronic social anxiety and may generally be apprehensive about interacting with an attractive other regardless of other considerations (Wenzel and Emerson 2009). In other cases, a recent experience that has decreased individuals feelings of self-efficacy might influence their perception that they can interact effectively with others. Moreover, situational factors that lead individuals to anticipate feeling embarrassed can increase their apprehension about their ability to create a good impression independently of the chronic anxiety they experience. The latter possibility is of particular relevance in consumer situations. Feelings of embarrassment can result from public actions that observers might consider foolish or inappropriate (Dahl et al. 2001). These actions threaten the positive public image that people try to convey and can impair the effectiveness of their social interactions (Brown 1970; Miller 1987; Schlenker and Leary 1982). Because embarrassment is aversive, people are likely to avoid situations that perpetuate these feelings (Apsler 1975). In consumer situations, self-presentation concerns are likely to be activated by the anticipation of purchasing a product or service that is typically used privately or calls attention to undesirable personal attributes (e.g., condoms, breast pads, weight-loss pills). Such purchases are particularly likely to elicit embarrassment when the salesperson is attractive and of the opposite sex. Therefore, people may be especially motivated to avoid interacting with such a salesperson. (For evidence that fear of embarrassment decreases affiliation, see Buunk and Hoorens 1992; Dong, Huang, and Wyer 2013; Sarnoff and Zimbardo 1961.)

4 WAN AND WYER 581 Self-Presentation Concerns in Same-Sex Interactions The conditions described in the previous section are likely to be evident in service situations in which the salesperson or service provider is an attractive person of the opposite sex. However, self-presentation concerns could also induce avoidance of interactions with attractive same-sex providers as well, albeit for different reasons. Attractive members of the same sex are generally regarded as potential threats to a person s status and self-esteem in social interactions (Agthe, Spörrle, and Maner 2011; Maner et al. 2009). In this case, self-presentation concerns are more likely to be driven by a social comparison process. For one thing, people are often more attentive to attractive members of the same sex because they pose threats to their social status (Maner et al. 2009). Thus they try to impress members of the same sex by buying luxury products in intrasex competitions (Hudders et al. 2014; Wang and Griskevicius 2014). However, comparisons with an ostensibly superior person can lead to jealousy and can decrease self-esteem (Tesser and Collins 1988; see also Lockwood and Kunda 1997). These reactions are particularly likely when the individual to whom one compares oneself is of the same sex. Attractive same-sex individuals stimulate upward comparison and consequently threaten self-esteem (Agthe et al. 2011; Kenrick et al. 1993; Wood 1989). Women often compare their appearance to that of attractive female peers (Jones and Buckingham 2005) and models (Dittmar and Howard 2004; Richins 1991). These comparisons can elicit negative affect and decrease their satisfaction with their own physical attractiveness. Thus although people are in a positive mood after being exposed to attractive opposite-sex faces, they are in a negative mood after viewing attractive same-sex faces. In organizational contexts, both men and women react negatively to attractive job candidates of the same sex, being less likely to work with them and giving them poor performance evaluations (Agthe et al. 2011). Note that the different effects of physical attractiveness on reactions to same-sex and opposite-sex others may be reflected in liking for these persons (Sigall and Aronson 1969; Sigall and Landy 1973). Although consumers may be motivated to avoid interacting with attractive oppositesex salespersons when they experience self-presentation concerns, this avoidance does not necessarily decrease their liking for them. (As noted earlier, male participants may approach an attractive woman when they have an excuse for affiliating; Bernstein et al.1983.) However, if attractive same-sex providers stimulate upward comparisons, they are likely to feel inferior and experience envy (Agthe, Spörrle, and Maner 2010; Maner et al. 2009). Therefore, in contrast to their reactions to attractive opposite-sex others, consumers are likely to decrease their liking for an attractive same-sex provider. Summary The conceptualization we have outlined in the preceding discussion is summarized in Figure 1. We assume that when individuals encounter service providers of average attractiveness, their concerns about making a good impression are unlikely to be activated (Dong and Wyer 2014), and so they are unlikely to think about the quality of their interaction with the person. In this case, individuals desire to interact with a provider will increase with the provider s physical attractiveness, and this may be true regardless of whether the provider is of the same or the opposite sex. When the provider is physically attractive, however, they may have concerns about their self-presentation. These concerns could be activated by (1) individuals/ chronic social anxiety, (2) recent experiences that have had a negative impact on self-efficacy, and (3) characteristics of the interaction situation itself (e.g., the type of product or service being considered). When the provider is of the opposite sex, consumers are likely to experience evaluation apprehension, and their willingness to interact may decrease. When the provider is of the same sex, consumers are likely to engage in upward comparison and evaluate themselves negatively in relation to the provider. In this case, they may dislike the provider and be unwilling to interact for this reason. Thus activating self-presentation concern should generally decrease the willingness to interact with a highly attractive service provider, but the reason may depend on whether the provider is of the same or the opposite sex. The research to be reported confirms implications of this conceptualization. An observational study found that consumers with chronically high social anxiety react less favorably to attractive than to unattractive opposite-sex service providers in an actual retail environment. A laboratory replicated these findings when consumption was public but not when it was private. Study 2 demonstrated that the impact of self-presentation concerns on consumers reactions to an attractive opposite-sex service provider can be triggered by unrelated experiences that occur before the interaction takes place. Two other studies found that selfpresentation concerns can be elicited by embarrassing consumption experiences in an actual shopping environment and can decrease the tendency to interact with attractive opposite-sex providers in such an environment. A final study indicated that consumers also react less favorably to attractive same-sex service providers that they encounter in a potentially embarrassing consumption situation. PILOT STUDY Individuals with high social anxiety have stronger selfpresentation concerns than those with low anxiety (Arkin et al. 1980). To this extent, consumers with chronically high social anxiety should react less favorably to attractive

5 582 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH FIGURE 1 CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF CONSUMER REACTIONS TO ATTRACTIVE SERVICE PROVIDER Opposite Sex + Evaluation Apprehension Provider Provider Attractive? Yes Self-Presentation Concern (e.g., chronic anxiety, embarrassment, private vs. public. Etc.) Yes + Sex of Provider? Interact + Purchase Intention Same Sex Social Comparison than to unattractive opposite-sex service providers in an actual retail environment. An observational study confirmed this hypothesis. Method Otaku is a group of Hong Kong Chinese who have an obsessive interest in computer technology but are socially inept in interpersonal interactions (Niu, Chiang, and Tsai 2012). Therefore, we expected these individuals to be high No + No in social anxiety and have high self-presentation concerns. Two additional characteristics of Otaku are their love of collecting toys and figures, and their disposition to shop alone. Based on these assumptions, we conducted an observational study at a store in a Hong Kong shopping center (Richmond Shopping Arcade) that specializes in Japanese figures, models, and gifts and is a popular place for Otaku to shop. The store that we considered had recently opened and allowed us to conduct the study on two weekdays during the first month of its operation. Because we were interested in observing how consumers interact individually with salespersons, we conducted the study during periods in which the store traffic was minimal (i.e., 2 to 6 PM). Before conducting the study, we observed that about 70% of the customers who stopped and looked at the window display during this period were male. The size of the store was small (162 square feet), and potential customers could easily see the physical appearance of the salesperson through the store window. Two female confederates with different levels of attractiveness (age 22 years) were used as salespersons. The highly attractive confederate wore makeup to accentuate her attractiveness; the average-looking confederate did not. A pretest using a between-subject design (n ¼ 42) confirmed that one confederate (after wearing makeup) was more attractive than the other (8.28 vs along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 40) ¼ 56.78; p <.001). Two observers were used to collect data. One observer stood outside the store and counted the number of male consumers who entered the store. The other stayed in a small room near the cashier behind a one-way mirror and observed the interaction between the salesperson and the consumers. The observers recorded the total number of male consumers who stopped to take a look at the window display, the number who entered the store and took a look at the products, the number who interacted with the salesperson (e.g., asking about product information, buying the product, etc.), and the duration of time that the salesperson and consumer interacted. Finally, we determined the amount of consumers purchases from the store s sales receipts. (Because the majority of consumers were men and we were only interested in observing male consumers who interact with the salesperson alone, we excluded analyses for female consumers and male consumers who entered the store with friends.) Results The results of this study are consistent with predictions. A similar number of male consumers stopped to look at the window display in both high attractiveness (n ¼ 84) and average attractiveness (n ¼ 80) conditions. However, fewer male consumers entered the store when the salesperson was attractive (n ¼ 48) than when she was average looking (n ¼ 65; v 2 ¼ 11.11, p <.01). Ninety-eight of the 113 male

6 WAN AND WYER 583 customers who entered the store interacted with the salesperson. The proportion of customers who interacted with the salesperson did not depend on whether the salesperson was attractive (40.8%) or average looking (59.2%; v 2 < 1). However, the duration of their interaction was shorter when the salesperson was attractive than when she was not (4.30 min vs min; F(1, 97) ¼ 18.35; p <.001). Finally, fewer males made a purchase when the salesperson was attractive than when she was average looking (38 vs. 56; v 2 ¼ 10.27, p <.01), and the average cost of the products they purchased was less in the former condition than the latter (US$28.85 vs. US$41.49; F(1, 92) ¼ 4.98; p <.05). STUDY 1 Consumers avoidance of attractive salespersons in the pilot study was presumably a result of their chronic social anxiety and therefore of their concern with how they presented themselves to these persons. Study 1 confirmed this effect in a laboratory experiment. As Leary and Kowalski (1990) note, individuals may attempt to manage not only the impressions that others form of them but also their impressions of themselves. However, we expected that the self-presentation concerns that are stimulated by an opposite-sex other s physical attractiveness would only occur when consumption requires interacting with the provider and would not be evident in private consumption situation such as online shopping. We expected that in a public consumption situation, consumers with low chronic social anxiety would not be concerned about their ability to create a favorable impression on a highly attractive consultant and would be more willing to interact with such a consultant than with a less attractive one. When consumers have high chronic anxiety, however, we expected the opposite to be true. In contrast, this difference should not be evident when interacting with the consultant is not in person. Method To identify participants who were high or low in social anxiety, 320 male participants completed an unrelated consumer survey two weeks prior to the main study. The survey included a seven item social anxiety scale (e.g., I often feel nervous even in casual get-togethers ) adopted from Leary (1983). Participants with a high (low) social anxiety score that was 1.00 above (below) the median (a ¼.86; median ¼ 5.43; along a scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 10 (Strongly agree)) were invited by phone to participate in another 15-minute study with HK$20 (approximately US$3). A total of 176 men agreed to participate in the study and were randomly assigned to conditions of a 2 (consumption conditions: public vs. private) 2 (consultant attractiveness: high vs. average) 2 (social anxiety: high vs. low) between-subjects design, with 22 participants in each cell. In the experiment, male participants imagined themselves to be interacting with a female consultant at a new university health care service center. Pictures were used to manipulate the attractiveness level of the consultant. The consultant in each picture was smiling and wearing a white shirt but differed in attractiveness. A pretest (n ¼ 50) using a between-subject design confirmed that one consultant was more attractive than the other (8.12 vs along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 48) ¼ 70.43; p <.001). In private consumption conditions, participants were told to imagine that they would use an online system to send their health information to the consultant for assessment and get a report with advice. In public consumption conditions, however, they were told to imagine that they would interact with the consultant to obtain health care advice. Participants then reported their intention to make an appointment with the consultant and their willingness to give health care information to the consultant along scales from 1 (Very unlikely) to 10 (Very likely). They also indicated their liking for the consultant along a scale from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Very much) and reported their agreement that the consultant was attractive along a scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 10 (Strongly agree). Results Manipulation Check. The attractiveness manipulation was successful; the highly attractive consultant was rated more positively than the average consultant (8.03 vs. 4.92, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ , p <.001). Intentions to Patronize. Participants intentions to patronize the health care center are summarized in the top section of Table 1 as a function of consumption conditions, consultant attractiveness, and social anxiety. Analyses of these data indicated that participants were generally more likely to patronize the health center when the consultant was highly attractive than when she was average looking (6.85 vs. 6.19, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 9.19, p <.01). However, this difference is qualified by an interaction of attractiveness and social anxiety (F(1, 168) ¼ 4.62, p <.05) and a three-way interaction of these variables and consumption conditions (F(1, 168) ¼ 6.85, p <.05). The form of the latter interaction is consistent with expectations. As shown in Table 1, participants who imagined communicating privately had greater intentions to patronize the health center when the consultant was highly attractive than when she was less so (7.13 vs. 6.21, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 9.27, p <.01), and this was true regardless of whether participants chronic anxiety was high (7.20 vs. 6.18, respectively) or low (7.05 vs. 6.23, respectively). The provider s attractiveness had a similar effect

7 584 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH TABLE 1 CONSUMER REACTIONS TO SERVICE PROVIDERS WITH DIFFERENT ATTRACTIVENESS LEVELS: STUDY 1 Private consumption Public consumption when low-anxiety participants imagined interacting with her in person (7.55 vs when attractiveness was high vs. low, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 27.59, p <.001). If participants had high anxiety, however, they reported less intention to patronize the health center when the consultant was attractive than when she was less so (5.61 vs. 6.25, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 5.77, p <.05). Intentions to Provide Information. Participants intentions to provide information, shown in the second section of Table 1, were affected similarly. Analyses of these judgments indicated that participants were generally more likely to give information in private than in public consumption conditions (7.09 vs. 6.48, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 9.54, p <.01) and more likely to do so when the consultant was highly attractive than she was moderately so (7.14 vs. 6.43, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 12.58, p <.01). Low-anxious participants were generally more likely to give information than high-anxious participants were (7.05 vs. 6.53, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 6.93, p <.01). However, the three-way interaction of consultant attractiveness, anxiety, and consumption conditions was also significant (F(1, 168) ¼ 3.78, p <.05). Participants reported a greater likelihood of communicating information privately to a highly attractive consultant than to a less attractive one regardless of their chronic anxiety (7.62 vs. 6.57, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 14.55, p <.001). When they imagined communicating in person, however, this was true only when their anxiety was low (7.64 vs. 6.36, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 21.34, p <.001). When participants were high in anxiety, they were less inclined to interact with the attractive consultant than with the less attractive one (5.68 vs. 6.23, respectively; F(1, 168) ¼ 4.41, p <.05). Liking for the Consultant. Although high-anxious participants reported less willingness to interact with a very attractive consultant than with a moderately attractive one in the public consumption, this was not true of their liking for her. Analyses of consultant evaluations, shown in the last Moderate attractiveness High attractiveness Moderate attractiveness High attractiveness Intention to patronize High anxiety 6.18 a (1.48) 7.20 c (1.38) 6.25 a (1.45) 5.61 b (1.51) Low anxiety 6.23 a (1.69) 7.05 c (1.43) 6.09 ab (1.32) 7.55 c (1.34) Intention to provide information High anxiety 6.64 a (1.26) 7.55 c (1.30) 6.23 a (1.60) 5.68 b (1.25) Low anxiety 6.50 a (1.41) 7.68 c (1.29) 6.36 ab (1.18) 7.64 c (1.22) Liking High anxiety 5.77 a (1.02) 7.05 b (1.12) 5.86 a (1.25) 7.18 b (1.01) Low anxiety 5.59 a (1.33) 7.14 b (1.13) 5.68 a (1.29) 7.41 b (0.91) NOTE. Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviations. Means with unlike subscripts are significantly different at p <.05. section of Table 1, yielded only a main effect of consultant attractiveness (F(1, 168) ¼ 72.64, p <.001), indicating that participants expected to like the consultant more when she was very attractive (M ¼ 7.20) than when she was moderately attractive (M ¼ 5.73). This difference was similar regardless of whether participants were high or low in social anxiety. Discussion The results of study 1 show that when participants imagined themselves interacting privately with the health consultant, they generally reported greater intentions to use the service and to provide personal information when the consultant was highly attractive. When participants imagined interacting publicly with the consultant, however, this was only true among consumers who were chronically low in social anxiety. When participants were high in anxiety, they were less willing to use the service and provide information when the consultant was physically attractive. Thus although individuals with high social anxiety like highly attractive individuals as much or more than less attractive ones, their concern over creating a poor impression leads them to avoid interacting with these individuals. This latter effect, which was replicated in the next three studies to be reported, distinguishes the effects of an opposite-sex provider s attractiveness on liking for the provider from the effects of a same-sex provider s attractiveness. We return to this difference in study 5. STUDY 2 Study 1 provided evidence that consumers chronic social anxiety generates self-presentation concerns and consequently decreases their willingness to interact with a highly attractive service provider. However, these concerns can also be influenced by recent experiences in which a person has been evaluated negatively and his or her

8 WAN AND WYER 585 self-efficacy is called into question. Study 2 investigated this possibility. Method A total of 144 participants (74 men) were paid HK$20 (approximately US$3) to take part in a 15 minute experiment. They were randomly assigned to cells of a 2 (consultant attractiveness: high vs. average) 2 (self-presentation priming conditions: high self-presentation concern vs. low concern) between-subjects design. (Consultants were of the opposite sex in all conditions.) Participants took part in two ostensibly unrelated experiments. They were told that the purpose of the first study was to understand people s reactions to job interview situations. On this pretense, they were asked to imagine a situation in which they needed to have an important job interview. In high self-presentation concern conditions, they imagined that they did not have enough time to prepare for the interview, whereas in low concern conditions, they imagined that they had plenty of time to do so. Participants were asked to put themselves into the situation and to form as vivid an image as possible. After reading the scenario, they listed their thoughts and feelings about their anticipated performance in the interview. After a five minute break, participants then performed an ostensibly unrelated experiment in which they reported their reactions to a new personalized health care service under conditions identical to the public consumption conditions of study 1. That is, they viewed a picture of an opposite-sex consultant beside a health care counter. They were told to imagine that they would interact with the consultant to obtain health care advice. Pretests using a between-subject design were conducted to assess consultants attractiveness. Twenty-six male participants evaluated the high and moderate attractiveness female pictures as 8.07 and 5.54, respectively, along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 24) ¼ 55.37; p <.001). Correspondingly, 26 females rated the two male pictures as 8.08 and 5.15, respectively F(1, 24) ¼ 33.93, p <.001). Participants were then given a questionnaire to assess their intention to make an appointment with the consultant and their willingness to give their health care information to the consultant along scales from 1 (Very unlikely) to 10 (Very likely). They also indicated their liking for the consultant along a scale from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Very much) and reported their agreement that the consultant was attractive along a scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 10 (Strongly agree). They also indicated the expected duration of their interaction with the consultant in minutes. Results Manipulation Check. Two independent coders rated each response generated during the priming task. Interrater agreement was 91%, and disagreements were resolved through discussion. As expected, participants in self-presentation concern conditions, relative to no-concern conditions, listed more thoughts that conveyed worry about their performance and the impression they made (e.g., I am so worried about my interview, I feel nervous about my performance ) (2.21 vs. 0.49, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ , p <.001). In contrast, they listed relatively fewer thoughts that conveyed confidence in their performance (e.g., I am confident in the interview as I am well prepared ) (0.48 vs. 2.03, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ , p <.001). The attractiveness manipulation was also successful. Male participants evaluated the highly and moderately attractiveness female consultants as 8.22 and 5.02, respectively (F(1, 70) ¼ ; p <.001). Correspondingly, women rated the highly attractive and moderately attractive male consultant as 8.06 and 5.11, respectively; F(1, 66) ¼ , p <.001). Preliminary analyses of each dependent variable as a function of priming, consultant attractiveness, and sex of subject yielded no significant effects involving the latter variable. Therefore, data are pooled over this variable in the analyses to follow. Intentions to Patronize. Participants intentions to patronize the health care center are shown in the first section of Table 2 as a function of priming and consultant attractiveness. Analyses of these data yielded a main effect of priming (F(1, 140) ¼ 7.52, p <.01) and an interaction of priming and attractiveness conditions (F(1, 140) ¼ 13.87, p <.001). As shown in Table 2, participants with low selfpresentation concerns were more inclined to patronize the center when the consultant was very attractive than when he or she was moderately so (7.20 vs. 6.36, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ 19.10, p <.001). When participants selfpresentation concerns were high, however, they were less TABLE 2 CONSUMER REACTIONS TO SERVICE PROVIDERS WITH DIFFERENT ATTRACTIVENESS LEVELS: STUDY 2 Moderate attractiveness High attractiveness Intention to patronize High self-presentation concern 6.55 a (1.13) 5.92 b (1.04) Low self-presentation concern 6.36 a (1.02) 7.20 c (1.05) Intention to provide information High self-presentation concern 7.12 a (1.26) 6.13 b (1.42) Low self-presentation concern 6.94 a (1.35) 7.56 c (1.33) Interaction time (in minutes) High self-presentation concern a (11.49) b (11.54) Low self-presentation concern a (11.50) c (13.34) Liking High self-presentation concern 6.47 a (1.44) 7.10 b (1.13) Low self-presentation concern 6.72 a (1.21) 7.34 b (1.75) NOTE. Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviations. Means with unlike subscripts are significantly different at p <.05.

9 586 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH inclined to do so in the former case (5.92 vs. 6.55, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ 10.81, p <.001). Intentions to Provide Information. The effects of selfpresentation concerns and consultant attractiveness on intentions to provide information to the consultant were very similar to their effects on intentions to patronize the health center. The main effect of priming and its interaction with attractiveness conditions were significant (F(1, 140) ¼ 8.15, p <.01 and F(1, 140) ¼ 8.39, p <.01, respectively). As shown in the second section of Table 2, participants were more willing to give information to a highly attractive consultant than to a moderately attractive consultant when their self-presentation concerns were low (7.56 vs. 6.94, F(1, 140) ¼ 8.49, p <.01), but the opposite was true when their self-presentation concerns were high (6.13 vs. 7.12, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ 19.37, p <.001). Interaction Duration. The time that participants imagined interacting with the consultant was affected in much the same way. The effect of priming and its interaction with attractiveness were again significant (F(1, 140) ¼ 8.15, p <.01 and F(1, 140) ¼ 8.39, p <.01, respectively). As shown in the third section of Table 2, participants imagined spending more time interacting with the highly attractive consultant than with the moderately attractive one when their self-presentation concerns were low (29.98 minutes vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ 10.06, p <.01) but were less willing to do so when their self-presentation concerns were high (18.54 minutes vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 140) ¼ 6.90, p <.01). Liking for the Consultant. Although participants with high self-presentation concerns reported less willingness to interact with a very attractive consultant than with a moderately attractive one, this was not true of their liking for him or her. Analyses of consultant evaluations, shown in the last section of Table 2, yielded only a main effect of consultant attractiveness (F(1, 140) ¼ 7.27, p <.01), indicating that participants expected to like her more when she was very attractive (M ¼ 7.22) than when she was moderately attractive (M ¼ 6.60), and this was true regardless of whether participants had concern about their self-presentation or not. Discussion The results of study 2 provided further evidence that when participants self-presentation concerns are relatively low, they have a greater intention to interact with a highly attractive service provider than with a moderately attractive one. When they are concerned about the impression they create, however, the reverse is true. This difference is evident even when self-presentation concerns are activated in a situation that is ostensibly quite unrelated to the consumption situation in which the interaction is contemplated. Moreover, it occurs despite the fact that persons believe they would like the attractive consultant more. Thus although individuals who are concerned with their self-presentation have more favorable reactions to highly attractive individuals than to less attractive ones, their concern over creating a bad impression overrides these reactions and leads them to avoid interacting with the individuals. STUDY 3 Study 2 demonstrated that self-presentation concerns can be situationally primed, carrying over from one context to another. However, these concerns can also be induced by features of the consumption situation itself. As noted earlier, the consumption of some products or services (e.g., condoms, hemorrhoid creams, medical checkups, and weight-loss services) can generate embarrassment (Dahl et al. 2001), an emotional state that threatens an individual s desired social identity in social interactions. Embarrassing consumption is likely to endanger the positive self-image one is motivated to convey in social situations, and this may be particularly true when others in the situation are attractive and thus are people one wants to impress. If this is so, consumers may wish to avoid interacting with attractive providers in an embarrassing consumption situation. The next two studies indicate that this is in fact the case. Method Study 3 was conducted in a health care products retail setting. Consumers reactions to attractive (vs. unattractive) providers were assessed in embarrassing and nonembarrassing consumption situations and employed a 2 (attractive: high vs. average) 2 (consumption conditions: embarrassing vs. not embarrassing) between-subjects design. We contacted a company that regularly organizes road shows to sell health care products at major hospitals in Hong Kong. The company has two different product lines concerning body care and foot care and was launching a new product in each line: a thermal waist belt (a product to facilitate weight loss) and a foot insole (for use in relieving foot pressure and pain). A pretest with 40 participants showed that consumers associated a higher level of embarrassment with purchasing the thermal waist belt than with purchasing the foot insole (6.06 vs. 2.67, respectively, along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 38) ¼ ; n ¼ 40). According to the company s experience, about 70% of road show consumers in hospitals (patients family and friends, administrative staff, nurses, and doctors) were women from 25 to 50 years of age. The company arranged to have two part-time male salespersons (age 31 and 33 years, respectively) with different levels of physical

10 WAN AND WYER 587 attractiveness. A pretest of their attractiveness with 40 participants using a between-subject design indicated that one of them was more attractive than the other (7.90 vs. 5.25, respectively, along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 38) ¼ 71.07). We therefore expected that consumers would be more likely to interact with the attractive salesperson when he was selling foot insoles but would be less likely to interact with this person when he was selling thermal waist belts. The road show was conducted at a major hospital in Hong Kong between 10 AM and 6 PM (excluding lunch time). The company set up two booths at the show about 5 meters apart, one of which displayed each type of product. One salesperson manned each booth, switching their position every hour. To measure consumer purchasing decisions, two observers counted the total number of people who (1) stopped and looked at the products and (2) interacted with the salesperson (asking about the product, etc.). The observers also recorded the length of the interactions between the salesperson and consumers. In addition, we determined the number of purchases that consumers made from the company s receipts. (The price of each thermal waist belt was around US$84, and the price for each pair of foot insoles was around US$80.) Results A total of 239 people walked by the booths; 80.3% of the consumers were women between the ages of 30 and 50. The thermal waist belt was designed for women, and a few male consumers purchased the thermal waist belts as a gift. However, we restricted our consideration to female consumers who were shopping alone. Number of People Who Stopped. A total of 153 female consumers stopped to look at the products. Of these, 78 stopped to look at the nonembarrassing product, and 75 stopped to look at the embarrassing product. However, although more consumers stopped at the nonembarrassing products booth when the salesperson was attractive (n ¼ 44) than when he was average looking (n ¼ 34), fewer consumers stopped at the embarrassing product in the former condition than in the latter (29 vs. 46). The difference in these proportions was significant (v 2 ¼ 4.83, p <.05). Interaction Duration. Of the 153 women who stopped at the booths, 92 interacted with the salesperson and made a purchase and 17 interacted but did not make a purchase. Of the 44 consumers who did not interact with the salesperson, only 7 made a purchase. When the product was not embarrassing, more consumers interacted with the attractive salesperson (n ¼ 36) than with the average-looking one (n ¼ 20) and spent relatively more time doing so (4.44 minutes vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 105) ¼ 7.24; p <.01). When the product was embarrassing, however, consumers interacted less often with the attractive salesperson (n ¼ 20) than with the average-looking one (n ¼ 33) and, if they interacted, spent relatively less time doing so (2.95 minutes vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 105) ¼ 36.71; p <.001). Purchases. As predicted, more consumers made a purchase of the nonembarrassing product when the salesperson was attractive (n ¼ 34) than when he was not (n ¼ 19), whereas fewer consumers made a purchase of the embarrassing product when the salesperson was attractive (n ¼ 16) than when he was not (n ¼ 30). This difference was significant (v 2 ¼ 8.50, p <.01). Analyses of the amount of money spent on purchases yielded a marginally significant interaction of attractiveness and embarrassment conditions (F(1, 95) ¼ 3.79, p ¼.05). The price of the embarrassing and nonembarrassing products was similar (US$84 vs. US$80, respectively). However, consumers spent more money on purchases of the nonembarrassing product when the salespersonwasattractivethanwhenhewasnot(us$117.65vs. US$101.50, respectively; F(1, 95) ¼ 4.85; p <.05) but spent less money on purchases of the embarrassing product in the former case than in the latter (US$89.25 vs. US$103.60, respectively; F(1, 95) ¼ 3.62; p ¼.06). Discussion Study 3 provided evidence that consumers are actually more likely to interact with a physically attractive salesperson than with an unattractive one when they are buying nonembarrassing products but are less likely to do so if they are buying embarrassing products. Although the results of this field study are provocative, however, we were unable to examine the factors that mediated the impact of embarrassing consumption on consumer reactions. Furthermore, the study used different product lines (i.e., body care and foot care) to exemplify embarrassing and nonembarrassing consumption. Finally, mood was not assessed in either this or other studies we have reported. It is possible that seeing an attractive (vs. unattractive) provider induces a positive mood and results in more favorable consumer reactions when self-presentation concern is not activated but leads to a more negative mood and to stronger negative reactions when self-presentation concern exists. Study 4, a laboratory study, eliminated these ambiguities. STUDY 4 Method A total of 132 female participants received HK$50 (around US$6.41) for participating. They were assigned to cells of a 2 (salesperson attractiveness: high vs. average) 2 (consumption conditions: embarrassing vs. not embarrassing) between-subjects design.

11 588 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH Participants were told that a company wanted to receive feedback about a new product (thermal waist belt, the body care product used in study 3) and their sales representatives. Participants were run individually in a room that simulated a retail setting, showing the product display, leaflets, and advertising posters (see appendix). The leaflets and posters contained pictures and text messages that manipulated the embarrassment level of the product. In embarrassing consumption conditions, the materials indicated that the primary function of the waist belt was to reduce weight and contained a picture of the front of a model using the waist belt (without showing her face). In the nonembarrassing consumption condition, the materials indicated that the waist belt was intended to relax muscles, improve circulation, and alleviate lower back pain. A pretest confirmed that consumers a priori preference for these functions did not differ (5.92 vs. 5.68; respectively; F < 1). A physically attractive male confederate (age 26 years) served as the salesperson. In the high attractiveness condition, he styled his hair and wore a T-shirt that fitted him well and flattered his shape. In the average attractiveness condition, the same confederate was ungroomed and wore an oversize T-shirt and a pair of glasses to detract from his appearance. A pretest (n ¼ 38) using a between-subject design confirmed the difference in the confederate s attractiveness in the two conditions (8.05 vs. 5.94, respectively, along a scale from 1 (Not attractive at all) to 10 (Very attractive); F(1, 36) ¼ 62.81, p <.001). Participants in each condition had a chance to touch the product, to read the product information, and to interact with the confederate (e.g., asking for more product information, asking for help on a free trial, etc.). After doing so, they completed a short questionnaire. First, participants reported their agreement with two statements, I would buy this product, and I would be interested in using the product along a scale from 1 (Very unlikely) to 10 (Very likely). Responses to these items were correlated.69 and were averaged to provide an index of purchase intentions. They then reported their liking for the salesperson ( I like the salesperson ) and the extent to which they felt nervous when interacting with the salesperson ( I felt nervous during the interaction ), along scales from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Very much). The latter item was used to measure their self-presentation concern (Schlenker and Leary 1982). As manipulation checks, participants then reported their agreement that The salesperson is physically attractive and The product is embarrassing along scales from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 10 (Strongly agree). Finally, they reported their mood (a ¼.86) along three scales from 1 (Sad/ bad/depressed) to 10 (Happy/good/cheerful). In addition, an observer watched participants interaction with the confederate through a one-way mirror and recorded the duration of their interaction. Results and Discussion Our manipulations of both physical attractiveness and embarrassing consumption were successful. Participants judged the salesperson to be more attractive in the high attractiveness condition than in the average attractiveness condition (8.17 vs. 5.53, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ , p <.001). Furthermore, participants felt the product was more embarrassing in embarrassing consumption conditions than in nonembarrassing consumption conditions (6.11 vs. 4.38, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 67.16, p <.001). We expected that participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions would have higher purchase intentions and would interact a longer time with the attractive salesperson than with the average-looking one. However, we expected participants in embarrassing consumption conditions to have a lower purchase intention and to interact for less time when the salesperson was attractive than when he was not. Intention to Purchase. Participants intentions to purchase the product are shown in Table 3 as a function of attractiveness level and embarrassing consumption. The interaction of these variables was significant (F(1, 128) ¼ 10.25, p <.01) and of the form expected. That is, participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions reported a greater intention to purchase the product when the salesperson was attractive than when he was not (6.50 vs. 5.61, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 11.92; p <.001). However, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions reported less intention to purchase when the salesperson was attractive than when he was not (5.29 vs. 6.08, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 9.48; p <.01). TABLE 3 CONSUMER REACTIONS TO SERVICE PROVIDERS WITH DIFFERENT ATTRACTIVENESS LEVELS: STUDY 4 Moderate attractiveness High attractiveness Intention to patronize Nonembarrassing 5.61 ac (1.27) 6.50 b (1.80) Embarrassing 6.08 ab (1.07) 5.29 c (1.77) Interaction time, minutes Nonembarrassing 4.06 a (1.32) 5.09 b (1.28) Embarrassing 3.52 a (1.42) 2.76 c (1.30) Liking Nonembarrassing 4.88 a (1.75) 6.70 b (1.78) Embarrassing 5.00 a (1.54) 6.85 b (1.77) Self-presentation concern Nonembarrassing 3.67 a (1.22) 3.48 a (1.44) Embarrassing 3.97 a (1.47) 5.30 b (1.59) Mood Nonembarrassing 5.59 a (1.22) 6.65 b (1.44) Embarrassing 5.56 a (1.47) 6.16 c (1.59) NOTE. Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviations. Means with unlike subscripts are significantly different at p <.05.

12 WAN AND WYER 589 Interaction Duration. The duration of participants interaction with the salesperson is summarized in the second section of Table 3. Participants were generally more likely to interact with the salesperson in nonembarrassing consumption conditions than in embarrassing conditions (4.58 vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 38.57; p <.001). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was significant (F(1, 128) ¼ 14.88, p <.001) and indicated that participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions interacted longer with the attractive salesperson than with the average-looking one (5.09 min vs min, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 20.31; p <.001). In contrast, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions interacted for a shorter time with the attractive salesperson than with the average-looking one (2.76 minutes vs minutes, respectively; F (1, 128) ¼ ; p <.01). Liking for the Salesperson. Participants reported greater liking for the highly attractive salesperson than for the moderately attractive one (6.78 vs. 4.94; F(1, 128) ¼ 37.69, p <.001) and, as in other experiments, this was true regardless of whether participants were embarrassed (6.85 vs. 5.00) or not (6.70 vs. 4.88). In other words, embarrassing consumption did not have a negative impact on participants liking of the attractive consultant; it only affected their willingnesstointeractwithhim. Self-Presentation Concern. The self-presentation concern that participants reported are shown in the third section of Table 3. Participants reported greater concern about their self-presentation in embarrassing consumption conditions than in nonembarrassing consumption conditions (4.64 vs. 3.58, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 18.03, p <.001). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was also significant (F(1, 128) ¼ 9.20, p <.01) and indicated that participants in embarrassing consumption conditions had greater concern with the impression they created when the salesperson was attractive than when he was average looking (5.30 vs. 3.97, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 16.26, p <.001). In nonembarrassing consumption conditions, however, this was not true (3.48 vs. 3.67, respectively; F < 1). Mediation. We assumed that the interactive effects of attractiveness and embarrassing consumption on purchase intentions would be mediated by their effects on self-presentation concerns. Bootstrapping analyses confirmed this assumption; based on 5000 samples, the indirect path of self-presentation concerns was significant; that is, a model of attractiveness embarrassing consumption! selfpresentation concern! purchase intention (model 8, Hayes, 2013) was supported; 95% confidence interval (CI), from to.3687). Participants had a more positive mood when the salesperson was highly attractive than when he was average looking (6.41 vs. 5.58, respectively; F(1, 128) ¼ 14.55, p <.001). However, additional analyses using mood as a mediator in each embarrassing and nonembarrassing consumption condition showed mood to have no effect (p >.10). In summary, study 4 confirmed the implications of study 3. That is, consumers react more favorably to an attractive salesperson when they are buying nonembarrassing products, but they react more negatively to an attractive salesperson when they are buying embarrassing products. Moreover, these reactions influence purchase intentions. Finally, our results confirm the assumption that the effect of embarrassing consumption on purchase intentions was mediated by self-presentation concerns and the impact of these concerns on interaction behavior. Although feelings of anxiety are likely to reflect self-presentation concerns (Schlenker and Leary 1982), two additional items adapted from Leary et al. (1994) were used to measure this construct in study 5. STUDY 5 The preceding study demonstrated that when a consumption situation is likely to be embarrassing, attractive opposite-sex providers can lead consumers to have selfpresentation concerns and that when this occurs, it has a detrimental effect on purchase decisions. In contrast, study 5 examined consumers reactions to attractive providers of the same sex. We expected consumers to react more favorably to an attractive (vs. unattractive) same-sex provider when consumption was not embarrassing and did not activate self-presentation concerns. However, we expected that attractive same-sex providers would lead consumers to have self-presentation concerns when the product being considered was potentially embarrassing and would resist interacting with these providers. In contrast to interactions with attractive opposite-sex providers, however, we expected self-presentation concerns to be driven by a social comparison motive. Thus in contrast to their effect on individuals reactions to attractive opposite-sex providers, these concerns should decrease consumers liking for attractive same-sex providers. We expected that when they anticipated interacting with a same-sex salesperson, participants in an embarrassing consumption situation would be less willing to interact with a highly attractive same-sex salesperson than with an average-looking one, and that this disposition would be accompanied by a higher social comparison tendency, more negative emotions, and a decreased perception of personal attractiveness. In nonembarrassing consumption conditions, however, these differences were not expected. Method A total of 112 female undergraduate students received HK$30 (around US$3.85) for participating. They were

13 590 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH assigned to cells of a 2 (salesperson attractiveness: high vs. average) 2 (consumption conditions: embarrassing vs. not embarrassing) between-subjects design. Participants were told to imagine that a company wanted to receive feedback from consumers about having a thermal wear product and their sales representatives. On this pretense, their attention was directed to a computer screen showing a picture of a same-sex salesperson and some pictures of the thermal wear with product descriptions. They were asked to imagine themselves interacting with the salesperson. A physically attractive female confederate (age 28 years) served as the salesperson in the picture. In the high attractiveness condition, she styled her hair, wore makeup, and wore a T-shirt that fitted her well and flattered her shape and looked thin. In the average attractiveness condition, the same confederate did not wear makeup and wore an oversize T-shirt and a pair of glasses to detract from her appearance. A pretest using a between-subject design (n ¼ 56) confirmed the confederate s difference in attractiveness in the two conditions (8.10 vs. 4.96, respectively, along a scale from 1 to 10; F(1, 54) ¼ , p <.001). Written messages were used to manipulate the embarrassment level of the product. In embarrassing consumption conditions, the materials indicated that the primary function of the thermal wear was to reduce weight and keep the body slim during the winter. In the nonembarrassing consumption condition, the materials indicated that the thermal wear was intended to keep the body warm during the winter. A pretest confirmed that consumers a priori preference for these functions did not differ (6.96 vs. 6.73; respectively; F < 1). After reading the information, participants were told that in understanding people s responses to situations, it was often helpful to know their perceptions of themselves. On this pretense, they were asked to rate themselves with respect to six attributes (e.g., intelligent, honest, dependable), one of which was physically attractive. These ratings were reported along scales from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Very). They then responded to measures of purchase intention and liking that were identical to those employed in study 4 except that an additional two items adapted from Leary et al. (1994) were used to measure self-presentation concerns. Responses to the three items (When you thought about interacting with the salesperson, to what extent did you Think about how the salesperson would evaluate you? Imagine how the salesperson would perceive you? and Feel nervous? ) were reported along a scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 10 (Strongly agree) and were averaged (a ¼.83). They also estimated the likely duration of their interaction with the salesperson in minutes. Participants social comparison motivation was inferred from responses to three items adapted from Richins (1991) (e.g., To what extent did you think about how well or how badly you look compared to the salesperson? ) along a scale from 1 (Definitely no) to 10 (Definitely yes) and were averaged (a ¼.86). Finally, participants reported their general mood along three scales from 1 (Sad/bad/ depressed) to 10 (Happy/good/cheerful) (a ¼.78) and indicated their feelings of jealousy along a scale from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Extremely). Results and Discussion Both physical attractiveness and embarrassing consumption were successfully manipulated. Participants judged the salesperson to be more attractive in the high attractiveness condition than in the average attractiveness condition (8.04 vs. 4.91, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 80.26, p <.001). Furthermore, participants felt the product was more embarrassing in embarrassing consumption than in nonembarrassing consumption conditions (6.27 vs. 3.16, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 80.66, p <.001). We expected that female participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions would have a greater liking for the salesperson, interact a longer time with her, and have higher purchase intentions when she was attractive than when she was average looking. However, we expected participants in embarrassing consumption conditions to like the salesperson less, to imagine interacting with her for less time, and to have less intention to make a purchase when the salesperson was attractive than when she was not. Intention to Purchase. Participants intentions to purchase the product are shown in Table 4 as a function of attractiveness level and embarrassing consumption. Participants reported a greater intention to purchase the product in nonembarrassing consumption conditions than in embarrassing consumption conditions (6.61 vs. 5.68, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 19.80; p <.001). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was also significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 10.57, p <.01) and of the form expected. That is, participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions reported a greater intention to purchase the product when the salesperson was attractive than when she was not (6.95 vs. 6.27, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 11.44; p <.01). In embarrassing consumption conditions, however, participants reported lower intentions to make a purchase when the salesperson was attractive than when she was not (5.34 vs. 6.02, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 11.45; p <.01). Interaction Duration. The duration of participants imagined interaction with the salesperson is summarized in the second section of Table 4. Participants were willing to interact with the salesperson for a longer time in nonembarrassing consumption conditions than in embarrassing consumption conditions (5.24 vs minutes, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 11.16; p <.01). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was also significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 6.04, p <.05)

14 WAN AND WYER 591 TABLE 4 CONSUMER REACTIONS TO SERVICE PROVIDERS WITH DIFFERENT ATTRACTIVENESS LEVELS: STUDY 5 Moderate attractiveness High attractiveness Intention to patronize Nonembarrassing 6.27 a (1.05) 6.95 b (1.17) Embarrassing 6.02 a (1.04) 5.34 c (1.16) Interaction time, minutes Nonembarrassing 4.79 a (1.59) 5.68 b (1.22) Embarrassing 4.54 a (1.32) 4.04 c (1.79) Liking Nonembarrassing 5.75 ac (1.21) 6.36 b (1.31) Embarrassing 5.96 a (1.35) 5.39 c (1.29) Self-presentation concern Nonembarrassing 4.37 a (1.05) 4.62 a (1.16) Embarrassing 4.64 a (1.19) 5.89 b (1.34) Social comparison Nonembarrassing 3.96 a (1.10) 4.23 a (1.07) Embarrassing 4.16 a (1.14) 5.21 b (1.10) Self-attractiveness rating Nonembarrassing 6.36 a (1.13) 6.25 a (1.24) Embarrassing 6.54 a (0.96) 5.61 b (1.10) Mood Nonembarrassing 5.14 a (1.11) 5.43 a (1.05) Embarrassing 5.10 a (1.07) 4.62 b (1.19) Jealousy Nonembarrassing 2.46 a (1.29) 3.21 b (1.48) Embarrassing 2.71 a (1.18) 4.79 c (1.50) NOTE. Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviations. Means with unlike subscripts are significantly different at p <.05. and indicated that participants in nonembarrassing consumption conditions imagined interacting longer with the attractive salesperson than with the average-looking one (5.68 min vs min, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 10.31; p <.01). In contrast, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions imagined interacting for a shorter time with the attractive salesperson than with the averagelooking one (4.04 min vs min, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 3.56; p ¼.06). Liking for the Salesperson. Participants liking for the salesperson is summarized in the third section of Table 4. The interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 5.86, p <.01). That is, participants in the nonembarrassing consumption condition liked the attractive salesperson more than the average-looking one (6.36 vs. 5.75, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 6.88; p <.01). In contrast to conditions in which the salesperson was of the opposite sex, however, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions liked the attractive salesperson less than the average-looking one (5.39 vs. 5.96, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 6.08; p <.05). Self-Presentation Concern. The self-presentation concern that participants reported are shown in the fourth section of Table 4. Participants reported more concern when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (5.26 vs. 4.51, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 11.11, p <.01), and more concern in embarrassing consumption conditions than in nonembarrassing conditions (5.27 vs. 4.50, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 11.83, p <.01). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was also significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 4.94, p <.05). That is, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions felt greater concern when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (5.89 vs. 4.64, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 31.56, p <.001). In nonembarrassing consumption conditions, however, this was not true (4.62 vs. 4.37, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 1.94, p >.10). Emotional Reactions to Social Comparison. We expected that a highly attractive salesperson would stimulate participants to engage in social comparison and that this would have negative consequences on both their selfperceptions of attractiveness and their reactions to the salesperson. Responses summarized in Table 4 confirm these expectations. Participants reported a greater tendency to engage in social comparison when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (4.72 vs. 4.06, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 10.02, p <.01), and in embarrassing conditions than in nonembarrassing conditions (4.69 vs. 4.10, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 7.97, p <.01). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was also marginally significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 3.65, p ¼.059). That is, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions had a higher social comparison tendency when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (5.21 vs. 4.16, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 26.6, p <.001). In nonembarrassing consumption conditions, however, this was not true (4.23 vs. 3.96, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 2.49, p >.10). As a result of these comparisons, participants reported their own attractiveness to be a lower rating when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was not (5.93 vs. 6.45, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 6.08, p <.05). However, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 3.83, p ¼.05) and indicated that this difference was evident in embarrassing consumption conditions (5.61 vs. 6.54, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 20.44, p <.001) but not in nonembarrassing consumption conditions (6.25 vs. 6.36, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 1.08, p >.10). Analyses of participants mood as a function of experimental conditions yielded a marginally significant interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions (F(1, 108) ¼ 3.33, p ¼.07). That is, participants in embarrassing consumption conditions experienced less positive moods when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (4.62 vs. 5.10, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 6.10, p <.05). In nonembarrassing consumption conditions, however, this was not true (5.43 vs. 5.14, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 2.74, p >.10).

15 592 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH Finally, participants reported feeling more jealous when the salesperson was highly attractive than when she was average looking (4.00 vs. 2.59, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 29.78, p <.001), and more jealous in embarrassing conditions than in nonembarrassing conditions (3.75 vs. 2.84, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 12.41, p <.01). As shown in Table 4, however, the interaction of salesperson attractiveness and embarrassment conditions was significant (F(1, 108) ¼ 6.09, p <.05) and indicated that the impact of physical attractiveness on jealousy was greater in embarrassing consumption conditions (4.79 vs. 2.71, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 32.37, p <.001) than in nonembarrassing conditions (3.21 vs. 2.46, respectively; F(1, 108) ¼ 4.21, p <.05). Mediation. To test the underlying process of the interactive effects of attractiveness and embarrassing consumption on purchase intention, mediation analyses using bootstrapping were performed. As in study 4, the interactive effects of attractiveness and embarrassment conditions on purchase intentions was significantly mediated by self-presentation concerns, that is, the model of attractiveness embarrassing consumption! self-presentation concern! purchase intention (model 8, Hayes, 2013) was supported; based on 5000 bootstrapping samples, 95% CI from.9609 to.0432). More important, a sequential mediation analyses indicated that the effects of self-presentation concerns were driven by a social comparison process; that is, a model of attractiveness embarrassing consumption! social comparison! self-presentation concern! purchase intention was also supported (model 6, Hayes, 2013; based on 5000 bootstrapping samples, 95% CI from.4043 to.0583). In summary, study 5 generalized the effects of previous experiments to same-sex interactions. That is, consumers react more favorably to an attractive same-sex salesperson than to an average-looking one when they are buying nonembarrassing products but react less favorably to an attractive salesperson than to an unattractive one when they are buying embarrassing products. (Note: These conclusions were confirmed in a preliminary study we conducted in which sufficient mediators were not assessed; for details, see the online appendix.) Moreover, these effects are mediated by self-presentation concerns. However, these concerns are driven by a social comparison motive, as reflected in an increased tendency to engage in social comparison, a decrease in self-perceptions of attractiveness, a decrease in mood, increased feelings of jealousy, and a decrease in liking for the salesperson. GENERAL DISCUSSION Consumers often react more favorably to physically attractive than to unattractive service providers, as evidenced by their greater satisfaction with the service and their intentions to purchase the products being sold (Ahearne et al. 1999; Argo et al. 2008; Reingen and Kernan 1993). To our knowledge, the conditions in which the physical attractiveness of a service provider can decrease as well as increase consumption behavior have not previously been examined. To this extent, our research not only identifies these conditions but also provides evidence of the mechanisms that underlie these effects. When consumers are motivated to make a good impression on an attractive provider, factors that make them feel apprehensive about their ability to accomplish this can lead them to anticipate that interacting with the provider will be unpleasant and to avoid doing so, decreasing their likelihood of purchasing the product or service at hand. These self-presentation concerns can be affected by chronic social anxiety or can be aroused by unrelated experiences that consumers have before being exposed to the service interactions. Moreover, it can also be activated by the type of product being sold. More important, although consumers interact the same way with an opposite-sex attractive salesperson and a same-sex attractive salesperson, our research findings show that different processes underlie the effects in the two cases. When the service provider is of the opposite sex, self-presentation concerns are driven by sexual motivation. Consumer avoidance of interacting with the provider is motivated by attraction to the provider and apprehension about conveying a positive impression. When the provider is of the same sex, however, self-presentation concerns appear to be driven by social comparison processes, leading consumers to dislike the provider and to avoid interacting for this reason. Our conceptualization of these effects intersects with theory and research in three different areas, concerning (1) the effect of the beautiful is good stereotype, (2) impression management motivation, and (3) the determinants and consequences of social anxiety and embarrassment. Each factor is a necessary condition for the adverse effect of a provider s attractiveness to occur. That is, when a provider is of only average attractiveness, individuals are likely to focus their attention on the product or service they are considering and are unlikely to be sensitive to the impression they create on the provider. When the provider is very attractive, consumers desire to make a good impression is increased (Dong and Wyer 2014), and if they feel relatively confident about their ability to create this impression, they may be more motivated to interact with the provider than they might otherwise be. However, if they are chronically anxious about their ability to create a good impression in social situations, or if recent experiences have decreased their confidence in their self-efficacy, these concerns may generalize to the interaction they anticipate having with the service provider and lead them to avoid engaging in this interaction. This avoidance motivation may also be activated by the type of product or service being considered, as studies 3, 4, and 5 indicate. The impact of embarrassment in the situations we investigated extend previous research in this area. Previous research has been mainly concerned with how

16 WAN AND WYER 593 embarrassment prevents consumers from achieving positive benefits, for example, purchasing condoms to have safe sex (Moore et al. 2006), having appropriate medical examination to save lives (Consedine, Krivoshekova, and Harris 2007; Consedine et al. 2011), and using in-store coupons to save money (Brumbaugh and Rosa 2009). With few exceptions (e.g., Dahl et al. 2001; Dong et al. 2013; Wan 2013), little attention has been given to the factors that influence consumers behavioral reactions to embarrassing consumption situations. To this extent, our research adds to our understanding of the moderating effect of embarrassment on consumers behavior in actual consumption situations and the conditions in which this effect occurs. In this regard, our research suggests constraints on the desirability of using attractive service providers to increase the sale of products. This strategy may indeed be effective when the product being promoted is not embarrassing. It can have an adverse effect, however, when the purchasing of the product is embarrassing. That said, it is important to note that the negative impact of attractive service providers is restricted to face-to-face situations in which the motive to create a favorable impression is relatively high. The use of attractive models or celebrities in advertisements for embarrassing products could have a positive effect on online shopping in which social interaction is not an issue. APPENDIX Study 4 Advertisement posters Embarrassing Consumption Cultural or dispositional variables might also moderate the relationship between attractiveness and consumer reactions to embarrassing consumptions. For example, people with an interdependent self-construal are more concerned with impression management than those with an independent self-construal (Lalwani, Shavitt, and Johnson 2006). If this is so, these differences should be particularly evident in embarrassing consumption situations. In contrast, if nonembarrassing consumptions do not elicit self-presentation concerns, differences in self-construal might not affect purchase behavior. This possibility is worth exploring. DATA COLLECTION INFORMATION The first author conducted the pilot study at the Hong Kong Richmond Shopping Arcade in the winter of 2013 and study 3 at the Hong Kong Prince of Wales Hospital in the spring of The first author supervised the collection of data for studies 1, 2, 4, and 5 by three research assistants at the Chinese University of Hong Kong in The first author supervised the collection of data for study 5 by a research assistant at the Chinese University of Hong Kong in the spring of Data from all studies were analyzed by the first author and were discussed by both authors. Non-Embarrassing Consumption

17 594 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH REFERENCES Agthe, Maria, Matthias Spörrle, and Jon K. Maner (2010), Don t Hate Me Because I m Beautiful: Anti-attractiveness Bias in Organizational Evaluation and Decision Making, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46 (6), (2011), Does Being Attractive Always Help? Positive and Negative Effects of Attractiveness on Social Decision Making, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37 (8), Ahearne, Michael, Thomas W. Gruen, and Cheryl Burke Jarvis (1999), If Looks Could Sell: Moderation and Mediation of the Attractiveness Effect on Salesperson Performance, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 16 (4), Apsler, Robert (1975), Effects of Embarrassment on Behavior Toward Others, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (1), Argo, Jennifer J., Darren W. Dahl, and Andrea C. Morales (2008), Positive Consumer Contagion: Responses to Attractive Others in a Retail Context, Journal of Marketing Research, 45 (6), Arkin, Robert M., Alan J. Appelman, and Jerry M. Burger (1980), Social Anxiety, Self-Presentation, and the Self-Serving Bias in Causal Attribution, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38 (1), Bernstein, William M., Blair O. Stephenson, Melvin L. Snyder, and Robert A. Wicklund (1983), Causal Ambiguity and Heterosexual Affiliation, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19 (1), Berscheid, Ellen and Elaine Walster (1974), Physical Attractiveness, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 7, ed. Leonard Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press, Brown, Bert R. (1970), Face-saving Following Experimentally Induced Embarrassment, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6 (3), Brumbaugh, Anne M. and José Antonio Rosa (2009), Perceived Discrimination, Cashier Metaperceptions, Embarrassment, and Confidence as Influencers of Coupon Use: An Ethnoracial-Socioeconomic Analysis, Journal of Retailing, 85 (3), Buunk, Bram P. and Vera Hoorens (1992), Social Support and Stress: The Role of Social Comparison and Social Exchange Processes, British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31 (4), Consedine, Nathan S., Yulia S. Krivoshekova, and Christine R. Harris (2007), Bodily Embarrassment and Judgment Concern as Separable Factors in the Measurement of Medical Embarrassment: Psychometric Development and Links to Treatment-Seeking Outcomes, British Journal of Health Psychology, 12 (3), Consedine, Nathan S., Inga Ladwig, Maike K. Reddig, and Elizabeth A. Broadbent (2011), The Many Faeces of Colorectal Cancer Screening Embarrassment: Preliminary Psychometric Development and Links to Screening Outcome, British Journal of Health Psychology, 16 (3), Dabbs, James M. Jr. and Neil A. Stokes III (1975), Beauty Is Power: The Use of Space on the Sidewalk, Sociometry, 38 (4), Dahl, Darren W., Rajesh V. Manchanda, and Jennifer J. Argo (2001), Embarrassment in Consumer Purchase: The Role of Social Presence and Purchase Familiarity, Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (3), DeShields, Oscar W. Jr., Ali Kara, and Erdener Kaynak (1996), Source Effects in Purchase Decisions: The Impact of Physical Attractiveness and Accent of Salesperson, International JournalofResearchinMarketing,13(1), Dion, Karen, Ellen Berscheid, and Elaine Walster (1972), What Is Beautiful Is Good, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24 (3), Dittmar, Helga and Sarah Howard (2004), Thin-Ideal Internalization and Social Comparison Tendency as Moderators of Media Model s Impact on Women s Body- Focused Anxiety, Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23 (6), Dong, Ping, Xun (Irene) Huang, and Robert S. Wyer Jr. (2013), The Illusion of Saving Face: How People Symbolically Cope with Embarrassment, Psychological Science, 24 (10), Dong, Ping and Robert S. Wyer Jr. (2014), How Time Flies: The Effects of Conversation Characteristics and Partner Attractiveness on Duration Judgments in a Social Interaction, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50 (January), Eagly, Alice H., Richard D. Ashmore, Mona G. Makhijani, and Laura C. Longo (1991), What Is Beautiful Is Good, But...: A Meta- Analytic Review of Research on the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype, Psychological Bulletin, 110 (1), Goffman, E. (1959), The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F. Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), Hayes, Andrew F. (2013), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, New York: Guilford. Hudders, Liselot, Charlotte De Backer, Maryanne Fisher, and Patrick Vyncke (2014), The Rival Wears Prada: Luxury Consumption as a Female Competition Strategy, Evolutionary Psychology, 12 (3), Jones, April M. and Justin T. Buckingham (2005), Self-Esteem as a Moderator of the Effect of Social Comparison on Women s Body Image, Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24 (8), Kenrick, Douglas T., Daniel R. Montello, Sara E. Gutierres, and Melanie R. Trost (1993), Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Affect and Perceptual Judgments: When Social Comparison Overrides Social Reinforcement, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19 (2), Koernig, Stephen K. and Albert L. Page (2002), What If Your Dentist Looked Like Tom Cruise? Applying the Match-up Hypothesis to a Service Encounter, Psychology & Marketing, 19 (1), Lalwani, Ashok K., Sharon Shavitt, and Timothy Johnson (2006), What Is the Relation Between Cultural Orientation and Socially Desirable Responding, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (1), Leary, Mark R. (1983), Social Anxiousness: The Construct and Its Measurement, Journal of Personality Assessment, 47 (1), (1986), The Impact of Interactional Impediments on Social Anxiety and Self-Presentation, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22 (2),

18 WAN AND WYER 595. (1996), Self-Presentation: Impression Management and Interpersonal Behavior, Boulder, CO: Westview. Leary, Mark R. and Robin M. Kowalski (1990), Impression Management: A Literature Review and Two-Component Model, Psychological Bulletin, 107 (1), Leary, Mark R., John B. Nezlek, Deborah Downs, Julie Radford- Davenport, Jeffrey Martin, and Anne McMullen (1994), Self-Presentation in Everyday Interactions: Effects of Target Familiarity and Gender Composition, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (4), Lockwood, Penelope and Ziva Kunda (1997), Superstars and Me: Predicting the Impact of Role Models on the Self, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (1), Maner, Jon K., Saul L. Miller, D. Aaron Rouby, and Matthew T. Gailliot (2009), Intrasexual Vigilance: The Implicit Cognition of Romantic Rivalry, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (1), Miller, Rowland S. (1987), Empathic Embarrassment: Situational and Personal Determinants of Reactions to the Embarrassment of Another, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 (6), Moore, Sarah G., Darren W. Dahl, Gerald J. Gorn, and Charles B. Weinberg (2006), Coping with Condom Embarrassment, Psychology, Health & Medicine, 11 (1), Mori, DeAnna, Shelly Chaiken, and Patricia Pliner (1987), Eating Lightly and the Self-Presentation of Femininity, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 (4), Niu, Han-Jen, Yung-Sung Chiang, and Hsien-Tang Tsai (2012), An Exploratory Study of the Otaku Adolescent Consumer, Psychology & Marketing, 29 (10), Reingen, Peter H. and Jerome B. Kernan (1993), Social Perception and Interpersonal Influence: Some Consequences of the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype in a Personal Selling Setting, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2 (1), Richins, Marsha L. (1991), Social Comparison and the Idealized Images of Advertising, Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (1), Sarnoff, Irving and Philip G. Zimbardo (1961), Anxiety, Fear, and Social Isolation, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62 (2), Schlenker, Barry R. (1980), Impression Management: The Self- Concept, Social Identity, and International Relations, Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Schlenker, Barry R. and Mark R. Leary (1982), Social Anxiety and Self-Presentation: A Conceptualization Model, Psychological Bulletin, 92 (3), Sigall, Harold and Elliot Aronson (1969), Liking for an Evaluator as a Function of Her Physical Attractiveness and Nature of the Evaluations, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5 (1), Sigall, Harold and David Landy (1973), Radiating Beauty: Effects of Having a Physically Attractive Partner on Person Perception, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 (2), Snyder, Mark, Elizabeth D. Tanke, and Ellen Berscheid (1977), Social Perception and Interpersonal Behavior: On the Self-Fulfilling Nature of Social Stereotypes, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (9), Tesser, Abraham and James E. Collins (1988), Emotion in Social Reflection and Comparison Situations: Intuitive, Systematic and Exploratory Approaches, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55 (5), Tyler, James M. and Robert S. Feldman (2004), Truth, Lies, and Self-Presentation: How Gender and Anticipated Future Interaction Relate to Deceptive Behavior, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34 (12), Walster, Elaine, Vera Aronson, and Darcy Abrahams (1966), Importance of Physical Attractiveness in Dating Behavior, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 (5), Wan, Lisa C. (2013), Culture s Impact on Consumer Complaining Responses to Embarrassing Service Failure, Journal of Business Research, 66 (3), Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), Conspicuous Consumption, Relationships, and Rivals: Women s Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women, Journal of Consumer Research, 40 (5), Wenzel, Amy and Tracy Emerson (2009), Mate Selection in Socially Anxious and Nonanxious Individuals, Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28 (3), Wood, Joanne V. (1989), Theory and Research Concerning Social Comparisons of Personal Attributes, Psychological Bulletin, 106 (2),

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing Birgit Quante and Lothar Mühlbach Heinrich-Hertz-Institut für Nachrichtentechnik Berlin GmbH (HHI) Einsteinufer 37, D-15087 Berlin, Germany, http://www.hhi.de/

More information

CATHERINE W. M. YEUNG AND ROBERT S. WYER, JR.*

CATHERINE W. M. YEUNG AND ROBERT S. WYER, JR.* 1 Affect, Pictures and Consumer Judgment: The Impact of Initial Appraisals of a Product on Evaluations and Expectancy-Confirmatory Information Processing CATHERINE W. M. YEUNG AND ROBERT S. WYER, JR.*

More information

SOCIETY OF ACTUARIES THE AMERICAN ACADEMY OF ACTUARIES RETIREMENT PLAN PREFERENCES SURVEY REPORT OF FINDINGS. January 2004

SOCIETY OF ACTUARIES THE AMERICAN ACADEMY OF ACTUARIES RETIREMENT PLAN PREFERENCES SURVEY REPORT OF FINDINGS. January 2004 SOCIETY OF ACTUARIES THE AMERICAN ACADEMY OF ACTUARIES RETIREMENT PLAN PREFERENCES SURVEY REPORT OF FINDINGS January 2004 Mathew Greenwald & Associates, Inc. TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION... 1 SETTING

More information

Brand Loyalty in Insurance Companies

Brand Loyalty in Insurance Companies Journal of Economic Development, Management, IT, Finance and Marketing, 4(1), 12-26, March 2012 12 Brand Loyalty in Insurance Companies Sancharan Roy, (B.E., MBA) Assistant Professor, St. Joseph's College

More information

COI Research Management Summary on behalf of the Department of Health

COI Research Management Summary on behalf of the Department of Health COI Research Management Summary on behalf of the Department of Health Title: Worth Talking About Campaign Evaluation 2010 / 2011 Quantitative research conducted by TNS-BMRB COI Reference number: 114770

More information

A survey of public attitudes towards conveyancing services, conducted on behalf of:

A survey of public attitudes towards conveyancing services, conducted on behalf of: A survey of public attitudes towards conveyancing services, conducted on behalf of: February 2009 CONTENTS Methodology 4 Executive summary 6 Part 1: your experience 8 Q1 Have you used a solicitor for conveyancing

More information

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL 1. The Effects of Alcohol on the Acceptability of Casual Sex. College of Western Idaho

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL 1. The Effects of Alcohol on the Acceptability of Casual Sex. College of Western Idaho Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL 1 The Effects of Alcohol on the Acceptability of Casual Sex College of Western Idaho EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL 2 Abstract The use of alcohol to excess and the engagement of binge

More information

Tracking Survey of Graduates from Self-financed Associate Degree & Higher Diploma Programmes of 2005 and 2006 Cohorts SURVEY REPORT

Tracking Survey of Graduates from Self-financed Associate Degree & Higher Diploma Programmes of 2005 and 2006 Cohorts SURVEY REPORT JOINT QUALITY REVIEW COMMITTEE Tracking Survey of Graduates from Self-financed Associate Degree & Higher Diploma Programmes of 2005 and 2006 Cohorts SURVEY REPORT June 2009 Content 1. EXECUTIVE SUMMARY....

More information

Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships

Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships Breeana Skinner Faculty Advisor: Carmen Wilson-VanVoorhis, Department of Psychology ABSTRACT This study examined women s perceptions of a

More information

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior Icek Ajzen Brief Description of the Theory of Planned Behavior According to the theory, human behavior is guided by three kinds of considerations:

More information

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION CHAPTER OVERVIEW Chapter 4 introduces you to the related concepts of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination. The chapter begins with definitions of these three

More information

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE Reference: Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.

More information

The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys

The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys Philip Gendall It has been suggested that the response rate for a self administered questionnaire will be enhanced if the cover of the questionnaire

More information

Altarum Comparison of Physician and Consumer Health Care Opinions. September 20, 2012

Altarum Comparison of Physician and Consumer Health Care Opinions. September 20, 2012 Altarum Comparison of Physician and Consumer Health Care Opinions September 20, 2012 W. Lynch C. Ciucci Altarum Comparison of Physician and Consumer Health Care Opinions September 20, 2012 Table of Contents

More information

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 15

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 15 Mind on Statistics Chapter 15 Section 15.1 1. A student survey was done to study the relationship between class standing (freshman, sophomore, junior, or senior) and major subject (English, Biology, French,

More information

PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME

PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME CHAPTER- V PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME 5.1 Introduction The present study intended to investigate the senior citizen s retirement planning and their perception

More information

HOW DO EMOTIONS INFLUENCE SAVING BEHAVIOR?

HOW DO EMOTIONS INFLUENCE SAVING BEHAVIOR? April 2009, Number 9-8 HOW DO EMOTIONS INFLUENCE SAVING BEHAVIOR? By Gergana Y. Nenkov, Deborah J. MacInnis, and Maureen Morrin* Introduction Employers have moved away from traditional defined benefit

More information

lean in DISCUSSION GUIDE FOR ALL AUDIENCES

lean in DISCUSSION GUIDE FOR ALL AUDIENCES lean in DISCUSSION GUIDE FOR ALL AUDIENCES Introduction We are grateful for what we have. What did past generations have to deal with that we don t have to deal with? What are you most grateful for? Men

More information

Better Than Better-Than-Average (or Not): Elevated and Depressed Self-evaluations Following Unfavorable Social Comparisons

Better Than Better-Than-Average (or Not): Elevated and Depressed Self-evaluations Following Unfavorable Social Comparisons Seta, J. J., Seta, C. E. & McElroy, T. (2006). Better Than Better-Than-Average (or Not): Elevated and Depressed Self-evaluations Following Unfavorable Social Comparisons. Self and Identity, 5: 51-72. Published

More information

Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination

Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 4, No. 3, April 2009, pp. 235 247 Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination Todd J. Thorsteinson University of

More information

Appreciating the Complex World of Bachelors

Appreciating the Complex World of Bachelors Appreciating the Complex World of Bachelors "Lifelong bachelors" (heterosexual men who are at least 40 years old and have never married) have long been the subject of public scrutiny. Usually this attention

More information

SCHOOL-BASED ENTERPRISE INSTRUCTIONAL UNITS

SCHOOL-BASED ENTERPRISE INSTRUCTIONAL UNITS SCHOOL-BASED ENTERPRISE SCHOOL-BASED ENTERPRISE INSTRUCTIONAL UNITS SELLING Generous support for DECA s School-based Enterprise Program is provided by SCHOOL-BASED ENTERPRISE INSTRUCTIONAL UNITS TOPIC:

More information

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life Final report December 2012 Context Sport England s 2012-17 strategy aims to help people and communities across the country transform our sporting culture, so that

More information

Beautiful but Dangerous: Effects of Offender Attractiveness and Nature of the Crime on Juridic Judgment

Beautiful but Dangerous: Effects of Offender Attractiveness and Nature of the Crime on Juridic Judgment Journal ol Personality and Social Psychology 1975, Vol. 31, No. 3, 410-414 Beautiful but Dangerous: Effects of Offender Attractiveness and Nature of the Crime on Juridic Judgment Harold Sigall and Nancy

More information

China s Middle Market for Life Insurance

China s Middle Market for Life Insurance China s Middle Market for Life Insurance May 2014 Sponsored by: SOA International Section SOA Marketing & Distribution Section SOA Research Expanding Boundaries Pool The opinions expressed and conclusions

More information

Despite retailers best intentions, consumers often shop

Despite retailers best intentions, consumers often shop Iana A. Castro, Andrea C. Morales, & Stephen M. Nowlis The Influence of Disorganized Shelf Displays and Limited Product Quantity on Consumer Purchase The current research explores how shelf display organization

More information

Teaching Phonetics with Online Resources

Teaching Phonetics with Online Resources Teaching Phonetics with Online Resources Tien-Hsin Hsing National Pingtung University of Science of Technology, Taiwan thhsin@mail.npust.eud.tw Received October 2014; Revised December 2014 ABSTRACT. This

More information

Paediatric Outpatient Survey 2011

Paediatric Outpatient Survey 2011 Paediatric Outpatient Survey 2011 States of Jersey Health & Social Services August 2011 Final Report www.pickereurope.org https://www.picker-results.org Copyright 2011 Picker Institute Europe. All rights

More information

Administrator Suggestions Regarding the Recruitment of Male Elementary Teachers. Julia Wilkins, Ph.D. Robert J. Gamble, Ph.D.

Administrator Suggestions Regarding the Recruitment of Male Elementary Teachers. Julia Wilkins, Ph.D. Robert J. Gamble, Ph.D. Recruitment of Male Elementary Teachers, 1 Administrator Suggestions Regarding the Recruitment of Male Elementary Teachers Julia Wilkins, Ph.D. Robert J. Gamble, Ph.D. D Youville College, Buffalo, NY Recruitment

More information

CHAPTER EIGHT. BEYOND THE GRADUATE PROGRAM: APPLYING FOR POSTDOCTORAL FELLOWSHIPS AND OTHER RESEARCH POSITIONS IN THE FIELD OF THE PhD

CHAPTER EIGHT. BEYOND THE GRADUATE PROGRAM: APPLYING FOR POSTDOCTORAL FELLOWSHIPS AND OTHER RESEARCH POSITIONS IN THE FIELD OF THE PhD CHAPTER EIGHT BEYOND THE GRADUATE PROGRAM: APPLYING FOR POSTDOCTORAL FELLOWSHIPS AND OTHER RESEARCH POSITIONS IN THE FIELD OF THE PhD APPLYING FOR A POSTDOCTORAL POSITION IN THE SCIENCES The postdoctoral

More information

Review for Grade 9 June Exam - Unit 9 - Probability and Statistics

Review for Grade 9 June Exam - Unit 9 - Probability and Statistics Name: Date: Review for Grade 9 June Exam - Unit 9 - Probability and Statistics Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. The last three days Alexa

More information

Drivers and Outcomes of Perceived Fairness in Consumer Financial Decisions. Suzanne B. Shu, UCLA Sylvia Morelli, UCLA. Working draft: March 2012

Drivers and Outcomes of Perceived Fairness in Consumer Financial Decisions. Suzanne B. Shu, UCLA Sylvia Morelli, UCLA. Working draft: March 2012 Drivers and Outcomes of Perceived Fairness in Consumer Financial Decisions Suzanne B. Shu, UCLA Sylvia Morelli, UCLA Working draft: March 2012 Abstract In financial decision making, perceived fairness

More information

Understanding the Accounting Faculty Shortage:

Understanding the Accounting Faculty Shortage: Understanding the Accounting Faculty Shortage: THE FUTURE ROLE OF THE MANAGEMENT ACCOUNTANT About IMA IMA, the association of accountants and fi nancial professionals in business, is one of the largest

More information

THE DESIGN AND EVALUATION OF ROAD SAFETY PUBLICITY CAMPAIGNS

THE DESIGN AND EVALUATION OF ROAD SAFETY PUBLICITY CAMPAIGNS THE DESIGN AND EVALUATION OF ROAD SAFETY PUBLICITY CAMPAIGNS INTRODUCTION This note discusses some basic principals of the data-led design of publicity campaigns, the main issues that need to be considered

More information

Test Anxiety, Student Preferences and Performance on Different Exam Types in Introductory Psychology

Test Anxiety, Student Preferences and Performance on Different Exam Types in Introductory Psychology Test Anxiety, Student Preferences and Performance on Different Exam Types in Introductory Psychology Afshin Gharib and William Phillips Abstract The differences between cheat sheet and open book exams

More information

The. LTCI Claims. Experience

The. LTCI Claims. Experience The LTCI Claims Experience Understanding the Differences What is the difference between a long term care insurance claim and other types of insurance claims? The answer lies in the purpose of the insurance.

More information

Interview Skills Guide

Interview Skills Guide Interview Skills Guide The main purpose of an interview is to sell yourself to a company/organization and convince them that they should hire you. As a candidate you are a salesperson, selling the most

More information

WHEN BRANDS GET PERSONAL IN ONLINE CHATTER: THE EFFECTS OF SELF-DISCLOSURE AND ANTHROPOMORPHISM ON CONSUMER BRAND RELATIONSHIPS

WHEN BRANDS GET PERSONAL IN ONLINE CHATTER: THE EFFECTS OF SELF-DISCLOSURE AND ANTHROPOMORPHISM ON CONSUMER BRAND RELATIONSHIPS WHEN BRANDS GET PERSONAL IN ONLINE CHATTER: THE EFFECTS OF SELF-DISCLOSURE AND ANTHROPOMORPHISM ON CONSUMER BRAND RELATIONSHIPS HUANG LI DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY CITY UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG SEPTEMBER 2013

More information

Preventing Unplanned Pregnancy and Completing College AN EVALUATION OF ONLINE LESSONS

Preventing Unplanned Pregnancy and Completing College AN EVALUATION OF ONLINE LESSONS Preventing Unplanned Pregnancy and Completing College AN EVALUATION OF ONLINE LESSONS About Us The National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy is a non-profit, nonpartisan organization that

More information

4 Possessive/Jealous. Men in Relationships

4 Possessive/Jealous. Men in Relationships 4 Possessive/Jealous Men in Relationships A possessive/jealous man will: Tell you how to dress; Be overly concerned about where you are going when socializing; Insist on escorting you to mundane places;

More information

ESOL Customer Service Training: Unit 1 1: 1 Student Book. Unit 1: Talking With Your Customer

ESOL Customer Service Training: Unit 1 1: 1 Student Book. Unit 1: Talking With Your Customer ESOL Customer Service Training: Unit 1 1: 1 Unit 1: Talking With Your Customer ESOL Customer Service Training: Unit 1 1: 2 What are your goals? Note to Instructor: If you have permission, use Stand Out

More information

Mid-Cap Business Strategies:

Mid-Cap Business Strategies: Mid-Cap Business Strategies: How Mid-Caps Think about Growth and Financing, How Motivated They Are for Growth, What Drives Their Ambition, What Are Their Major Concerns or Worries A COMPAS Report for Roynat/Financial

More information

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY M S I - S T A T E W I D E S U R V E Y O N I N S U R A N C E F R A U D

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY M S I - S T A T E W I D E S U R V E Y O N I N S U R A N C E F R A U D EXECUTIVE SUMMARY The majority of Virginians believe you should obey the law without question but also suggest that right and wrong are ultimately determined by the individual. Nearly three out of five

More information

Executive Summary and Recommendations

Executive Summary and Recommendations Executive Summary and Recommendations To download a free copy of the complete report, go to www.aauw.org/learn/research/whysofew.cfm. Executive Summary Women have made tremendous progress in education

More information

Emotionally Intelligent Leadership

Emotionally Intelligent Leadership Emotionally Intelligent Leadership Leadership and Emotions Copyright 2005 Claus Møller Consulting. All rights reserved. This work may not be altered in any way, but may be distributed freely in its current

More information

COUNSELING AFFILIATES SEXUAL ADDICTION TREATMENT PROGRAM GUIDELINES FOR WRITING A DISCLOSURE TO RELATIONSHIP PARTNERS

COUNSELING AFFILIATES SEXUAL ADDICTION TREATMENT PROGRAM GUIDELINES FOR WRITING A DISCLOSURE TO RELATIONSHIP PARTNERS COUNSELING AFFILIATES SEXUAL ADDICTION TREATMENT PROGRAM GUIDELINES FOR WRITING A DISCLOSURE TO RELATIONSHIP PARTNERS 1. Start with the text of the First Step if available. Only do disclosure after completing

More information

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan ABSTRACT The focus of this research is to determine the impact of

More information

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts Gender Stereotypes Associated 1 Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts Lacey D. Seefeldt Undergraduate Student, Psychology Keywords: Altruism, Gender Stereotypes, Vignette Abstract Possible

More information

Influence of Gender and Age in Aggressive Dream Content of Spanish Children and Adolescents

Influence of Gender and Age in Aggressive Dream Content of Spanish Children and Adolescents Influence of Gender and Age in Aggressive Dream Content of Spanish Children and Adolescents Ursula Oberst, Cristian Charles, and Ander Chamarro Ramon Llull University This study investigated the aggressive

More information

Social Security Disability Resources For Self Advocacy

Social Security Disability Resources For Self Advocacy Social Security Disability Resources For Self Advocacy Introduction This guide is intended to help people with multiple sclerosis (MS) advocate effectively to obtain the Social Security Disability Insurance

More information

CASE STUDY. The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE ENGAGEMENT and VIDEO CONTENT POPULARITY DECEMBER 2011 INTRODUCTION KEY FINDINGS

CASE STUDY. The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE ENGAGEMENT and VIDEO CONTENT POPULARITY DECEMBER 2011 INTRODUCTION KEY FINDINGS CASE STUDY DECEMBER 2011 TABLE OF CONTENTS Situation Analysis... 2 Research Hypothesis... 2 Case Study Context... 2 Methodology...3-8 Business Implications...7-8 The POWER of VISUAL EFFECTS to DRIVE AUDIENCE

More information

Chapter 1: Data and Decisions Quiz A

Chapter 1: Data and Decisions Quiz A Chapter 1: Data and Decisions Quiz A Name 1.1.1 Identify various aspects of studies and experiments and/or classify variables. 1. Viamedia, the cable industry's leading independent cable rep firm and provider

More information

Usability features of mobile apps and healthcare mobile apps: similarities and differences. Teuta Cata, Northern Kentucky University

Usability features of mobile apps and healthcare mobile apps: similarities and differences. Teuta Cata, Northern Kentucky University Abstract Usability features of mobile apps and healthcare mobile apps: similarities and differences. Teuta Cata, Northern Kentucky University Ben Martz, Northern Kentucky University The purpose of this

More information

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES.

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. 277 CHAPTER VI COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. This chapter contains a full discussion of customer loyalty comparisons between private and public insurance companies

More information

PAYMENT PROTECTION INSURANCE RESEARCH

PAYMENT PROTECTION INSURANCE RESEARCH PAYMENT PROTECTION INSURANCE RESEARCH ANALYTICAL REPORT NOVEMBER 2015 ABOUT COMRES ComRes provides specialist research and insight into reputation, public policy and communications. It is a founding member

More information

Dissertation Title: How is rude service interpreted? Cultural orientation affects perceptions of power threat

Dissertation Title: How is rude service interpreted? Cultural orientation affects perceptions of power threat Jimmy Wong 350 Wohlers Hall, MC-706 Champaign, Illinois 61820 U.S.A. Phone: (217) 819-9272; Email: jswong2@illinois.edu Education Background (UIUC) Doctoral Candidate (ABD) in Business Administration,

More information

Enhancing Critical Thinking and Daily-Life Application in a Psychology Course: A Case Album Approach

Enhancing Critical Thinking and Daily-Life Application in a Psychology Course: A Case Album Approach Enhancing Critical Thinking and Daily-Life Application in a Psychology Course: A Case Album Approach J.P. Leung, Darius K.S. Chan and Catherine S.K. Tang The Chinese University of Hong Kong A b s t r a

More information

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 13

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 13 Mind on Statistics Chapter 13 Sections 13.1-13.2 1. Which statement is not true about hypothesis tests? A. Hypothesis tests are only valid when the sample is representative of the population for the question

More information

Hospital Authority. 2013 Hospital-based Patient Experience and Satisfaction Survey

Hospital Authority. 2013 Hospital-based Patient Experience and Satisfaction Survey For discussion on 25.9.2014 HAB-P211 Hospital Authority 2013 Hospital-based Patient Experience and Satisfaction Survey Purpose This paper reports to Members the findings of the 2013 Hospital-based Patient

More information

PREPARING FOR THE INTERVIEW

PREPARING FOR THE INTERVIEW U N I V E R S I T Y C A R E E R S E R V I C E S PREPARING FOR THE INTERVIEW THE INTERVIEW The interview is an opportunity to demonstrate to an employer why you are the best fit for the position. Essentially,

More information

April 7, 2015 The 2015 Tax Refund Consumer Spending Report. Anne Wilson Iris Wang Ishani Banerji, Ph.D. Kurt Carlson, Ph.D.

April 7, 2015 The 2015 Tax Refund Consumer Spending Report. Anne Wilson Iris Wang Ishani Banerji, Ph.D. Kurt Carlson, Ph.D. April 7, 2015 The 2015 Tax Refund Consumer Spending Report Anne Wilson Iris Wang Ishani Banerji, Ph.D. Kurt Carlson, Ph.D. The 2015 Tax Refund Consumer Spending Report 1 EXECUTIVE SUMMARY The 2015 Tax

More information

Story about going to Palestine. How to get through security without lying?

Story about going to Palestine. How to get through security without lying? LYING THE SIN WE ALL COMMIT Alfred Snider University of Vermont 10 December 2014 Faculty of Arts, University of Ljubljana These are the notes used by the lecturer. Story about going to Palestine. How to

More information

Problems for Chapter 9: Producing data: Experiments. STAT 145-023. Fall 2015.

Problems for Chapter 9: Producing data: Experiments. STAT 145-023. Fall 2015. How Data are Obtained The distinction between observational study and experiment is important in statistics. Observational Study Experiment Observes individuals and measures variables of interest but does

More information

THE EFFECT OF STATING EXPECTATIONS ON CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND SHOPPING EXPERIENCE. Chezy Ofir School of Business Administration, Hebrew University

THE EFFECT OF STATING EXPECTATIONS ON CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND SHOPPING EXPERIENCE. Chezy Ofir School of Business Administration, Hebrew University 1 THE EFFECT OF STATING EXPECTATIONS ON CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND SHOPPING EXPERIENCE Chezy Ofir School of Business Administration, Hebrew University Itamar Simonson Graduate School of Business, Stanford

More information

First Survey on Junior High School Students Values and Financial Behaviors and Teens Financial Summer Camp Announced

First Survey on Junior High School Students Values and Financial Behaviors and Teens Financial Summer Camp Announced [Citi Foundation - Women s Rescue Foundation United News Release] June 19, 2008 First Survey on Junior High School Students Values and Financial Behaviors and Teens Financial Summer Camp Announced The

More information

Code of Professional Conduct & Ethics Association of Accounting Technicians of Sri Lanka

Code of Professional Conduct & Ethics Association of Accounting Technicians of Sri Lanka Code of Professional Conduct & Ethics Association of Accounting Technicians of Sri Lanka (Incorporated under the Companies Act No: 17 of 1982) Contents 1. Purpose of the Code of Ethics 2. Fundamental Principles

More information

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness Stigmatisation of people with mental illness Report of the research carried out in July 1998 and July 2003 by the Office for National Statistics (ONS) on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists Changing

More information

Benefits-Based Performance Management. White Paper

Benefits-Based Performance Management. White Paper Benefits-Based Performance Management White Paper 1 Overview Employee engagement is a somewhat overused term. But it is obviously very important for employees to be engaged and to care. A real challenge

More information

101 Characteristics of Americans/American Culture

101 Characteristics of Americans/American Culture 101 Characteristics of Americans/American Culture To help you compare and contrast what you observe of American culture and your own, mark the similarities and differences between your culture and what

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 32 (2009) 286 290 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development A method for eliciting scale errors in preschool classrooms Karl S. Rosengren a,,

More information

Guide to Using Results

Guide to Using Results Guide to Using Results Contact Information For assistance, call ACT Engage Customer Service at 319.337.1893, 8:30 a.m. 5:00 p.m., central time, Monday through Friday, or email engage@act.org. Resources

More information

Objectives of Occupational Therapy in Mental Health

Objectives of Occupational Therapy in Mental Health Objectives of Occupational Therapy in Mental Health I) To help establish an atmosphere conductive to recovery (containing minimal anxiety & maximum support) by utilizing individual & group activity program.

More information

Vol. 1-6 (09/03/2003) by Nicholas Yee. (http://www.nickyee.com/daedalus)

Vol. 1-6 (09/03/2003) by Nicholas Yee. (http://www.nickyee.com/daedalus) The Daedalus Project Vol. 1-6 (09/03/2003) by Nicholas Yee (http://www.nickyee.com/daedalus) FAQ on the Good and Bad (transcript of interview) I was recently interviewed over email over some issues centered

More information

The Leader s Edge. How Best Practices Programs Can Be Used Most Effectively to Support the Growth of Women Leaders

The Leader s Edge. How Best Practices Programs Can Be Used Most Effectively to Support the Growth of Women Leaders The Leader s Edge How Best Practices Programs Can Be Used Most Effectively to Support the Growth of Women Leaders The Leaders Edge research study completed in February, 2004 demonstrates the need for companies

More information

Ipsos / Europ Assistance barometer Europeans Plans and Concerns for the Holidays Summary Ipsos for the Europ Assistance Group May 24th 2012

Ipsos / Europ Assistance barometer Europeans Plans and Concerns for the Holidays Summary Ipsos for the Europ Assistance Group May 24th 2012 Ipsos / Europ Assistance barometer Europeans Plans and Concerns for the Holidays Summary Ipsos for the Europ Assistance Group May 24th 2012 Contacts Ipsos Public Affairs: Fabienne SIMON 01 41 98 92 15

More information

Winning The Settlement Keys to Negotiation Strategy

Winning The Settlement Keys to Negotiation Strategy ABA Section of Litigation Corporate Counsel CLE Seminar, February 11-14, 2010 Winning The Settlement Keys to Negotiation Strategy Winning The Settlement Keys to Negotiation Strategy Thomas A. Dye Carlton

More information

Public Assessments and Public Attitudes toward Israel Prison Service 2013

Public Assessments and Public Attitudes toward Israel Prison Service 2013 STATE OF ISRAEL Ministry of Public Security Bureau of the Chief Scientist Public Assessments and Public Attitudes toward Israel Prison Service 2013 DR. Shuki Hendeles Taldor This study reported herewith

More information

The goal you want to accomplish during a telephone interview is to receive an invitation for an on-site interview.

The goal you want to accomplish during a telephone interview is to receive an invitation for an on-site interview. Telephone Interviewing Tips The goal you want to accomplish during a telephone interview is to receive an invitation for an on-site interview. Conduct your telephone interview in an area where you can

More information

The Presentation of Self (Goffman's Dramaturgical model)

The Presentation of Self (Goffman's Dramaturgical model) The Presentation of Self (Goffman's Dramaturgical model) A. The Dramaturgical Model 1. All the World s a Stage 2. The Performance B. Impression Management 1. The Definition of the Situation 2. Expressions

More information

Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level

Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level psyct.psychopen.eu 2193-7281 Research Articles Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level Shalini Sisodia* a, Ira Das a [a] Department of Psychology,

More information

The usability of videophones for seniors and hospice providers: a brief report of two studies

The usability of videophones for seniors and hospice providers: a brief report of two studies Computers in Biology and Medicine 35 (2005) 782 790 www.intl.elsevierhealth.com/journals/cobm The usability of videophones for seniors and hospice providers: a brief report of two studies Debra R. Parker

More information

Word count: 2,567 words (including front sheet, abstract, main text, references

Word count: 2,567 words (including front sheet, abstract, main text, references Integrating gaze direction and expression in preferences for attractive faces Benedict C. Jones 1, Lisa M. DeBruine 2, Anthony C. Little 3, Claire A. Conway 1 & David R. Feinberg 2 1. School of Psychology,

More information

Staff Perceptions of the Professional Practice Environment 2006

Staff Perceptions of the Professional Practice Environment 2006 Staff Perceptions of the Professional Practice Environment 2006 Values Philosophy Standards of Practice Collaborative Decisionmaking Professional Development Patient Care Delivery Models Privileges, Credentialing,

More information

Results Report: HEALTHY OPPORTUNITIES FOR MEANINGFUL EXPERIENCE SOCIETY

Results Report: HEALTHY OPPORTUNITIES FOR MEANINGFUL EXPERIENCE SOCIETY Results Report: HEALTHY OPPORTUNITIES FOR MEANINGFUL EXPERIENCE SOCIETY May 2011 Community Living British Columbia 7th Floor, Airport Square 1200 West 73rd Avenue Vancouver, BC, V6P 6G5 Ph: 1.877.660.2522

More information

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal Executive Summary Abstract The ability to make quality decisions that influence people to exemplary

More information

Outpatient Services. Partial Hospitalization Program (PHP) & Intensive Outpatient Program (IOP) Patient Handbook. Bringing Life Into Balance

Outpatient Services. Partial Hospitalization Program (PHP) & Intensive Outpatient Program (IOP) Patient Handbook. Bringing Life Into Balance Bringing Life Into Balance Outpatient Services Partial Hospitalization Program (PHP) & Intensive Outpatient Program (IOP) Patient Handbook PHP/IOP Patient Handbook Page 1 of 7 Confidentiality The confidentiality

More information

Peer Educators Take Family Planning Messages to HIV-Positive Support Groups

Peer Educators Take Family Planning Messages to HIV-Positive Support Groups Family Planning for Healthy Living Project in Ghana : Stories of Peer Educators and Community Champions July 2008 Peer Educators Take Family Planning Messages to HIV-Positive Support Groups In Sub-Saharan

More information

Recovery Strategies for Service Failures: The Case of Restaurants

Recovery Strategies for Service Failures: The Case of Restaurants Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management ISSN: 1936-8623 (Print) 1936-8631 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/whmm20 Recovery Strategies for Service Failures: The Case of Restaurants

More information

Branding and Search Engine Marketing

Branding and Search Engine Marketing Branding and Search Engine Marketing Abstract The paper investigates the role of paid search advertising in delivering optimal conversion rates in brand-related search engine marketing (SEM) strategies.

More information

Teens and Cyberbullying

Teens and Cyberbullying Teens and Cyberbullying EXECUTIVE SUMMARY OF A REPORT ON RESEARCH Conducted for NATIONAL CRIME PREVENTION COUNCIL (NCPC) Released February 28, 2007 By the National Crime Prevention Council Survey conducted

More information

QUALITY OF LIFE REPORT

QUALITY OF LIFE REPORT / 01 SURVEY QUALITY OF LIFE REPORT Private Assets in 2015 Amount to 110 Trillion CNY respondents will increase luxury consumption next year 2.01 Million High Net Worth Families China's Growing Wealth respondents

More information

Retail Sales Potential Identification (RSPI) Report

Retail Sales Potential Identification (RSPI) Report CONTENTS Page Recommendations 2 Summary of Scores & Cautions 3 Selection Considerations 4 Responses from Opinions Section 5 #NOQ1E6OTO7KI 2015/04/01 Name: John Smith Telephone: 12345678 Email: admin@optimalconsulting.com.sg

More information

IS THIS STALKING? A COMPARISON BETWEEN LEGAL AND COMMUNITY DEFINITIONS OF STALKING

IS THIS STALKING? A COMPARISON BETWEEN LEGAL AND COMMUNITY DEFINITIONS OF STALKING IS THIS STALKING? A COMPARISON BETWEEN LEGAL AND COMMUNITY DEFINITIONS OF STALKING Susan Dennison and Don Thomson Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences and Liberal Studies, Charles Sturt

More information

WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD

WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD WHAT IS PTSD? A HANDOUT FROM THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR PTSD BY JESSICA HAMBLEN, PHD Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is an anxiety disorder that can occur following the experience or witnessing of a

More information

Restaurant Revenue Management: Do perceived scarcity of space in a restaurant and the price difference matter? ABSTRACT

Restaurant Revenue Management: Do perceived scarcity of space in a restaurant and the price difference matter? ABSTRACT Restaurant Revenue Management: Do perceived scarcity of space in a restaurant and the price difference matter? Cindy (Yoonjoung) Heo, Ph.D. School of Hotel and Tourism Management The Hong Kong Polytechnic

More information

JOURNAL OF OBJECT TECHNOLOGY

JOURNAL OF OBJECT TECHNOLOGY JOURNAL OF OBJECT TECHNOLOGY Online at www.jot.fm. Published by ETH Zurich, Chair of Software Engineering JOT, 2005 Vol. 4, No. 5, July-August 2005 Customer Interface Management John D. McGregor, Clemson

More information

Title: How reading comics affect teenagers

Title: How reading comics affect teenagers HONG KONG DIPLOMA OF SECONDARY EDUCATION LIBERAL STUDIES INDEPENDENT ENQUIRY STUDY Year: 2014 Title: How reading comics affect teenagers interpersonal relationship? Table of Contents Chapter 1 Abstract

More information

Does Social Exclusion Motivate Interpersonal Reconnection? Resolving the Porcupine Problem

Does Social Exclusion Motivate Interpersonal Reconnection? Resolving the Porcupine Problem INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES Does Social Exclusion Motivate Interpersonal Reconnection? Resolving the Porcupine Problem Jon K. Maner, C. Nathan DeWall, and Roy F. Baumeister Florida State

More information

E-COMMERCE EAT PRAY SHOP: RAMADAN CASE STUDY. acommerce Insights Indonesia

E-COMMERCE EAT PRAY SHOP: RAMADAN CASE STUDY. acommerce Insights Indonesia acommerce Insights Indonesia EAT PRAY SHOP: E-COMMERCE RAMADAN CASE STUDY Published July 2014 www.acommerce.asia Corporate Marketing: Felicia Moursalien 2 Does Ramadan Boost E-Commerce in Indonesia? This

More information

B-11-3. Response: Page 1 of 1

B-11-3. Response: Page 1 of 1 Information Request No..1 RR BCCA.BI.1.a-b Dated 04 May 16 March Insurance Corporation of British Columbia B-11-3.1 RR BCCA.BI.1.a-b Reference: Page 5-2, paragraph 6, the first bullet point refers to "An

More information