American Sociological Review

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "American Sociological Review"

Transcription

1 volume 76 j number 1 j February 2011 American Sociological Review Q 2010 ASA Presidential Address Constructing Citizenship Evelyn Nakano Glenn Group Tensions Neighborhood Immigration and Native Out-Migration Kyle Crowder, Matthew Hall, and Stewart E. Tolnay Network Centrality, Gender Segregation, and Aggression Robert Faris and Diane Felmlee Cross-National Stratification Who Does More Housework: Rich or Poor? Jan Paul Heisig The Limit of Equality Projects Cheol-Sung Lee, Young-Bum Kim, and Jae-Mahn Shim Trends in Political Protest Cohort and Period Trends in Protest Attendance and Petition Signing Neal Caren, Raj Andrew Ghoshal, and Vanesa Ribas Protesting While Black? Differential Policing of American Activism Christian Davenport, Sarah A. Soule, and David A. Armstrong II j A Journal of the American Sociological Association j

2 Volume 76 Number 1 February 2011 American Sociological Review Contents 2010 Presidential Address Constructing Citizenship: Exclusion, Subordination, and Resistance Evelyn Nakano Glenn 1 Articles Neighborhood Immigration and Native Out-Migration Kyle Crowder, Matthew Hall, and Stewart E. Tolnay 25 Status Struggles: Network Centrality and Gender Segregation in Same- and Cross-Gender Aggression Robert Faris and Diane Felmlee 48 Who Does More Housework: Rich or Poor? A Comparison of 33 Countries Jan Paul Heisig 74 The Limit of Equality Projects: Public-Sector Expansion, Sectoral Conflicts, and Income Inequality in Postindustrial Economies Cheol-Sung Lee, Young-Bum Kim, and Jae-Mahn Shim 100 A Social Movement Generation: Cohort and Period Trends in Protest Attendance and Petition Signing Neal Caren, Raj Andrew Ghoshal, and Vanesa Ribas 125 Protesting While Black? The Differential Policing of American Activism, 1960 to 1990 Christian Davenport, Sarah A. Soule, and David A. Armstrong II 152

3 Status Struggles: Network Centrality and Gender Segregation in Same- and Cross-Gender Aggression American Sociological Review 76(1) Ó American Sociological Association 2011 DOI: / Robert Faris a and Diane Felmlee a Abstract Literature on aggression often suggests that individual deficiencies, such as social incompetence, psychological difficulties, or troublesome home environments, are responsible for aggressive behavior. In this article, by contrast, we examine aggression from a social network perspective, arguing that social network centrality, our primary measure of peer status, increases the capacity for aggression and that competition to gain or maintain status motivates its use. We test these arguments using a unique longitudinal dataset that enables separate consideration of same- and cross-gender aggression. We find that aggression is generally not a maladjusted reaction typical of the socially marginal; instead, aggression is intrinsic to status and escalates with increases in peer status until the pinnacle of the social hierarchy is attained. Over time, individuals at the very bottom and those at the very top of a hierarchy become the least aggressive youth. We also find that aggression is influenced not so much by individual gender differences as by relationships with the other gender and patterns of gender segregation at school. When cross-gender interactions are plentiful, aggression is diminished. Yet these factors are also jointly implicated in peer status: in schools where cross-gender interactions are rare, cross-gender friendships create status distinctions that magnify the consequences of network centrality. Keywords social networks, aggression, gender, status Aggression is commonplace in U.S. schools: bullying and other forms of proactive aggression adversely affect 30 percent, or 5.7 million, American youth each school year (Nansel et al. 2001). The National Education Association (1995) estimates that each weekday, 160,000 students skip school to avoid being bullied. This aggression has important consequences. Being victimized by bullies positively relates to a host of mental health problems, including depression (Baldry 2004), anxiety (Sharp, Thompson, and Arora 2000), and suicidal ideation (Carney 2000), as well as physical health problems (Ghandour et al. 2004), social isolation (Nansel et al. 2001), and academic struggles (Berthold and a University of California-Davis Corresponding Author: Robert Faris, Department of Sociology, University of California-Davis, 2247 Social Sciences and Humanities, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA rwfaris@ucdavis.edu

4 Faris and Felmlee 49 Hoover 2000). Many of these outcomes last well into adulthood (Schäfer et al. 2004). Moreover, it is not only victims who suffer negative consequences; aggressors (compared with bystanders) also experience problems with school adjustment, mental health, and integration (Nansel et al. 2004). Perhaps because psychologists produce the bulk of the literature on aggression, research tends to focus on the role of the involved individuals personality traits. With some exceptions (e.g., Hawley 2003; Pelligrini and Long 2002), these traits consist of psychological pathologies or developmental deficiencies, conveying an impression of aggression as a counterproductive response to psychological difficulties or problems in the home. Here, we provide a contrasting view that emphasizes the role of peer status and social context. We argue that the role of personal deficiencies is overstated and that concerns over status drive much aggressive behavior. As peer status increases, so does the capacity for aggression, and competition to gain or maintain status motivates the use of aggression. Aggression wanes only at the highest echelons of status, where its utility is questionable. Gender receives particular attention in the literature on aggression, with debate centering on whether females bully as frequently and in the same ways as males (for a review, see Espelage and Swearer [2003]). We suggest that, for the most part, gender affects aggression insofar as it relates to peer status. Extending arguments about gender as a status characteristic, we suggest that the effect of gender is primarily relational and contextual: adolescents aggressive behaviors are influenced not so much by their own gender as by their relationships with the other gender, and by the degree of gender segregation at the school level, which are jointly implicated in peer status. While our primary focus is on the direct effect of peer status as reflected in social network centrality we also suggest that the effect of social network centrality on aggression is magnified when youth have cross-gender friendships in schools where such relationships are rare. Our emphasis on inter-gender relationships is not limited to friendship. Rather than presume aggressive tendencies are insensitive to the characteristics of potential victims, we consider whether same- and cross-gender aggression follow similar patterns. Conceptual Definitions Before proceeding, we should clarify our use of the terms aggression, power, status, and centrality. Following Kinney (2007), we define aggression 1 broadly as behavior directed toward harming or causing pain to another, including physical (e.g., hitting, shoving, and kicking), verbal (e.g., namecalling and threats), and indirect aggression (also called social or relational 2 aggression). Indirect aggression is defined as harmful actions perpetrated outside of a victim s immediate purview, such as spreading rumors and ostracism. Here, peer status refers to adolescents position within their informal school prestige hierarchy, based on schoolmates assessments. We use the term power to reflect the capacity to influence others and the ability to resist others influence. Hypothetically, power and status may not always overlap, but we argue that in the adolescent context, power is rooted in peer status and both are reflected by social network centrality. THEORY Social Network Centrality and Aggression The relationship between aggression and social network centrality depends on (1) whether centrality increases the capacity for aggression and (2) whether status is a primary motivation for aggression. The first question pertains to the relationship between power and network position. The strong argument

5 50 American Sociological Review 76(1) is that network position empowers the wellsituated to pursue their ends. The weak version is that network position does not create, but merely reflects, underlying power arrangements. Most scholars of networks make the strong argument that networks create power and status (Burt 1982; Freeman 1979; Friedkin 1991). Actors at the center of the network, on whom the more peripheral actors are dependent, are the most powerful actors in the system (Marsden and Laumann 1977:217). Individuals who link otherwise disconnected actors that is, individuals who bridge structural holes enjoy special advantages in social and economic exchanges (Burt 1982) and exercise control over information flows (Freeman 1979). Power-dependency theory shows that networks create tangible power imbalances by regulating the availability of alternative exchange partners (Cook et al. 1983; Emerson 1972; Molm 1990). Even if network position is initially caused by exogenous personal qualities, central actors likely receive a disproportionate share of new nominations, not because of personal qualities, but by virtue of having already received many nominations (Gould 2002). To accept the weak argument, one would have to assume that no such Matthew Effects occur (or that ties do not matter). Some scholars may be unaccustomed to talking about power in the context of high schools. However, as media coverage of youth who were bullied to death suggests, some adolescents have the power to make their peers lives miserable. These same adolescents are also apt to employ more subtle means of persuasion and pressure, backed by threat or reward, to achieve their goals. 3 Youths who hold this power, exercised or latent, are likely found at the busy intersections of social networks rather than at the margins. Youths who bridge structural holes that is, youths who have many friends who are not friends with each other have greater access to valued resources like potential romantic partners or social options on the weekend. Because their social lives do not depend on any single friend or group of friends, they can exclude others more easily than they can be excluded themselves. This gives them greater leverage in each of their friendships (these friendships often involve subtle dominance struggles [Gould 2003]). Because these youths have ties to multiple peer groups, they are also in a position to receive and disseminate information and gossip (accurate or otherwise). In addition, actors in brokerage positions presumably have a large number of allies they can mobilize as needed. Even if the weak argument is correct and network centrality only reflects some set of underlying qualities, central students likely wield greater influence than do marginal students. The characteristics that make them central affluence, attractiveness, athleticism, or charisma should enable them to influence, manipulate, or otherwise dominate their peers. In addition, occupying a central position within a school s social network, as compared to a more isolated position, directly increases the opportunities to engage in aggressive interactions with one s peers. Regardless of the causal relationship between power and network position, these arguments agree that network centrality is positively associated with the ability to exploit or otherwise harm others. What they do not explain, however, is when and why harm doing occurs. With respect to the second question about the motivation for aggression, many adolescentsperceivepeerstatustobeanimportant goal and aggression to be an effective means of attaining it. Alternatively, they view a lack of aggressiveness, and in particular, passivity in the face of another s harmful behavior, as leading to subsequent losses in social status and power. Little research examines whether aggression is, in fact, an effective way of gaining or maintaining status (although one recent study found that, under certain conditions, aggression led to subsequent status gains [Rodkin and Berger 2008]), but more important for our purposes is whether aggression is believed to increase

6 Faris and Felmlee 51 status. To our knowledge, this question has not been addressed directly, but research has established a correlation between status concerns and aggression. Gould (2003) convincingly argues that social rank is of utmost importance for interaction and that serious, even deadly, conflicts arise when relative social rank is unclear or in dispute. 4 Other scholars maintain that ambiguity with respect to ownership of resources increases conflict and violence (Villareal 2004). These arguments present aggression as a reaction to status-related affronts, but other research finds a link to proactive aggression as well. Subcultural theories of crime and violence underscore the argument that enhancing peer status and respect are motivations for violent behavior among certain subcultures that prize readiness to fight (Anderson 1994; Cloward and Ohlin 1960; Miller 1958). Recent research finds that violence among males relates positively to peer acceptance in school, particularly for students with poor academic performance (Kreager 2007b). Additionally, proactively aggressive adolescents have strong and their victims weak status motivations (Sijtsema et al. 2009). Adolescents who seek status become more aggressive over time; and net of their own individual motivations, youth in friendship groups that emphasize status are also more aggressive (Faris and Ennett forthcoming). If centrality increases the capacity for aggression, and if adolescents view aggression as a means to enhance status (or prevent losses), then greater network centrality likely leads to increases in subsequent aggression. Our argument thus far that aggression is perceived to be instrumental behavior for gaining status and that centrality is associated with greater capacity for it stands in contrast to much of the psychological literature on aggression, which views it as a reaction to psychological problems, social insecurities, or troubled home lives. Many studies find that aggressive youth are disliked, unpopular, and experience difficulty maintaining relationships with classmates (e.g., Adler and Adler 1995; Garandeau and Cillessen 2006). This perspective suggests that aggressive youth are likely peripheral in a school s social network and, far from enhancing status, acting out likely completes their marginalization. While we suspect that such research overestimates the link between social marginality and aggression due to measurement methods indirect aggression, for instance, is often ignored and theoretical emphases on mental health, we acknowledge the capacity of any student to be violent, and that certain socially marginal subcultures can be quite aggressive. We thus anticipate some exceptions to the generally positive association between centrality and aggression, but we argue that the more substantial departure occurs at the top, not the bottom, of the status hierarchy. There are several reasons to expect individuals at the pinnacle of the status hierarchy to desist in aggression. Fundamentally, if aggression is purely instrumental for status attainment, then individuals at the top willnotfinditparticularly useful or needed; pro-social tactics may achieve their ends more effectively (Hawley 2003). Kindness maybealuxurymosteasilyenjoyedfrom a secure position at the pinnacle of a hierarchy, or by individuals who have no hope (or desire) to reach such heights. A friendly compliment from the homecoming queen or the star quarterback is likely to cement, not minimize, status differences, while a cutting remark is likely to signal status insecurity rather than dominance. In a similar vein, aggression may expose power arrangements that, once revealed, render further aggression unnecessary. Thus, while we expect that centrality monotonically increases the capacity for aggression, the need for aggression decreases once one attains the upper reaches of social prominence. This leads to our first hypothesis: Hypothesis 1: Network centrality at Time 1 increases aggression at Time 2 until relatively high levels of centrality are reached,

7 52 American Sociological Review 76(1) at which point additional increases in centrality decrease subsequent aggression. Cross-Gender Friendships That males are more aggressive than females is virtually axiomatic for many researchers, and most empirical studies support this view (e.g., Coie and Dodge 1998), including several meta-analyses (e.g., Archer 2004; Hyde 1984). Gender differences are especially pronounced if the analysis is limited to physical aggression, but they often persist even with broad conceptions of aggression. Proposed explanations for the difference include the Male-Warrior Hypothesis, which emphasizes biological traits (Van Vugt, De Cremer, and Janssen 2007), as well as entrenched cultural beliefs about gender roles and stereotypes (Berger, Cohen, and Zelditch 1972; Ridgeway and Correll 2004). Other scholars note that differences between the genders in interpersonal behavior tend to be exaggerated or essentialized, and that differences in social context and structural location account for large amounts of variance in behavior (Felmlee 1999). Understanding the magnitude and root causes of these differences is important, but we argue that preoccupation with gender differences obscures other important ways in which gender matters for aggression. With few exceptions (e.g., Rodkin and Berger 2008), scholars have not considered how inter-gender interaction shapes aggression. Therefore, in addition to individual-level gender effects, we consider the consequences of cross-gender friendships and corresponding levels of informal gender segregation at the school level. Our expectations are guided by two propositions in addition to our fundamental assertion that aggression is enabled by, and instrumental for, social climbing. First, social interaction increases opportunities for conflict. Second, favorable interaction with the other gender is a common goal for many adolescents. Regarding the former, social interaction is a prerequisite for aggression, and the more time people spend together, the more opportunity there is for conflict to arise. There are additional reasons, however, why aggression may accompany friendship formation, and cross-gender friendship formation in particular. Gould (2003) argues that friendships feature underlying dominance claims that, when challenged, result in dramatic conflicts. This argument encompasses same- and crossgender friendships alike, but social ties between the two genders include the additional difficulties of higher levels of uncertainty and topic avoidance. Cross-gender friendships thus hold greater potential for misunderstanding and conflict (Afifi and Burgoon 1998; Tannen 1990). Indeed, recent research finds an association between cross-gender friendships and violent or antisocial behavior (Arndorfer and Stormshak 2008; Haynie, Steffensmeier, and Bell 2003). For female youth, studies find a link between a high proportion of cross-gender friendships and crime (McCarthy, Felmlee, and Hagan 2004). Cross-gender friendships also lead to dating activity (Feiring 1999), which may further intensify conflict. 5 While aggression does transpire between friends, we suspect the bulk occurs within adjacent relations (e.g., when classmates become rivals for the attention of a mutual friend). These relations are both more prevalent than friendship (one invariably has more friends-of-friends than one has friends) and lack incentives to keep conflict in check. Friends, after all, at least like each other, whatever their differences. There is no such presumption about friends-of-friends. Crossgender friendships will entail a certain amount of conflict, but more important, they will bring actors into more frequent contact with cross-gender friends-of-friends. We thus anticipate that cross-gender aggression will increase with the rate of cross-gender friendships, by virtue of increased opportunity and competition. In a similar vein, we expect that high levels of gender segregation at the school level (or a low rate of cross-gender friendships relative to what we would expect

8 Faris and Felmlee 53 by chance alone [Freeman 1978]) will decrease the opportunity for interaction and conflict between the two genders and therefore produce lower levels of cross-gender aggression over time. Hypothesis 2a: Cross-gender friendships increase cross-gender aggression. Hypothesis 2b: School gender segregation decreases cross-gender aggression. Our second gender proposition, that crossgender relationships are an important goal for many adolescents, is unlikely to be controversial and finds support in numerous studies. In the 1960s, for example, Coleman s (1961) ethnography of high school documented the importance of success with the other gender. While young children s relationships are usually completely homogenous with respect to gender (Eder and Hallinan 1978), gender homophily wanes after the onset of puberty, and cross-gender friendships become increasingly common (Shrum, Cheek, and Hunter 1988). Such friendships become tied to peer status as adolescence proceeds (Bukowski, Sippola, and Hoza 1999; Collins 2003), perhaps because they often lead to romantic relationships (Feiring 1999), which also flourish over this period (Laursen and Williams 1997). If cross-gender friendships are an important goal and aggression is generally instrumental, we might expect youth who form such ties, and reach their goal, to desist from aggression. Additionally, compared with individuals without such friendships, adolescents with multiple cross-gender friends arguably spend less of their free time interacting and hence, feuding with peers of their own gender. For the same reason, we anticipate relatively higher rates of same-gender aggression in gender-segregated schools, where there are few cross-gender friendships and arguably less interaction between genders. We therefore test the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 3a: Cross-gender friendships decrease same-gender aggression. Hypothesis 3b: School gender segregation increases same-gender aggression. Gender Bridges Our primary interest with respect to gender, and cross-gender friendship specifically, is its relationship to peer status. We suggested that cross-gender friendships are a desirable goal. Goal attainment, however, does not necessarily imply status distinction. Because status is relative, distinctions can only be created when traits or behaviors are uncommon. In the present case, cross-gender friendships may provide their own satisfactions, but they cannot create status distinctions if they are common. Furthermore, the direction of the status distinction whether non-normative behavior leads to prestige or stigma depends in part on the status of those engaged in it. In schools where cross-gender friendships are uncommon, the social distinctions created by these friendships are likely to be empowering (e.g., by providing access to the other gender) for central adolescents and stigmatizing (e.g., by violating gender norms) for marginal ones. Because we believe that aggression is entwined with power and status, we call attention to a unique social position, a gender bridge. We define a gender bridge as a student who has cross-gender friendships in schools where such friendships are rare. We argue that the positive effect of centrality on aggression is enhanced for gender bridges, such that socially prominent gender bridges enjoy substantial magnification of their already high levels of peer status. These students are thus likely to be even more aggressive toward other students (compared with comparably central students). For example, consider two hypothetical students attending a gender-segregated school. Students A and B have high centrality, but A has multiple cross-gender friendships while B has none. Assuming cross-gender friendships are desirable, A s status is magnified relative to B s by virtue of having these friendships when few of their classmates do. We suggest that

9 54 American Sociological Review 76(1) this effect operates primarily through the symbolic value of having rare and valued ties, but these friendships may also provide more tangible advantages such as invitations to parties, access to a wider range of gossip, and exposure to more potential dating partners. Whatever the mechanisms, such magnification in peer status is likely accompanied by increased aggression, for reasons outlined earlier. We therefore test our final hypothesis: Hypothesis 4: The effect of centrality on sameand cross-gender aggression will be magnified for gender bridges. Before proceeding with a description of the data, three notes are in order. First, some research on cross-gender interaction finds different consequences by gender (Haynie et al. 2003). Such findings suggest it is not crossgender friendships per se, but friendships with boys that influence outcomes. Although we construct our variables in cross-gender and same-gender terms (as opposed to male friends and female friends), we take these results seriously and test for gender interactions in our analyses. Second, much research explores differences in how adolescents are aggressive. Some scholars assume that because females have less physical strength, 6 for example, they may rely more on indirect and verbal aggression (Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, and Kaukiainen 1992; Hyde 2005). 7 However, we agree with Espelage and Swearer (2003) and Jackman (1994) that broader not narrower or more varied notions of aggression are needed to fully examine the role of gender. If aggression is used tactically, we might expect it to take on a variety of forms depending on the situation. Nonetheless, we test our primary hypotheses separately for physical, verbal, and indirect aggression. Finally, our argument about gender centers on platonic friendships rather than romantic activity. This is partly due to practical reasons (network data on dating partners is unavailable), but we also argue that while cross-gender friendships often lead (directly or indirectly) to dating relationships, they are important in their own right and likely to outlast transitory romantic relationships (Brown, Feiring, and Furman 1999). Because dating activity is associated (under certain conditions) with delinquency (McCarthy and Casey 2008), we control for dating involvement and test for interactions with key variables. Contributions of This Study The literature on aggressive peer interaction is voluminous. Nevertheless, it exhibits a number of limitations. Many studies rely on small, cross-sectional samples, inhibiting investigation of predictors of aggression. Much research also relies solely on selfreported aggression, which may underestimate socially undesirable behavior. Few studies collect detailed sociometric data on friendship ties, and to our knowledge, only two other studies have collected data on networks of aggressive relationships, and these consist mostly of younger children (Rodkin and Berger 2008; Veenstra et al. 2007). We attempt to address these limitations by the following means: We obtain reports from both the respondents and their peers regarding incidents of harmful behavior. We use a relatively large sample of data from 3,722 students gathered from all of the public middle- and highschool students in three counties of one state (19 schools). We undertake a dynamic analysis of aggressive behavior using longitudinal data from each school gathered at two time points. Most important, we use a network measure of aggression that enables investigation of target characteristics. We analyze aggression relationally and consider new questions, in this case, whether cross-gender and samegender aggression arise from common factors. DATA, MEASURES, AND METHODS Data for these analyses come from The Context of Adolescent Substance Use study

10 Faris and Felmlee 55 (Ennett et al. 2000), a longitudinal in-school survey of adolescents in three counties in North Carolina that began in the spring of 2002 and was administered every six months through Wave 6 (in the fall of 2005), until it concluded with a final survey one year later (such that the oldest cohort was followed through 12th grade). At Wave 1, all publicschool students in grades 6 through 8 in each of three counties were eligible, resulting in a study population of 8,201 students, of which 7,173 (88 percent) participated at least once. For each of the seven waves, the response rate was maintained at or above 75 percent. Our sample includes the 4,266 students who participated in Waves 4 (fall 2004) and 5 (spring 2005); 8 after listwise deletion of missing data (544 of 4,266 total cases were dropped), our final sample includes 3,722 students. Models with school clustering and multiple imputation of missing data (with 20 imputations) for the 544 cases produce substantively identical results. Dependent Variables To measure aggression, students were asked to nominate up to five schoolmates who picked on or were mean to them, and up to five peers whom they picked on or were mean to. Students were instructed to disregard playful teasing and focus only on harmful actions. We merge the two networks (i.e., the network according to victims, and the network according to aggressors) such that an aggressive link from A to B is considered present if A nominated B as a victim or B nominated A as an aggressor. 9 From this combined network, we calculate aggression outdegree, or the number of students a respondent harassed or attacked. We then define cross-gender and same-gender aggression as the number of other- and same-gender peers toward whom a respondent was aggressive. Additionally, for each nomination, students were asked (separately) whether the relationship involved physical attacks, direct verbal harassment, or indirect aggression. From these reports, we generate networks of physical (e.g., links that involved physical attacks such as hitting, shoving, or tripping), verbal (e.g., name-calling and other verbal abuse), and indirect aggression (e.g., spreading rumors, ostracism, or related forms of behind-the-back harassment). All dependent variables were measured in the spring of 2005 (Wave 5). We control for the prior level of each outcome by including the appropriate aggression measures from the beginning of that same school year (fall 2004, or Wave 4). Independent Variables We measure the key independent variables using friendship network data. Students were asked to name up to five of their best friends; from these nominations, we calculate measures of social network centrality. We examine betweenness centrality, which is calculated by first determining the shortest paths, or geodesics, between all pairs of actors (using undirected ties), and then calculating the percentage of all these geodesics that include the focal actor. There are multiple centrality measures, each emphasizing different structural properties (Borgatti and Everett 2006; Freeman 1979). We focus on betweenness centrality, as opposed to other popular measures like Bonacich centrality (Bonacich 1987), because it best captures the brokerage position we associate with status in this context and matches our conception of gender bridges. 10 We also use the friendship nominations to calculate multiple cross-gender friendships, a binary indicator of whether a respondent had at least two cross-gender friends. At the school level, we measure gender segregation using Freeman s (1978) segregation index, which compares observed and expected patterns of ties across groups. The measure ranges from 21 to 1, where a 1 is perfect segregation or homophily (i.e., all friendships are same-gender), a 0 indicates that same- and

11 56 American Sociological Review 76(1) cross-gender friendships are equally likely, and a 21 indicates perfect heterophily (i.e., all cross-gender friendships). Control Variables We include a number of conventional control variables, but some bear further discussion. First, one of the strongest predictors of adolescents behavior in a given domain is the behavior of their friends (Ennett and Bauman 1994; Mercken et al. 2010). Aggression may be particularly susceptible to peer influence processes, because it is often perpetrated by multiple adolescents (Craig and Pepler 1997; Salmivalli et al. 1996) and friends frequently target the same victims (Card and Hodges 2006). Network studies find strong correlations between the aggressive behavior of adolescents and their friends aggression (Mouttapa et al. 2004). Because central actors have more friends, they may also have more exposure to aggressive peers. 11 To ensure that the relationship between centrality and aggression is not a spurious result of peer influence and diffusion processes, we include friends average aggression, defined as the average aggression level of a respondent s friends, including those who nominate the respondent but do not receive a nomination in return (results do not change when only nominated friends are included). Romantic involvement and ensuing conflicts may also confound the relationship between status and aggression, so we include a binary indicator of whether a respondent had ever been on a date. The survey instrument defines dates as informal activities like meeting someone at the mall, a park, or at a basketball game as well as more formal activities like going out to eat or to a movie together. Studies find that pubertal development is positively related to aggression (Batsche and Knoff 1994; Olweus 1993), and this was measured by asking compared to most others your age and gender, do you think your physical development is much earlier, somewhat earlier, about the same, somewhat later, or much later? along with a series of detailed physical items. Cronbach s alpha for pubertal development is.73 for boys and.68 for girls. We measure grade point average (GPA) on a four-point scale based on a respondent s self-reported grades in English, mathematics, science, and social studies or history. We include a binary indicator of participation in school sports because some sports are linked to fighting (Kreager 2007a). We also include conventional demographic and socioeconomic control variables, including race (African American, Latino, other minority; white is the reference category), grade in school (9th or 10th grade; 8th grade is the reference), a binary indicator of a single-parent home, and a binary indicator of low educational attainment (this equals 1 if no parent attended college, 0 otherwise). 12 Methods Because the dependent variables are counts of the number of victims each respondent was aggressive toward (or the number of aggressors who picked on a respondent), we estimate negative binomial models that include a parameter that captures the overdispersion of the dependent variable (Long 1997). Coefficients can be interpreted such that a one-unit increase in X ij multiplies the expected outcome measure by a factor of exp (B j ). 13 Non-independence of data points is often a concern in analyses of students in schools, so we include school-level random effects in all of our models. We also tested school fixed effects and the results are identical. Moreover, we estimate a dynamic regression model with a lagged dependent variable and lagged covariates to control for potential problems associated with the interpretation of causal order in our models. We measure all covariates (including the lagged dependent variable) in fall 2004 (Time 1) and all dependent variables at a second time point approximately six months later (spring 2005).

12 Faris and Felmlee 57 RESULTS Descriptive Statistics The average student was aggressive toward just.63 schoolmates and, consistent with national prevalence estimates (Nansel et al. 2001), the majority (67 percent) of students were not aggressive toward anyone at Time 2 (see Table 1). Average aggression, like all other aggression measures, declined from fall 2004 to spring Betweenwave correlations of all aggression measures range from.25 to.36, indicating substantial individual variability across time. Females were significantly more aggressive than males at the start of the school year (although not at the end), but as might be expected, males were more physically aggressive. Males were also more aggressive toward females than females were toward males, and females were more aggressive toward other females than males were toward other males. Compared to boys, girls occupy significantly more central network positions, are more physically developed, and earn higher grades, but they are less likely to participate in sports. Roughly two-thirds of all students had been on a date. We plot the friendship and aggression networks of two typical high schools, Washington and Jefferson (see Figure 1). The nodes are shaded and shaped according to gender. We established their positions in the graphs by applying a springer-embedder routine in the Netdraw component of UCINET (Borgatti, Everett, and Freeman 2002) to the friendship network such that node positions are based on friendship relations and do not change when the friendship relations are replaced with aggression (the bottom half of the figures). We exclude adolescents who had no friendly or aggressive links with their peers. Relatively isolated individuals are located on the periphery, while central students fall in the dense regions of the graph. Washington High is highly segregated with respect to gender (Freeman segregation =.84); cross-gender friendships are more common in Jefferson High (Freeman segregation =.53). Both aggression networks are substantially less dense than their corresponding friendship networks, but aggressive relationships roughly follow a similar pattern as friendship the most dense regions of the aggression network are also the most dense in the friendship network. Additionally, most aggressive links are of modest length, with few aggressive acts stretching from one side of the graph to the other. This suggests a tendency to target victims within, rather than across, social circles. Finally, cross-gender aggression appears to be less common than same-gender aggression, but it seems relatively more likely to occur in the school with lower gender segregation. We turn to multivariate analyses to determine whether this is in fact the case. Centrality and Aggression We begin multivariate analysis by estimating models of overall aggression as well as the three subtypes of aggression, controlling for prior aggression levels (see Table 2). Our purpose in these initial models is to test whether centrality has the hypothesized effect on aggression, and whether it has the same effect on each subtype. As hypothesized, we find that centrality has a curvilinear effect on change in aggression (Model 1). Initially, increases in centrality are accompanied by increases in the rate of aggression (see Figure 2). When betweenness centrality reaches approximately 4 (i.e., a student is included in 4 percent of all geodesics), aggression plateaus and then begins to decline. Note that the distribution of centrality is highly skewed and approximately 86 percent of students have scores less than 2. Increases in centrality would thus result in decreasing aggression for only a small percentage of very high-status adolescents. For the vast majority of students, additional centrality increases lead to increased aggression. We also find the hypothesized curvilinear

13 58 American Sociological Review 76(1) Table 1. Descriptive Statistics Females Males Overall SE Min. Max. Overall aggression, spring Overall aggression, fall Physical aggression, spring Physical aggression, fall Verbal aggression, spring Verbal aggression, fall Indirect aggression, spring Indirect aggression, fall Cross-gender aggression, spring Cross-gender aggression, fall Same-gender aggression, spring Same-gender aggression, fall Betweenness centrality Has multiple cross-gender friendships School gender segregation Friends average aggression Has been on a date GPA Sports Pubertal development School size Female Male African American Latino Other minority White Grade Grade Grade Single-parent home No parent attended college Note: N = 3,722. Bold = statistically significant gender difference (p \.05). effect of centrality on change in physical, indirect, and verbal aggression (Models 2 to 4), although we only find this effect for physical and indirect aggression after removing five outliers. 14 As with overall aggression, all three subtypes plateau when centrality approaches 3 to 4 percent and then decline at the upper echelons (see Figure 2). We find no evidence of gender differences in overall, physical, or verbal aggression, although models of physical aggression that exclude physical aggression at Time 1 (not shown) do find that females are less physically aggressive than males. Expected rates of indirect aggression, however, are 22 percent higher for females than for males. For all outcomes, we tested for interactions between gender and both centrality parameters (not shown), but none are statistically significant. Because centrality has nearly identical effects on each subtype, and because the majority of aggressive relationships involve more than one form of aggression, we adopt the composite measure of aggression in subsequent models.

14 Faris and Felmlee 59 Figure 1. Networks of Friendship (Top) and Aggression (Bottom) in Washington and Jefferson High Schools, by Gender Note: Dark circle = male; light triangle = female. Before proceeding to models of gendered aggression, some control variables are noteworthy. As might be expected, we find evidence of peer influence: a one-unit increase in the measure of friends average aggression is associated with increases in aggression, ranging (across the outcomes) from 9 (verbal aggression) to 16 percent (physical aggression). Adolescents who play sports became more aggressive, but this appears to mostly take the form of verbal aggression. Youth involved in dating activities are between 22 and 30 percent more aggressive than their peers who have never been on a date. High school sophomores are 69 percent less physically aggressive than 8th graders, but there are no other age or cohort effects. Perhaps because it is more difficult to spread rumors in very large settings, small schools have higher rates of indirect aggression. With the exception of low parent education (which decreases rates of overall and verbal aggression), race and family background factors have no effect on aggressive behavior. Same-Gender Aggression Next, we turn to models of same-gender aggression, where we grand-mean center betweenness centrality and school gender segregation to ease interpretation of interaction effects (see Table 3). Contrary to expectations, we find that centrality does not have a significant effect on change in same-gender aggression, even after removing five outliers (Model 1). However, when we add crossgender friends and school gender segregation to the model (Model 2), we find the hypothesized positive main effect of centrality (onetail test), although the quadratic term is not significant. School gender segregation has no effect on same-gender aggression, but we find that cross-gender friendships decrease aggression, as hypothesized. Adolescents who have multiple cross-gender

15 60 American Sociological Review 76(1) Table 2. Random Effects Negative Binomial Regression of Overall, Physical, Indirect, and Verbal Aggression, with School Intercepts, Spring 2005 (Time 2) (1) Overall Aggression (2) Physical Aggression (3) Indirect Aggression (4) Verbal Aggression b SE b SE b SE b SE Female ** Centrality.13**.04.22**.07.10*.05.12*.05 Centrality squared 2.01* ** * ^.01 Friends average aggression.10**.04.15**.05.11*.05.09*.04 Has been on a date.20***.06.23*.09.26***.08.14*.07 GPA Sports.12* *.07 Pubertal development Dependent variable in.28***.01.38***.03.35***.02.33***.02 fall 2004 (Time 1) African American Latino Other minority Grade Grade * Single-parent home No parent attended college 2.13* *.07 School size (hundreds) * Constant 2.70*** *** *** ***.23 Ln R Ln S Log likelihood Wald Chisq(17) *** *** *** *** N 3,722 3,717 3,717 3,722 ^p \.05 (one-tail test); * p \.05; ** p \.01; *** p \.001 (two-tail test). friends are 16 percent less aggressive toward their same-gender peers, on average. Finally, we test our gender bridge hypothesis (Final Model). After including the hypothesized three-way and all possible twoway interaction terms, the main and quadratic effects of centrality become significant (and in the expected directions). The main effect of cross-gender friends is essentially unchanged. We also find the hypothesized positive threeway interaction between centrality, crossgender friends, and school gender segregation, net of all possible two-way interactions (none of which are statistically significant). To ease interpretation, we plot the joint predicted effect of centrality, cross-gender friendships, and school gender segregation in Figures 3a to 3c. 15 We advise caution in interpreting these results, and we emphasize that a relatively small number of students qualify as gender bridges (approximately 8 percent of our sample); of these, roughly half have above-average centrality scores, and oneeighth (or 1 percent of the full sample) have scores above 2. Nonetheless, we find that, at average levels of school gender segregation (see Figure 3a), centrality exerts a modest positive effect on same-gender aggression for students who have multiple cross-gender friendships and those who do not. Moving from 21 to2on centrality increases the predicted same-gender aggression of both groups by.13 and.19, respectively (see Stolzenberg [2004] for a discussion of using partial derivatives to measure effects in polynomial regressions). However,

16 Faris and Felmlee Predicted Aggression Rate Overall Physical Verbal Indirect Betweenness Centrality Figure 2. Predicted Aggression Rate by Social Network Centrality as school gender segregation increases (see Figures 3b and 3c), the slope of centrality increases for youth with multiple cross-gender friendships. In highly gender-segregated schools (see Figure 3c), increases in centrality are associated with sharp increases in samegender aggression for students who have more than one cross-gender friendship. For other students, centrality has a negligible or negative effect on same-gender aggression. Note, however, that the effect of having cross-gender friendships strongly depends on centrality, and it is associated with reduced aggression among the socially marginal students only at relatively high levels of centrality (above 2) does it increase rates of aggression. As we anticipated, the effect of centrality on aggression is greatly magnified for gender bridges. However, even these youth ultimately desist from same-gender aggression when centrality reaches the upper extremes (beyond those shown in the figures), as suggested by the significant negative coefficient for centrality squared. Some control variables are noteworthy. First, despite the significant bivariate gender difference in same-gender aggression, females are not significantly different from males when other variables are controlled. Consistent with earlier models, having aggressive friends increases and having parents who never attended college significantly decreases the rate of same-gender aggression. Youth involved in dating are also more aggressive toward classmates of the same gender. Compared with whites and African American students, Latino students are more aggressive toward same-gender peers. Cross-Gender Aggression Next, we estimate models of cross-gender aggression (see Table 4). We again find the

17 62 American Sociological Review 76(1) Table 3. Random Effects Negative Binomial Regression of Same-Gender Aggression, with School Intercepts, Spring 2005 (Time 2) Model 1 Model 2 Final Model b SE b SE b SE Female Centrality ^.04.10*.04 Centrality squared *.01 Multiple cross-gender 2.17* *.10 friendships School gender segregation Multiple cross-gender friendships centrality Gender segregation 3 centrality Multiple cross-gender friendships gender segregation Multiple cross-gender friendships 1.79**.69 3 gender segregation 3 centrality Friends average aggression.11*.04.11**.04.11*.04 Has been on a date.20**.07.21**.07.21**.07 GPA Sports Pubertal development Same-gender aggression, fall.36***.02.36***.02.36*** (Time 1) African American Latino.43**.15.42**.15.41**.15 Other minority Grade Grade Single-parent home No parent attended college 2.20** ** **.07 School size (hundreds) Constant 2.82*** *** ***.25 Ln R Ln S Log likelihood Wald Chi Square (df) *** (17) *** (19) *** (23) Note: N = 3,717. ^p \.05 (one-tail test); * p \.05; ** p \.01; *** p \.001 (two-tail test). hypothesized curvilinear, inverted-u effect of network centrality (Model 1). Contrary to our hypotheses, however, cross-gender friendships have no significant effect on cross-gender aggression. Also contrary to our expectations, school gender segregation significantly increases cross-gender aggression. Compared with students in an average school, students in a school that is.1 points higher on gender-segregation are approximately 17 percent more aggressive toward the other gender (Model 2). Finally, we test the three-way interaction between centrality, cross-gender friendships, and gender segregation (along with all possible two-way interactions). Again, we note that caution is in order due to the relatively small number of cases involved. However, we again find support for the gender bridge hypothesis: the positive effect of centrality

18 Faris and Felmlee 63 Predicted Same-Gender Aggression a. Gender Segregation =.0 Multiple Opposite-Gender Friends Zero or One Opposite-Gender Friend 3b. Gender Segregation = Betweenness Centrality Betweenness Centrality 3c. Gender Segregation = Betweenness Centrality Figure 3. Predicted Rate of Same-Gender Aggression, by Centrality, Gender Segregation, and Cross-Gender Friendships on cross-gender aggression is significantly magnified for gender bridges (Final Model). The joint effects are plotted in Figures 4a to 4c and are quite similar to those shown in Figures 3a to 3c (although predicted rates of cross-gender aggression are substantially lower). In schools with average levels of gender segregation (see Figure 4a), the effect of centrality is virtually identical for adolescents regardless of the number of cross-gender friendships they have. For both groups, cross-gender aggression is predicted to be more than two times as high for students near the pinnacle of the social hierarchy, compared with those at the bottom. As school gender segregation increases (see Figures 4b and 4c), however, centrality begins to have a strong positive effect on the aggressive behavior of youth with multiple cross-gender friendships, while having a diminishing and ultimately negligible effect on their schoolmates. Gender bridges have predicted rates of cross-gender aggression that are several times greater than comparably central students. For example, when gender segregation equals.2 and centrality equals 3.0, predicted cross-gender aggression for youth with multiple cross-gender friends is 1.79, compared with.55 for equally central classmates without friends of the other gender. Our results are strikingly similar for same- and cross-gender aggression network centrality generally increases aggression, especially for gender bridges, until the very highest levels of centrality are attained. A few control variables merit attention, particularly gender. In contrast to samegender aggression, females are just 73 percent as aggressive toward males as males are toward females. Other results are consistent across both outcomes: youth who are involved in dating are 23 percent more aggressive toward the other gender, compared with students who had never been on a date. We also find higher rates of cross-gender aggression among youth whose friends are aggressive. However, we find no significant differences based on family background or other control variables. We illustrate the actual aggressive and friendly relations of gender bridges (see Figure 5) using friendship and aggression relations (both from the fall) in a highly gendersegregated network of 8th graders (Freeman segregation index =.83). Although there are some cross-gender friendships, students are strongly clustered according to gender. The gray nodes represent students with cross-gender friendships and high centrality scores. As the dark lines suggest, these gender bridges are deeply embedded in the aggression network. MODEL ROBUSTNESS For each of the preceding analyses, we had a number of methodological concerns

19 64 American Sociological Review 76(1) Table 4. Random Effects Negative Binomial Regression of Opposite-Gender Aggression, with School Intercepts, Spring 2005 (Time 2) Model 1 Model 2 Final Model b SE b SE b SE Female 2.32*** *** ***.09 Centrality.16***.05.16***.05.18***.05 Centrality squared 2.02* * **.01 Multiple cross-gender friendships School gender segregation 1.56* *.72 Multiple cross-gender friendships centrality Gender segregation 3 centrality Multiple cross-gender friendships gender segregation Multiple cross-gender friendships 1.70*.79 3 gender segregation 3 centrality Friends average aggression.13*.06.13*.06.13*.06 Has been on a date.20*.09.21*.09.21*.09 GPA Sports Pubertal development Cross-gender aggression, fall.47***.04.47***.04.47*** (Time 1) African American Latino Other minority Grade Grade Single-parent home No parent attended college School size (hundreds) Constant 2.80** *** *.30 Ln R Ln S Log likelihood Wald Chi Square (df) *** (17) *** (19) *** (23) Note: N = 3,722. ^p \.05 (one-tail test); * p \.05; ** p \.01; *** p \.001 (two-tail test). regarding our measures, their empirical distributions, and the causal relationships among them. We were concerned that the effects of our variables might differ by gender, age, grade, or dating involvement, so we tested for interactions between these and our key independent variables. None are significant. Additionally, while we believe that betweenness centrality best fits our conception of social status in this context, we also tested Bonacich centrality and friendship indegree (i.e., the number of friendship nominations received). We find the same curvilinear effect for Bonacich centrality for same- and cross-gender aggression, and (likely because of its more limited distribution) a linear but positive effect of indegree for both forms of aggression (see Table S1 in the online supplement [ Our most important

20 Faris and Felmlee 65 Predicted Cross-Gender Aggression a. Gender Segregation =.0 Multiple Opposite-Gender Friends Zero or One Opposite-Gender Friend Betweenness Centrality 4b. Gender Segregation = Betweenness Centrality 4c. Gender Segregation = Betweenness Centrality Figure 4. Predicted Rate of Cross-Gender Aggression, by Centrality, Gender Segregation, and Cross-Gender Friendships results are relatively robust to the choice of centrality measures. Furthermore, it is possible that aggression and friendship coincide (at least within the three-month period the measures reference), and moreover, that such aggression may differ in meaningful ways from non-friend aggression. We find that a small percentage of aggressive behavior (less than 7 percent) occurred within friendships, and we find no substantive differences if we exclude these joint friend aggression relationships. It is also possible that the curvilinear effects in our models are an artifact of the highly skewed centrality distribution. 16 We examine that possibility by excluding all cases in which centered betweenness is greater than 2 (the top 7 percent of the distribution) and reanalyzing the data. We continue to find significant negative coefficients for the quadratic terms for all three aggression outcomes (i.e., same-gender, other-gender, and overall aggression). Again, we find the same inverse-u result if we use Bonacich centrality, which is substantially less skewed. We do truncate the range of centrality in our figures (cutting off at 5 on the mean-centered score, when the maximum observed is 12), because the data are so sparse at the upper extreme. 17 In a similar vein, we were concerned about influential outliers. As mentioned earlier, we did find at least five influential cases that, if included in the model of same-gender aggression, reduce the threeway gender bridge interaction as well as the squared term of centrality to nonsignificant levels. These five cases are predominantly female (four of five), minority (four of five), and athletes (all five), and are highly aggressive (on average, 10 times the sample mean) and highly central (at the 97th percentile or above). These cases represent substantively interesting exceptions to our argument regarding desistence at the pinnacle of the social hierarchy. Additionally, one of the schools is an outlier with respect to gender segregation, with a value of.98 on a scale of 21 to 1, indicating that cross-gender friendships were nearly nonexistent. Dropping this school does not affect the results for cross-gender aggression, and it only marginally affects the results for same-gender aggression (the three-way interaction term s significance falls to p \.05 with a one-tail test). Fundamentally, we were also concerned with the complexity of estimating a three-way interaction effect (along with the corresponding set of two-way interactions), so we tested our gender bridge hypothesis by stratifying the sample based on school gender segregation. We initially divided the sample into three categories high (approximately the top 15 percent of the gendersegregation distribution), medium (the middle 70 percent of gender segregation), and low (the bottom 15 percent) but the results

Casualties of Social Combat: School Networks of Peer Victimization and Their Consequences

Casualties of Social Combat: School Networks of Peer Victimization and Their Consequences 524573ASRXXX10.1177/0003122414524573American Sociological ReviewFaris and Felmlee 2014 Casualties of Social Combat: School Networks of Peer Victimization and Their Consequences American Sociological Review

More information

NEW PERSPECTIVES ON BULLYING PREVENTION: WHY ARE CURRENT PROGRAMS NOT WORKING?

NEW PERSPECTIVES ON BULLYING PREVENTION: WHY ARE CURRENT PROGRAMS NOT WORKING? NEW PERSPECTIVES ON BULLYING PREVENTION: WHY ARE CURRENT PROGRAMS NOT WORKING? Dorothy L. Espelage, Ph.D. Professor, Child Development Division; Educational Psychology espelage@illinois.edu This research

More information

Bullying. Take Action Against. stealing money. switching seats in the classroom. spreading rumors. pushing & tripping

Bullying. Take Action Against. stealing money. switching seats in the classroom. spreading rumors. pushing & tripping switching seats in the classroom stealing money Take Action Against Bullying spreading rumors pushing & tripping U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH & HUMAN SERVICES Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration

More information

Multilevel Models for Social Network Analysis

Multilevel Models for Social Network Analysis Multilevel Models for Social Network Analysis Paul-Philippe Pare ppare@uwo.ca Department of Sociology Centre for Population, Aging, and Health University of Western Ontario Pamela Wilcox & Matthew Logan

More information

Preventing Bullying and Harassment of Targeted Group Students. COSA August 2013 John Lenssen

Preventing Bullying and Harassment of Targeted Group Students. COSA August 2013 John Lenssen Preventing Bullying and Harassment of Targeted Group Students COSA August 2013 John Lenssen Definition Bullying is unfair and one-sided. It happens when someone keeps hurting, frightening, threatening,

More information

Predicting Successful Completion of the Nursing Program: An Analysis of Prerequisites and Demographic Variables

Predicting Successful Completion of the Nursing Program: An Analysis of Prerequisites and Demographic Variables Predicting Successful Completion of the Nursing Program: An Analysis of Prerequisites and Demographic Variables Introduction In the summer of 2002, a research study commissioned by the Center for Student

More information

Basic Concepts in Research and Data Analysis

Basic Concepts in Research and Data Analysis Basic Concepts in Research and Data Analysis Introduction: A Common Language for Researchers...2 Steps to Follow When Conducting Research...3 The Research Question... 3 The Hypothesis... 4 Defining the

More information

Math Placement Acceleration Initiative at the City College of San Francisco Developed with San Francisco Unified School District

Math Placement Acceleration Initiative at the City College of San Francisco Developed with San Francisco Unified School District Youth Data Archive Issue Brief October 2012 Math Placement Acceleration Initiative at the City College of San Francisco Developed with San Francisco Unified School District Betsy Williams Background This

More information

Longitudinal Research on Game Effects and Implications for Public Policy Munich, Germany, November 20, 2008

Longitudinal Research on Game Effects and Implications for Public Policy Munich, Germany, November 20, 2008 Douglas A. Gentile, Ph.D. Director, Media Research Lab, Iowa State University Director of Research, National Institute on Media and the Family Research Fellow, Institute on Science and Society Associate

More information

Facts for Teens: Youth Violence

Facts for Teens: Youth Violence P.O. Box 6003 Rockville, MD 20849-6003 nyvprc@safeyouth.org www.safeyouth.org Facts for Teens: Youth Violence Introduction Many teenagers are concerned about youth violence, and with good reason. Each

More information

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender,

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender, This essay critiques the theoretical perspectives, research design and analysis, and interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender, Pair Composition and Computer

More information

Strategies for Promoting Gatekeeper Course Success Among Students Needing Remediation: Research Report for the Virginia Community College System

Strategies for Promoting Gatekeeper Course Success Among Students Needing Remediation: Research Report for the Virginia Community College System Strategies for Promoting Gatekeeper Course Success Among Students Needing Remediation: Research Report for the Virginia Community College System Josipa Roksa Davis Jenkins Shanna Smith Jaggars Matthew

More information

The Relationship between Ethnicity and Academic Success in Online Education Courses

The Relationship between Ethnicity and Academic Success in Online Education Courses The Relationship between Ethnicity and Academic Success in Online Education Courses Lori Kupczynski Texas A&M University-Kingsville Department of Educational Leadership and Counseling 700 University Blvd.,

More information

EFFECTIVENESS OF TREATMENT FOR VIOLENT JUVENILE DELINQUENTS

EFFECTIVENESS OF TREATMENT FOR VIOLENT JUVENILE DELINQUENTS EFFECTIVENESS OF TREATMENT FOR VIOLENT JUVENILE DELINQUENTS THE PROBLEM Traditionally, the philosophy of juvenile courts has emphasized treatment and rehabilitation of young offenders. In recent years,

More information

Assessing Bullying in New Jersey Secondary Schools

Assessing Bullying in New Jersey Secondary Schools 28 National Conference on the Social Norms Approach, Hyatt Regency San Francisco Airport, CA, July 22, 28 Assessing Bullying in New Jersey Secondary Schools Applying the Social Norms Model to Adolescent

More information

How To Study The Academic Performance Of An Mba

How To Study The Academic Performance Of An Mba Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Statistical Association, August 5-9, 2001 WORK EXPERIENCE: DETERMINANT OF MBA ACADEMIC SUCCESS? Andrew Braunstein, Iona College Hagan School of Business,

More information

Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011

Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011 Drug Abuse Prevention Training FTS 2011 Principles of Prevention Prevention programs should enhance protective factors and reverse or reduce risk factors (Hawkins et al. 2002). The risk of becoming a drug

More information

Rehabilitation programs for young offenders: Towards good practice? Andrew Day. Forensic Psychology Research Group. University of South Australia

Rehabilitation programs for young offenders: Towards good practice? Andrew Day. Forensic Psychology Research Group. University of South Australia 1 Rehabilitation programs for young offenders: Towards good practice? Andrew Day Forensic Psychology Research Group University of South Australia Andrew.day@unisa.edu.au Invited paper for the Understanding

More information

Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88)

Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88) Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88) Introduction The National Educational Longitudinal Survey (NELS:88) followed students from 8 th grade in 1988 to 10 th grade in

More information

Running head: CONFORMITY: DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE 1. Conformity: Drug and Alcohol Abuse. within Adolescent Communities. Amber R.

Running head: CONFORMITY: DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE 1. Conformity: Drug and Alcohol Abuse. within Adolescent Communities. Amber R. Running head: CONFORMITY: DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE 1 Conformity: Drug and Alcohol Abuse within Adolescent Communities Amber R. Tolley Lincoln Memorial University CONFORMITY: DRUG AND ALCOHOL ABUSE 2 Conformity:

More information

Fairfield Public Schools

Fairfield Public Schools Mathematics Fairfield Public Schools AP Statistics AP Statistics BOE Approved 04/08/2014 1 AP STATISTICS Critical Areas of Focus AP Statistics is a rigorous course that offers advanced students an opportunity

More information

Teacher Management of Classroom Social Dynamics in Elementary School Opportunities for Professional Development & Intervention

Teacher Management of Classroom Social Dynamics in Elementary School Opportunities for Professional Development & Intervention Teacher Management of Classroom Social Dynamics in Elementary School Opportunities for Professional Development & Intervention Scott D. Gest Human Development & Family Studies Prevention Research Center

More information

Determining Future Success of College Students

Determining Future Success of College Students Determining Future Success of College Students PAUL OEHRLEIN I. Introduction The years that students spend in college are perhaps the most influential years on the rest of their lives. College students

More information

Gender Effects in the Alaska Juvenile Justice System

Gender Effects in the Alaska Juvenile Justice System Gender Effects in the Alaska Juvenile Justice System Report to the Justice and Statistics Research Association by André Rosay Justice Center University of Alaska Anchorage JC 0306.05 October 2003 Gender

More information

The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools Early Report Newsletter Spring 2002

The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools Early Report Newsletter Spring 2002 The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools Early Report Newsletter Spring 2002 Introduction Anthony Pellegrini, University of Minnesota; Peter Blatchford, University of London

More information

Whitney Colbert Research Methods for the Social Sciences Trinity College Spring 2012

Whitney Colbert Research Methods for the Social Sciences Trinity College Spring 2012 ALCOHOL IN COLLEGE ATHLETICS: THE FIGHT TO RAISE AWARENESS OF BINGE DRINKING ON COLLEGE ATHLETIC TEAMS Whitney Colbert Research Methods for the Social Sciences Trinity College Spring 2012 While there is

More information

Psychological Warfare and Terrorism

Psychological Warfare and Terrorism Psychological Warfare and Terrorism Robert D. Hanser This brief article demonstrates that there are common psychological principles that can be effectively and directly applied to illustrate the utility

More information

Co-Curricular Activities and Academic Performance -A Study of the Student Leadership Initiative Programs. Office of Institutional Research

Co-Curricular Activities and Academic Performance -A Study of the Student Leadership Initiative Programs. Office of Institutional Research Co-Curricular Activities and Academic Performance -A Study of the Student Leadership Initiative Programs Office of Institutional Research July 2014 Introduction The Leadership Initiative (LI) is a certificate

More information

Association Between Variables

Association Between Variables Contents 11 Association Between Variables 767 11.1 Introduction............................ 767 11.1.1 Measure of Association................. 768 11.1.2 Chapter Summary.................... 769 11.2 Chi

More information

THE IMPACT OF CHILDHOOD HEALTH AND COGNITION ON PORTFOLIO CHOICE

THE IMPACT OF CHILDHOOD HEALTH AND COGNITION ON PORTFOLIO CHOICE THE IMPACT OF CHILDHOOD HEALTH AND COGNITION ON PORTFOLIO CHOICE Dimitris Christelis, Loretti Dobrescu, Alberto Motta 214-2010 5 The Impact of Childhood Health and Cognition on Portfolio Choice Dimitris

More information

EDUCATION AND LAW ENFORCEMENT 1. Education and Law Enforcement: Final Version of Research Proposal. Ralphie Manzano

EDUCATION AND LAW ENFORCEMENT 1. Education and Law Enforcement: Final Version of Research Proposal. Ralphie Manzano EDUCATION AND LAW ENFORCEMENT 1 Education and Law Enforcement: Final Version of Research Proposal Ralphie Manzano University of South Florida, Sarasota-Manatee EDUCATION AND LAW ENFORCEMENT 2 Abstract

More information

9. Sampling Distributions

9. Sampling Distributions 9. Sampling Distributions Prerequisites none A. Introduction B. Sampling Distribution of the Mean C. Sampling Distribution of Difference Between Means D. Sampling Distribution of Pearson's r E. Sampling

More information

Rethinking the Cultural Context of Schooling Decisions in Disadvantaged Neighborhoods: From Deviant Subculture to Cultural Heterogeneity

Rethinking the Cultural Context of Schooling Decisions in Disadvantaged Neighborhoods: From Deviant Subculture to Cultural Heterogeneity Rethinking the Cultural Context of Schooling Decisions in Disadvantaged Neighborhoods: From Deviant Subculture to Cultural Heterogeneity Sociology of Education David J. Harding, University of Michigan

More information

Why Taking This Course? Course Introduction, Descriptive Statistics and Data Visualization. Learning Goals. GENOME 560, Spring 2012

Why Taking This Course? Course Introduction, Descriptive Statistics and Data Visualization. Learning Goals. GENOME 560, Spring 2012 Why Taking This Course? Course Introduction, Descriptive Statistics and Data Visualization GENOME 560, Spring 2012 Data are interesting because they help us understand the world Genomics: Massive Amounts

More information

The NCAA Basketball Betting Market: Tests of the Balanced Book and Levitt Hypotheses

The NCAA Basketball Betting Market: Tests of the Balanced Book and Levitt Hypotheses The NCAA Basketball Betting Market: Tests of the Balanced Book and Levitt Hypotheses Rodney J. Paul, St. Bonaventure University Andrew P. Weinbach, Coastal Carolina University Kristin K. Paul, St. Bonaventure

More information

Contrast Coding in Multiple Regression Analysis: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Utility of Popular Coding Structures

Contrast Coding in Multiple Regression Analysis: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Utility of Popular Coding Structures Journal of Data Science 8(2010), 61-73 Contrast Coding in Multiple Regression Analysis: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Utility of Popular Coding Structures Matthew J. Davis Texas A&M University Abstract: The

More information

Local outlier detection in data forensics: data mining approach to flag unusual schools

Local outlier detection in data forensics: data mining approach to flag unusual schools Local outlier detection in data forensics: data mining approach to flag unusual schools Mayuko Simon Data Recognition Corporation Paper presented at the 2012 Conference on Statistical Detection of Potential

More information

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive Statistics Descriptive Statistics Primer Descriptive statistics Central tendency Variation Relative position Relationships Calculating descriptive statistics Descriptive Statistics Purpose to describe or summarize

More information

BULLY PREVENTION: ARE YOU PROMOTING HEALTHY RELATIONSHIPS IN YOUR CLASSROOMS AND SCHOOLS?

BULLY PREVENTION: ARE YOU PROMOTING HEALTHY RELATIONSHIPS IN YOUR CLASSROOMS AND SCHOOLS? BULLY PREVENTION: ARE YOU PROMOTING HEALTHY RELATIONSHIPS IN YOUR CLASSROOMS AND SCHOOLS? Dorothy L. Espelage, Ph.D. Professor, Child Development Division; Educational Psychology espelage@illinois.edu

More information

Jim Kjelland. Jim Kjelland Economic Returns to Higher Education: Signaling v. Human Capital Theory An Analysis of Competing Theories

Jim Kjelland. Jim Kjelland Economic Returns to Higher Education: Signaling v. Human Capital Theory An Analysis of Competing Theories Economic Returns to Higher Education: Signaling v. Human Capital Theory An Analysis of Competing Theories I. Introduction A decision to pursue higher education involves an informal analysis on part of

More information

The Local Educational and Regional Economic Foundations of Violence: A Spatial Analysis of Homicide across Mexico s Municipalities

The Local Educational and Regional Economic Foundations of Violence: A Spatial Analysis of Homicide across Mexico s Municipalities The Local Educational and Regional Economic Foundations of Violence: A Spatial Analysis of Homicide across Mexico s Municipalities January 16, 2014 Wilson Center Matthew C. Ingram University at Albany,

More information

The Effects of Focused Area of Study on Self-Reported Skill Gains and Satisfaction of MBA Graduates

The Effects of Focused Area of Study on Self-Reported Skill Gains and Satisfaction of MBA Graduates The Effects of Focused Area of Study on Self-Reported Skill Gains and Satisfaction of MBA Graduates Andrew J. Hussey, University of Memphis, USA ABSTRACT Using a longitudinal survey of registrants for

More information

Georgia Performance Standards. Health Education

Georgia Performance Standards. Health Education HIGH SCHOOL Students in high school demonstrate comprehensive health knowledge and skills. Their behaviors reflect a conceptual understanding of the issues associated with maintaining good personal health.

More information

Conduct Disorder: Treatment Recommendations. For Vermont Youth. From the. State Interagency Team

Conduct Disorder: Treatment Recommendations. For Vermont Youth. From the. State Interagency Team Conduct Disorder: Treatment Recommendations For Vermont Youth From the State Interagency Team By Bill McMains, Medical Director, Vermont DDMHS Alice Maynard, Mental Health Quality Management Chief, Vermont

More information

ANNEX 2: Assessment of the 7 points agreed by WATCH as meriting attention (cover paper, paragraph 9, bullet points) by Andy Darnton, HSE

ANNEX 2: Assessment of the 7 points agreed by WATCH as meriting attention (cover paper, paragraph 9, bullet points) by Andy Darnton, HSE ANNEX 2: Assessment of the 7 points agreed by WATCH as meriting attention (cover paper, paragraph 9, bullet points) by Andy Darnton, HSE The 7 issues to be addressed outlined in paragraph 9 of the cover

More information

THE EFFECT OF CRIME VICTIMIZATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARDS CRIMINAL JUSTICE IN LATIN AMERICA

THE EFFECT OF CRIME VICTIMIZATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARDS CRIMINAL JUSTICE IN LATIN AMERICA THE EFFECT OF CRIME VICTIMIZATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARDS CRIMINAL JUSTICE IN LATIN AMERICA Gabriel Demombynes Draft of May 2009 ABSTRACT This paper examines the effects victimization on criminal justice

More information

Juvenile Justice. CJ 3650 Professor James J. Drylie Chapter 3

Juvenile Justice. CJ 3650 Professor James J. Drylie Chapter 3 Juvenile Justice CJ 3650 Professor James J. Drylie Chapter 3 Measuring Juvenile Crime Fears related to juvenile crime reached new heights in the past two decades Fear remains high despite falling juvenile

More information

arxiv:1606.02283v1 [physics.soc-ph] 7 Jun 2016

arxiv:1606.02283v1 [physics.soc-ph] 7 Jun 2016 The Formation and Imprinting of Network Effects Among the Business Elite arxiv:1606.02283v1 [physics.soc-ph] 7 Jun 2016 Brian Uzzi 1,2, Yang Yang 1, and Kevin Gaughan 2 1 Northwestern Institute on Complex

More information

Bullying. Introduction

Bullying. Introduction Bullying 1 Introduction In recent years, bullying has become a topic of greater public concern. Research has shown the damaging long-term effects that bullying behavior can have on its victims. Approximately

More information

Running head: BODY ART AND ACADEMIC SUCCESS 1

Running head: BODY ART AND ACADEMIC SUCCESS 1 Running head: BODY ART AND ACADEMIC SUCCESS 1 The Psychology of Body Art and Academic Success Katherine Janney McKendree University Author note: This paper was created as a partial requirement for PSY

More information

AFTER HIGH SCHOOL: A FIRST LOOK AT THE POSTSCHOOL EXPERIENCES OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES

AFTER HIGH SCHOOL: A FIRST LOOK AT THE POSTSCHOOL EXPERIENCES OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES April 2005 AFTER HIGH SCHOOL: A FIRST LOOK AT THE POSTSCHOOL EXPERIENCES OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES A Report from the National Longitudinal Transition Study-2 (NLTS2) Executive Summary Prepared for: Office

More information

CSV886: Social, Economics and Business Networks. Lecture 2: Affiliation and Balance. R Ravi ravi+iitd@andrew.cmu.edu

CSV886: Social, Economics and Business Networks. Lecture 2: Affiliation and Balance. R Ravi ravi+iitd@andrew.cmu.edu CSV886: Social, Economics and Business Networks Lecture 2: Affiliation and Balance R Ravi ravi+iitd@andrew.cmu.edu Granovetter s Puzzle Resolved Strong Triadic Closure holds in most nodes in social networks

More information

Public Housing and Public Schools: How Do Students Living in NYC Public Housing Fare in School?

Public Housing and Public Schools: How Do Students Living in NYC Public Housing Fare in School? Furman Center for real estate & urban policy New York University school of law wagner school of public service november 2008 Policy Brief Public Housing and Public Schools: How Do Students Living in NYC

More information

Jenny Walker, Ph.D. Askdoctorwalker@cox.net www.cyberbullyingnews.com

Jenny Walker, Ph.D. Askdoctorwalker@cox.net www.cyberbullyingnews.com Jenny Walker, Ph.D. Askdoctorwalker@cox.net www.cyberbullyingnews.com 1 Overview Introduction: Why we are here the challenge Part 1: What do we need to know? the basics What is bullying and cyberbullying?

More information

Brief Strategic Family Therapy for Adolescent Drug Abuse

Brief Strategic Family Therapy for Adolescent Drug Abuse Brief Strategic Family Therapy for Adolescent Drug Abuse Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. Chapter One: Brief Strategic Family Therapy-An

More information

A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents in the Klang Valley, Malaysia

A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents in the Klang Valley, Malaysia 2011 International Conference on Humanities, Society and Culture IPEDR Vol.20 (2011) (2011) IACSIT Press, Singapore A Study of the Relationship between Violent Video Game Playing and Aggression among Adolescents

More information

UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL

UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL OF PUBLIC HEALTH Master Programme in Public Health - Programme and Courses Academic year 2015-2016 Public Health and Clinical Medicine Umeå International School of Public Health

More information

Chapter 5, Learning to Think

Chapter 5, Learning to Think From Derek Bok, Our Underachieving Colleges: A Candid Look At How Much Students Learn and Why They Should Be Learning More, Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press, 2006. Chapter 5, Learning

More information

EXCHANGE. J. Luke Wood. Administration, Rehabilitation & Postsecondary Education, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA

EXCHANGE. J. Luke Wood. Administration, Rehabilitation & Postsecondary Education, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 37: 333 338, 2013 Copyright# Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1066-8926 print=1521-0413 online DOI: 10.1080/10668926.2012.754733 EXCHANGE The Community

More information

Basic Statistics and Data Analysis for Health Researchers from Foreign Countries

Basic Statistics and Data Analysis for Health Researchers from Foreign Countries Basic Statistics and Data Analysis for Health Researchers from Foreign Countries Volkert Siersma siersma@sund.ku.dk The Research Unit for General Practice in Copenhagen Dias 1 Content Quantifying association

More information

Life Stressors and Non-Cognitive Outcomes in Community Colleges for Mexican/Mexican American Men. Art Guaracha Jr. San Diego State University

Life Stressors and Non-Cognitive Outcomes in Community Colleges for Mexican/Mexican American Men. Art Guaracha Jr. San Diego State University Life Stressors and Non-Cognitive Outcomes in Community Colleges for Mexican/Mexican American Men Art Guaracha Jr. San Diego State University JP 3 Journal of Progressive Policy & Practice Volume 2 Issue

More information

Bullying in U.S. Schools

Bullying in U.S. Schools Bullying in U.S. Schools 2014 STATUS REPORT Assessed using data collected from the Olweus Bullying Questionnaire Harlan Luxenberg, MA, Professional Data Analysts, Inc. Susan P. Limber, PhD, Clemson University

More information

Test Bias. As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but

Test Bias. As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but Test Bias As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but none are perfect. The reliability of test scores can be compromised by random measurement error (unsystematic

More information

Charles Williams Church in Wales Primary School. Bullying Prevention Policy. June 2014 Review date June 2016. A Definition Of Bullying

Charles Williams Church in Wales Primary School. Bullying Prevention Policy. June 2014 Review date June 2016. A Definition Of Bullying Charles Williams Church in Wales Primary School Bullying Prevention Policy June 2014 Review date June 2016 This Bullying Prevention Policy acknowledges the Welsh Government s Respecting Others: Anti- Bullying

More information

The relationship among alcohol use, related problems, and symptoms of psychological distress: Gender as a moderator in a college sample

The relationship among alcohol use, related problems, and symptoms of psychological distress: Gender as a moderator in a college sample Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 843 848 The relationship among alcohol use, related problems, and symptoms of psychological distress: Gender as a moderator in a college sample Irene Markman Geisner*, Mary

More information

When to Use a Particular Statistical Test

When to Use a Particular Statistical Test When to Use a Particular Statistical Test Central Tendency Univariate Descriptive Mode the most commonly occurring value 6 people with ages 21, 22, 21, 23, 19, 21 - mode = 21 Median the center value the

More information

Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016. 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu

Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016. 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu Behavioral Sciences CREDENTIAL TITLE PROGRAM OPTIONS CREDIT HOURS REQUIRED Certificate Behavioral

More information

Criminal Justice (CRJU) Course Descriptions

Criminal Justice (CRJU) Course Descriptions Criminal Justice (CRJU) Course Descriptions REQUIRED COURSES CRJU 1000 CRIMINAL JUSTICE: AN OVERVIEW This course is designed to provide an overview of the criminal justice process and the criminal justice

More information

Assumptions. Assumptions of linear models. Boxplot. Data exploration. Apply to response variable. Apply to error terms from linear model

Assumptions. Assumptions of linear models. Boxplot. Data exploration. Apply to response variable. Apply to error terms from linear model Assumptions Assumptions of linear models Apply to response variable within each group if predictor categorical Apply to error terms from linear model check by analysing residuals Normality Homogeneity

More information

The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment. Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households

The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment. Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households Executive Summary EXECUTIVE SUMMARY UNICEF/Schermbrucker Cover photograph: UNICEF/Pirozzi

More information

Course Descriptions. Seminar in Organizational Behavior II

Course Descriptions. Seminar in Organizational Behavior II Course Descriptions B55 MKT 670 Seminar in Marketing Management This course is an advanced seminar of doctoral level standing. The course is aimed at students pursuing a degree in business, economics or

More information

The Role of Active Learning in College Student Persistence

The Role of Active Learning in College Student Persistence 5 The primary goal of this study was to examine the impact of active learning on a student s level of overall social integration and perception of his or her institution s commitment to student welfare.

More information

VIDEO SCRIPT. Bullying, Harassment, & Civil Rights: An Overview of School Districts Federal Obligation to Respond to Harassment

VIDEO SCRIPT. Bullying, Harassment, & Civil Rights: An Overview of School Districts Federal Obligation to Respond to Harassment SCRIPT TITLE: PREPARED BY: LEGEND: Bullying, Harassment, & Civil Rights: An Overview of School Districts Federal Obligation to Respond to Harassment National Center on Safe Supportive Learning Environments,

More information

THE EDUCATIONAL PROGRESS OF BLACK STUDENTS

THE EDUCATIONAL PROGRESS OF BLACK STUDENTS NATIONAL CENTER FOR EDUCATION STATISTICS Findings from THE CONDITION OF EDUCATION 1994 NO. 2 THE EDUCATIONAL PROGRESS OF BLACK STUDENTS U.S. Department of Education Office of Educational Research and Improvement

More information

Volume 30, Issue 4. Market Efficiency and the NHL totals betting market: Is there an under bias?

Volume 30, Issue 4. Market Efficiency and the NHL totals betting market: Is there an under bias? Volume 30, Issue 4 Market Efficiency and the NHL totals betting market: Is there an under bias? Bill M Woodland Economics Department, Eastern Michigan University Linda M Woodland College of Business, Eastern

More information

MONTE VISTA CHRISTIAN SCHOOL SOSC 3422, Sociology Course Syllabus

MONTE VISTA CHRISTIAN SCHOOL SOSC 3422, Sociology Course Syllabus MONTE VISTA CHRISTIAN SCHOOL SOSC 3422, Sociology Course Syllabus Course Description : An elective social science course designed to challenge and prepare for the rigors of collegiate sociological study.

More information

Bridging Micro and Macro Domains: Workforce Differentiation and Strategic Human Resource Management

Bridging Micro and Macro Domains: Workforce Differentiation and Strategic Human Resource Management Special Issue: Bridging Micro and Macro Domains Journal of Management Vol. 37 No. 2, March 2011 421-428 DOI: 10.1177/0149206310373400 The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: http://www. sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav

More information

In the past two decades, the federal government has dramatically

In the past two decades, the federal government has dramatically Degree Attainment of Undergraduate Student Borrowers in Four-Year Institutions: A Multilevel Analysis By Dai Li Dai Li is a doctoral candidate in the Center for the Study of Higher Education at Pennsylvania

More information

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING *Zahra Gholami Ghareh Shiran 1, Ghodsi Ahghar 2, Afshin Ahramiyan 3, Afsaneh Boostan

More information

A Review of Conduct Disorder. William U Borst. Troy State University at Phenix City

A Review of Conduct Disorder. William U Borst. Troy State University at Phenix City A Review of 1 Running head: A REVIEW OF CONDUCT DISORDER A Review of Conduct Disorder William U Borst Troy State University at Phenix City A Review of 2 Abstract Conduct disorders are a complicated set

More information

5. EMPLOYMENT OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES AFTER HIGH SCHOOL

5. EMPLOYMENT OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES AFTER HIGH SCHOOL 5. EMPLOYMENT OF YOUTH WITH DISABILITIES AFTER HIGH SCHOOL By Renée Cameto Employment is the norm in American society, and it often begins at an early age. Approximately half of 12- and 13-year-olds in

More information

Social Security Eligibility and the Labor Supply of Elderly Immigrants. George J. Borjas Harvard University and National Bureau of Economic Research

Social Security Eligibility and the Labor Supply of Elderly Immigrants. George J. Borjas Harvard University and National Bureau of Economic Research Social Security Eligibility and the Labor Supply of Elderly Immigrants George J. Borjas Harvard University and National Bureau of Economic Research Updated for the 9th Annual Joint Conference of the Retirement

More information

A Sociologist s Perspective on Domestic Violence: A Conversation with Michael Johnson, Ph.D. Interview by Theodora Ooms, CLASP

A Sociologist s Perspective on Domestic Violence: A Conversation with Michael Johnson, Ph.D. Interview by Theodora Ooms, CLASP A Sociologist s Perspective on Domestic Violence: A Conversation with Michael Johnson, Ph.D. Interview by Theodora Ooms, CLASP From the May 2006 conference sponsored by CLASP and NCSL: Building Bridges:

More information

Algebra 1 Course Information

Algebra 1 Course Information Course Information Course Description: Students will study patterns, relations, and functions, and focus on the use of mathematical models to understand and analyze quantitative relationships. Through

More information

Interpreting Data in Normal Distributions

Interpreting Data in Normal Distributions Interpreting Data in Normal Distributions This curve is kind of a big deal. It shows the distribution of a set of test scores, the results of rolling a die a million times, the heights of people on Earth,

More information

RUNNING HEAD: FAFSA lists 1

RUNNING HEAD: FAFSA lists 1 RUNNING HEAD: FAFSA lists 1 Strategic use of FAFSA list information by colleges Stephen R. Porter Department of Leadership, Policy, and Adult and Higher Education North Carolina State University Raleigh,

More information

American Sociological Association Guidelines. CEOE Competencies. 0017 Understand methods of data collection and analysis in sociology.

American Sociological Association Guidelines. CEOE Competencies. 0017 Understand methods of data collection and analysis in sociology. Certification Examinations for Oklahoma Educators (CEOE) Framework Development Correlation Table The Framework Development Correlation Table provides information about possible alignment of some of the

More information

THE ROLE OF SOCIOGRAMS IN SOCIAL NETWORK ANALYSIS. Maryann Durland Ph.D. EERS Conference 2012 Monday April 20, 10:30-12:00

THE ROLE OF SOCIOGRAMS IN SOCIAL NETWORK ANALYSIS. Maryann Durland Ph.D. EERS Conference 2012 Monday April 20, 10:30-12:00 THE ROLE OF SOCIOGRAMS IN SOCIAL NETWORK ANALYSIS Maryann Durland Ph.D. EERS Conference 2012 Monday April 20, 10:30-12:00 FORMAT OF PRESENTATION Part I SNA overview 10 minutes Part II Sociograms Example

More information

2.1 Net enrolment ratio in primary education

2.1 Net enrolment ratio in primary education 2.1 Net enrolment ratio in primary education GOAL AND TARGET ADDRESSED Goal 2: Achieve universal primary education Target 2.A: Ensure that, by 2015, children everywhere, boys and girls alike, will be able

More information

The Impact of Social Connections on Credit Scoring

The Impact of Social Connections on Credit Scoring The Impact of Social Connections on Credit Scoring Yanhao Wei Pinar Yildirim Christophe Van den Bulte Chris Dellarocas April 2014 Abstract Motivated by the growing popularity of social data use in credit

More information

UNDERSTANDING THE INDEPENDENT-SAMPLES t TEST

UNDERSTANDING THE INDEPENDENT-SAMPLES t TEST UNDERSTANDING The independent-samples t test evaluates the difference between the means of two independent or unrelated groups. That is, we evaluate whether the means for two independent groups are significantly

More information

Executive Summary and Recommendations

Executive Summary and Recommendations Executive Summary and Recommendations To download a free copy of the complete report, go to www.aauw.org/learn/research/whysofew.cfm. Executive Summary Women have made tremendous progress in education

More information

Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice University of South Carolina

Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice University of South Carolina Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice University of South Carolina Doctoral Program Theory & Methods Past Comprehensive Examinations Sample Instructions, Spring 2012 The Theory/Methods Comprehensive

More information

Urban vs. Rural: Summer. A Look at Indiana State Students from Urban and Rural Areas in Illinois using MAP Works Results

Urban vs. Rural: Summer. A Look at Indiana State Students from Urban and Rural Areas in Illinois using MAP Works Results Summer 2011 Urban vs. Rural: A Look at Indiana State Students from Urban and Rural Areas in Illinois using MAP Works Results Prepared by Christopher Childs Indiana State University During the 2010 2011

More information

Relational Aggression and Subjective Well- Being in Independent Senior Living Communities

Relational Aggression and Subjective Well- Being in Independent Senior Living Communities Relational Aggression and Subjective Well- Being in Independent Senior Living Communities By Jacquelyn Benson, MA, Senior Research Associate Mather LifeWays Institute on Aging both the quantity and quality

More information

International Statistical Institute, 56th Session, 2007: Phil Everson

International Statistical Institute, 56th Session, 2007: Phil Everson Teaching Regression using American Football Scores Everson, Phil Swarthmore College Department of Mathematics and Statistics 5 College Avenue Swarthmore, PA198, USA E-mail: peverso1@swarthmore.edu 1. Introduction

More information

Technical Report. Teach for America Teachers Contribution to Student Achievement in Louisiana in Grades 4-9: 2004-2005 to 2006-2007

Technical Report. Teach for America Teachers Contribution to Student Achievement in Louisiana in Grades 4-9: 2004-2005 to 2006-2007 Page 1 of 16 Technical Report Teach for America Teachers Contribution to Student Achievement in Louisiana in Grades 4-9: 2004-2005 to 2006-2007 George H. Noell, Ph.D. Department of Psychology Louisiana

More information

Suggested APA style reference:

Suggested APA style reference: Suggested APA style reference: Preventing school shootings: A summary of a U.S. Secret Service Safe School Initiative report. (2002, March). National Institute of Justice Journal, 248, 11-15. Available

More information

Student Answer: Student Answer:

Student Answer: Student Answer: 1. Question : The degree to which members of society feel united by shared values and other social bonds is known as: sociology. sociological imagination. social integration. social cohesion. c and d.

More information