The role of intonation for the perception of sentence types in Greek Anthi Chaida 1, Marios Fourakis 2, Sofia Loui 1. University of Athens, Greece

Similar documents
SWING: A tool for modelling intonational varieties of Swedish Beskow, Jonas; Bruce, Gösta; Enflo, Laura; Granström, Björn; Schötz, Susanne

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

Prosodic focus marking in Bai

Intonation difficulties in non-native languages.

African American English-Speaking Children's Comprehension of Past Tense: Evidence from a Grammaticality Judgment Task Abstract

THE VOICE OF LOVE. Trisha Belanger, Caroline Menezes, Claire Barboa, Mofida Helo, Kimia Shirazifard

Phonetic and phonological properties of the final pitch accent in Catalan declaratives

stress, intonation and pauses and pronounce English sounds correctly. (b) To speak accurately to the listener(s) about one s thoughts and feelings,

Teaching and Learning Mandarin Tones. 19 th May 2012 Rob Neal

Aspects of North Swedish intonational phonology. Bruce, Gösta

Text-To-Speech Technologies for Mobile Telephony Services

Prosodic Characteristics of Emotional Speech: Measurements of Fundamental Frequency Movements

The Role of Listening in Language Acquisition; the Challenges & Strategies in Teaching Listening

Julia Hirschberg. AT&T Bell Laboratories. Murray Hill, New Jersey 07974

Intonation and information structure (part II)

Phonetic Perception and Pronunciation Difficulties of Russian Language (From a Canadian Perspective) Alyssa Marren

How the Computer Translates. Svetlana Sokolova President and CEO of PROMT, PhD.

The prosodic structure of quantificational sentences in Greek

Things to remember when transcribing speech

The role of prosody in toddlers interpretation of verbs argument structure

Prosodic Phrasing: Machine and Human Evaluation

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

SPEECH OR LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION

Social Media Study in European Police Forces: First Results on Usage and Acceptance

Language Meaning and Use

. Niparko, J. K. (2006). Speech Recognition at 1-Year Follow-Up in the Childhood

1 Introduction. Finding Referents in Time: Eye-Tracking Evidence for the Role of Contrastive Accents

Functional Auditory Performance Indicators (FAPI)

An Analysis of the Eleventh Grade Students Monitor Use in Speaking Performance based on Krashen s (1982) Monitor Hypothesis at SMAN 4 Jember

The use of focus markers. in second language word processing

Hyunah Ahn

209 THE STRUCTURE AND USE OF ENGLISH.

Master of Arts in Linguistics Syllabus

The effect of mismatched recording conditions on human and automatic speaker recognition in forensic applications

Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language

Cambridge English: Advanced Speaking Sample test with examiner s comments

Measuring and synthesising expressivity: Some tools to analyse and simulate phonostyle

ELPS TELPAS. Proficiency Level Descriptors

Pronunciation in English

Modelling compound intonation in Dala and Gotland Swedish Schötz, Susanne; Bruce, Gösta; Granström, Björn

?Infant Learning Lab Newsletter>

Degree of highness or lowness of the voice caused by variation in the rate of vibration of the vocal cords.

PTE Academic Preparation Course Outline

Speaking skills for Cambridge English: First for Schools (2015)

WinPitch LTL II, a Multimodal Pronunciation Software

A Computer Program for Pronunciation Training and Assessment in Japanese Language Classrooms Experimental Use of Pronunciation Check

An Arabic Text-To-Speech System Based on Artificial Neural Networks

The use of listening learning strategies by Lengua Inglesa students in five Mexican universities: preliminary results

Formant Bandwidth and Resilience of Speech to Noise

Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) Certificate Programs

Thirukkural - A Text-to-Speech Synthesis System

Characteristics of reading and understanding of hearing impaired students in classes VI-VIII

INVESTIGATING DISCOURSE MARKERS IN PEDAGOGICAL SETTINGS:

The Effects of Speaking Rate on Mandarin Tones. C2008 Yun H. Stockton

Mother Tongue Influence on Spoken English

The Minor Third Communicates Sadness in Speech, Mirroring Its Use in Music

Running head: MODALS IN ENGLISH LEARNERS WRITING 1. Epistemic and Root Modals in Chinese Students English Argumentative Writings.

Modern foreign languages

GCSE Speaking Support Meetings. GCSE Polish Speaking. Introduction 2. Guidance 3. Assessment Criteria 4-5. Student 1 - Speaking Commentary 6-7

4 Pitch and range in language and music

Susan Geffen, Ph.D. Postdoctoral Fellow

Regionalized Text-to-Speech Systems: Persona Design and Application Scenarios

French Language and Culture. Curriculum Framework

PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR

Convention Paper Presented at the 118th Convention 2005 May Barcelona, Spain

Carla Simões, Speech Analysis and Transcription Software

The Effect of Long-Term Use of Drugs on Speaker s Fundamental Frequency

Historical Linguistics. Diachronic Analysis. Two Approaches to the Study of Language. Kinds of Language Change. What is Historical Linguistics?

Applying Repair Processing in Chinese Homophone Disambiguation

CURRICULUM VITAE. Jessica Sari Fleming Hay. Department of Psychology University of Tennessee, Knoxville 1404 Circle Drive Knoxville, TN 37996

TEACHER CERTIFICATION STUDY GUIDE LANGUAGE COMPETENCY AND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION

Pronunciation: stress and intonation

An Overview of Applied Linguistics

M.A. Hispanic Linguistics. University of Arizona: Tucson, AZ.

Department of Modern Languages

Emotion Detection from Speech

Mixed-effects regression and eye-tracking data

Research Portfolio. Beáta B. Megyesi January 8, 2007

Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution: Effects of Prosodic Breaks and Prosodic Length

CHARTES D'ANGLAIS SOMMAIRE. CHARTE NIVEAU A1 Pages 2-4. CHARTE NIVEAU A2 Pages 5-7. CHARTE NIVEAU B1 Pages CHARTE NIVEAU B2 Pages 11-14

An Evaluation of Bone-conducted Ultrasonic Hearing-aid regarding Transmission of Japanese Prosodic Phonemes

Socio-Cultural Knowledge in Conversational Inference

Electrophysiological studies of prosody

Office Phone/ / lix@cwu.edu Office Hours: MW 3:50-4:50, TR 12:00-12:30

UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA CRUZ. True to Form: Rising and Falling Declaratives as Questions in English

Congenitally Deaf Children Generate Iconic Vocalizations to Communicate Magnitude

Credibility: Norwegian Students Evaluate Media Studies Web Sites

SYLVIA L. REED Curriculum Vitae

ON THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PHONOLOGY AND PHONETICS (OR WHY PHONETICS IS NOT PHONOLOGY)

Scandinavian Dialect Syntax Transnational collaboration, data collection, and resource development

Extended Projections of Adjectives and Comparative Deletion

An Analysis of Classroom Discourse: The Usefulness of Sinclair and Coulthard s Rank Scale in a Language Classroom

Clauses and Phrases. For Proper Sentence Structure

The Cambridge English Scale explained A guide to converting practice test scores to Cambridge English Scale scores

How Can Teachers Teach Listening?

10th Grade Language. Goal ISAT% Objective Description (with content limits) Vocabulary Words

SPEECH ACTS THEORIES A.

Career Paths for the CDS Major

A discourse approach to teaching modal verbs of deduction. Michael Howard, London Metropolitan University. Background

Glossary of key terms and guide to methods of language analysis AS and A-level English Language (7701 and 7702)

Transcription:

The role of intonation for the perception of sentence types in Greek Anthi Chaida 1, Marios Fourakis 2, Sofia Loui 1 1 Laboratory of Phonetics and Computational Linguistics, University of Athens, Greece 2 Department of Communication Disorders, Wisconsin-Madison, US 1 Introduction The present experimental study focuses on the effects of intonation on sentence type-perception in Greek. It aims to investigate whether intonation can be a decisive factor for the perception of statements, polar questions, wh-questions and commands. For this purpose two experiments were carried out: (a) one based on artificial (hum) sound analogs, where all linguistic information, except prosody, is eliminated (Chaida, 8), and (b) one based on flat synthetic stimuli, where all intonational information was eliminated. 1.1 Background It has often been assumed that one of the most uncontroversial functions of intonation is that of conveying different illocutionary aspects or modes (Hirst & Di Cristo, 1998). Different sentence types are usually associated with both local and global tonal structures. Questions e.g., in comparison to statements, are mostly associated with higher tonal structures, such as a higher tonal register, less tonal declination or a final tonal rise (among others, Gussenhoven, 1984; Grønnum, 1998; t Hart, 1998; van Heuven & Haan, ; Botinis, et al., 1; Makarova, 1). Although several linguistic factors may be associated with different sentence types, such as lexical, syntactic and prosodic ones, the prosodic factor is critical, especially in languages where different sentence types may differ only in prosody, such as polar questions in Greek and Italian. In the absence of any morphological or syntactic markers, the distinction between sentence types is assumed to be based on the prosodic form. Studies in Greek prosody have indicated that there are several tonal characteristics, which may uniquely distinguish each sentence type (Botinis, 1998; Baltazani, 2, 3, 7; Chaida, 7, among others). More specifically, the following (see Figure 1 from Chaida, 7) typical tonal structures have been found to correspond to statements, (polar and wh-) questions and commands. 7

Figure 1. Stylized tonal structures and tonal boundaries in four main sentence types in Greek. 2 Experiment A A main question posed for the first experiment (preliminary data were reported in Chaida, 8) is whether prosody alone can be a decisive factor for the perception of statements, polar and wh- questions and commands. For that purpose, artificial (hum) sound analogs were used, without any linguistic information, except for prosody. 2.1 Methodology A A representative set of utterances produced by three female speakers in their early twenties, with standard Athenian pronunciation, was selected from Greek speech material used in previous studies (Chaida 7). The speech material included variations of the simple sentence o ma'nolis ma'zevi le'moɲa ( Manolis is picking lemons ), and the complex sentence o ma'nolis ma'zevi le'moɲa ce/ 'otan/ pa'rolo pu i ma'ria mi'razi ba'loɲa ( Manolis is picking lemons and/when/although Maria is distributing balloons ), both coordinated and subordinated produced as statements, polar questions, wh-questions and commands, mainly by prosodic means and minor alterations (such as the addition of the wh-word jati -why for wh-questions and the use of the imperative form of the verb for commands). This set of recorded utterances was randomized in five different lists. These utterances comprised the perception test stimuli, which were processed with Praat 4.5.21 (Boersma & Weenik, 7), in order to create sounds with the algorithm described at Point Process: To Sound (hum), in which the glottal source sound was replaced by a hum source ( Pitch (hum) resynthesis ). This process aimed to create stimuli for which prosody would be the only factor judged without any lexical, morphological or syntactic influences. The synthetic stimuli were presented to 32 informants, male and female, - 4 years old, native speakers of Greek, with no hearing problems, through a program designed for this perception test on C#, functioning on.net framework. The experiment took place in a quiet room through headphones (Sennheiser HD5 closed back headphones, response bandwidth 14- Hz). The informants were asked to select the most suitable answer for each of the stimuli in one closed set test. They were instructed to identify statement, question and 8

command, after they heard each stimulus, and then grade their answer within a 1-6 scale of certainty (1=least certain, 6=most certain). Statistical analysis was carried out with Statgraphics 5. and StatView 5.. 2.2 Results A The results of the first experiment are shown in Figures 2-5. In the first instance, it is evident that, although overall identification rates are not as high as might be expected, the majority of identified sentence types match the intended ones, ranging from 35 to 72%. Statements (Figure 2) were identified as intended by 52.8% of the listeners, while 29.43% identified them as commands and 18.49% as questions. The mean grade for listeners certainty was 3.32 (out of 6), but the ANOVA table showed that certainty was not a significant factor for statements (p>.1, F(2,381)=1.33). 1 8 6 COMMAND 4 Figure 2. Identification rates (%) for intended productions of statements. Commands (Figure 3) were identified as intended by 35.42% of the listeners, while 32.47% identified them as questions and 32.12% as statements. The mean grade for listeners certainty was 3.41 (out of 6), but the ANOVA table showed that certainty was not a significant factor for commands (p>.1, F(2,573)=2.53). 1 8 6 COMMAND 4 COMMAND Figure 3. Identification rates (%) for intended productions of commands. 9

Wh-questions (Figure 4) were identified as intended (questions in general) by 72.92% of the listeners, while 11.72% identified them as commands and 15.36% as statements. Certainty was a significant factor for wh-questions, as shown by the ANOVA table (p<.1, F(2,381)=24.37). 1 8 6 COMMAND 4 WH- Figure 4. Identification rates (%) for intended productions of wh-questions. Polar questions (Figure 5) were identified as intended (questions in general) by 45.31% of the listeners, while 21.7% identified them as commands and 32.99% as statements. Certainty was a significant factor for polar questions, as shown by the ANOVA table (p<.1, F(2,573)=35.8). 1 8 6 COMMAND 4 POLAR Figure 5. Identification rates (%) for intended productions of polar questions. According to the chi-square test, the observed value of intended is related to its value for identified in all cases at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =288.57, df=6, p<.1). Concerning the factor of certainty, the relevant chi-square test carried out revealed that the observed value of certainty is related to its value for answer at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =86.19, df=5, p<.1). 1

Sentence complexity is proved to be a significant factor for sentence-type perception, related to the listeners answers at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =8.39, df=1, p<.1). An interesting observation is that simple sentences had lower rate of correct identification (42.86%), compared to complex ones (5.82%). Furthermore, conjunctions seem to influence listeners choice, since conjunction variation is related to the answers given at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =18.72, df=3, p<.1). Additionally, ANOVA tables split by sentence types show that conjunctions are significant for commands (F(3,572)=8.92, p<.1), for polar questions (F(3,572)=13.39, p<.1), and for wh-questions (F(3,38)=9.94, p<.1), but not for statements (F(3,38)=.75, p>.1). 3 Experiment Β The second experiment, in comparison to the first one, aimed to investigate the perception of sentence types without the influence of intonation, based only on linguistic information, with the aid of duration and intensity. The basic question investigated through this experiment is how important is the effect of intonation on the perception of sentence types in Greek, in particular of statements and polar (yes/no) questions. The experiment aims to examine if and to what extent sentence types can be recognized based only on morphosyntactic information. 3.1 Methodology B This experiment involved statements and polar questions and the same set of natural utterances was used, including simple and complex sentences. The material used consisted of 4 natural statement utterances and 4 natural polar question utterances of simple and complex sentences. The speech material included variations of the simple sentence o ma'nolis ma'zevi le'moɲa (Manolis is picking lemons), and the complex sentence o ma'nolis ma'zevi le'moɲa ce/ 'otan/ pa'rolo pu i ma'ria mi'razi ba'loɲa (Manolis is picking lemons and/when/although Maria is distributing balloons), both coordinated and subordinated (see 2.1). The material was produced by two women in their late twenties at the time of the experiment, native speakers of standard Athenian Greek. The natural stimuli were processed with Praat 4.5.21 (Boersma & Weenik, 7), in order to convert the F to a flat contour, based on the mean of tonal width of each utterance, so as only linguistic information with duration and intensity would be the only factors judged without any intonational influences. The synthetic stimuli were mixed with natural stimuli. Thus, a set of 16 utterance (8 flat synthetic + 8 natural) was set up, which was randomized 5 times. These stimuli were presented to 3 informants, 15 male and 15 female, in their early twenties, native speakers of Greek, with no hearing problems, through a program designed for this perception test on C#, functioning on.net framework. 11

The experiment took place in a quiet room through headphones (Sennheiser HD 5 closed back headphones, response bandwidth 14- Hz). The informants were asked to select the most suitable for them answer for each of the stimuli in one closed set test. They were instructed to identify statement, question and command, after they heard each stimulus, and then grade their answer within a 1-6 scale of certainty (1=least certain, 6=most certain). Statistic analysis was carried out with Statgraphics 5. and StatView 5.. 3.2 Results B The results of the second experiment are shown in Figures 6-9. It is worth mentioning that the natural stimuli alone were identified as intended 99% of the times. However, the results for flattened synthetic stimuli were disparate, since statements received quite high identification rates (8 %), while questions received quite low identification rates (23 %). In Figure 6 the identification results of the natural and flat synthetic statements, in total, are presented, and in Figure 7 the identification results of the natural and flat synthetic questions, in total, are presented. Statements, natural and synthetic, in total, were identified as statements 89.5 % of times and only 1.5 % as questions. Questions were identified as questions 6.67 % of times, while 39.33 % of them were identified as statements. However, it is reasonable to have such high percentages of correct identification, since in the total of answers identification of natural stimuli is also included. 1 8 6 4 Figure 6. Identification of statements, natural and flat synthetic in total (%). 12

1 8 6 4 POLAR Figure 7. Identification of polar questions, natural and flat synthetic in total (%). In Figure 8 the identification results of flat synthetic statements are presented alone, and in Figure 9 the identification results of flat synthetic questions are presented alone. It is shown that out of 3 flat synthetic statement stimuli 8.33 % of them were identified as statements 19.67 % of them as questions. However, the identification results for questions are of more interest, since 76.33 % out of 3 flat synthetic questions stimuli were identified as statements, while only 23.67 % of them were perceived as questions, and thus as intended. According to the chi-square test, the observed value of intended is related to its value for identified at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =327., df=1, p<.1). The ANOVA table showed that the gender of speakers or listeners was not significant (F(1,1198)=2.539, p>.1). Concerning the factor of certainty in all cases, the relevant chi-square test showed that it is related to its value for answer at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =, df=, p<.1). Figure 8. Identification of flat synthetic statements (%). 13

Figure 9. Identification of flat synthetic polar questions (%). Certainty ratings were fairly high, in general: 4.66 mean value for statements and 5.11 for questions (out of 6). Concerning the factor of certainty, in all cases, the chi-square test showed that the observed value of certainty is related to its value for answer at the 99% confidence level (x 2 =51.18, df=5, p<.1). Certainty was statistically significant for answers (F(1,1198)=24.61, p<.1). 4 Discussion The results of the present study indicate that intonation is a very important factor for the perception of sentence types in Greek. On one hand, the first experiment showed that sentence types may be identified through prosody, but it seems that this might not be the only distinctive cue for perception, since the identification rates were not very high in all cases. However, identification rates could not be expected to rise higher, because of the nature of the stimuli, which were obtained by synthetic modification (hum), as other studies with synthetic stimuli also indicate (e.g. Makarova, 1). Thus, in the present experiment, an identification of 5-6% might be considered as an indicator of categorization between the 3 sentence types (statement, question, command). Apart from that, statistic analysis revealed that the listeners choices were not random, since it was closely related to all factors that contributed to the experiment (sentence type, complexity, conjunctions). Wh-questions received the highest identification rates, while for the other sentence types falling boundary tones seem to be confusing for listeners. The findings of the first experiment (see also Chaida, 8) agree with earlier studies demonstrating that in many languages low or falling boundary tones elicit declarative judgments, and high or rising tones lead to interrogative (e.g. Thorsen, 198, Makarova 1). On the other hand, the second experiment showed that intonation is crucial for the perception of sentence types, since linguistic and other prosodic information 14

are not sufficient. In particular, it was shown that without any intonational information statements and questions could not be distinguished adequately. Quite apart from that, according to the results of the second experiment, listeners tend to prefer statements for sentences with SVO structure. The fact that statement is taken as a default by listeners, when no intonational cues are present requires more investigation, since it might involve psycholinguistic processes. In general, each sentence type has an acoustically distinct tonal structure, characterized in Greek by the type and location of tonal prominence (nucleus) and boundary tone (Botinis et al., ; Baltazani, 2, 3; Chaida, 5, 7). Actually, findings from previous studies showed that natural stimuli achieved one-to-one identification (Chaida, 5). In conclusion, according to the findings of this study, it seems that other linguistic or/and phonetic factors contribute to the perception of sentence types in Greek, however intonation plays a decisive role. Acknowledgments Many thanks to Charalabos Themistocleous for programming the software for the perception tests, to Antonis Botinis for his comments, and to all the informants. The present study is part of the program The prosodic structure of Greek sentences within the Kapodistrias II framework of the University of Athens. References Baltazani, M. 7. Intonation of polar questions and the location of nuclear stress in Greek. In: Carlos Gussenhoven & Tomas Riad (eds.), Tones and Tunes, Volume II, Experimental Studies in Word and Sentence Prosody. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, 387-45. Baltazani, M. 3. Broad focus across sentence types in Greek. In Proceedings of the 7th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology participants, EUROSPEECH 3. Geneva, Switzerland. Baltazani, M. 2. Quantifier Scope and the Role of Intonation in Greek. PhD Thesis, UCLA. Boersma, P., & D. Weenik. 7. Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.6.9). http://www.praat.org. Retrieved 17 August 1. Botinis, A. 1998. Intonation in Greek. In: Hirst, D. and Di Cristo, A. (eds), Intonation Systems: A Survey of Languages. Cambridge: CUP, 288-31. Botinis, A., Granström, B., Möbius, B. 1. Developments and paradigms in intonation research. Speech Communication 33, 263-296. 15

Botinis, A., Bannert, R., Tatham, M.. Contrastive tonal analysis of focus perception in Greek and Swedish. In: Botinis, A. (ed), Intonation: Analysis, Modelling and Technology. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 97-116. Chaida, A. 8. Prosodic perception of sentence types in Greek. In Proceedings of the 2nd ISCA Workshop on Experimental Linguistics, Athens, Greece, 61-64. Chaida, A. 7. Tonal structures of complex sentences in Greek. In Proceedings of the 8 th Internation Conference on Greek Linguistics. Ioannina, Greece. Chaida, A. 5. Intonation of sentence types and focus in Greek: production and perception. MA Thesis, University of Skövde, Sweden, & University of Athens, Greece. Hirst, D., Di Cristo, A. (eds). 1998. Intonation Systems: A Survey of Twenty Languages. Cambridge: CUP. Grønnum (Thorsen), N. 1998. Intonation in Danish. In: Hirst, D., Di Cristo, A. (eds), Intonation Systems: A Survey of Twenty languages. Cambridge: CUP, 131-151. Gussenhoven, C. 1984. On the grammar and semantics of sentences accents. Dordrecht: Foris. Makarova, V. 1. Perceptual correlates of sentence-type intonation in Russian and Japanese. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 137-154. t Hart, J. 1998. Intonation in Dutch. In: Hirst, D., Di Cristo, A. (eds), Intonation Systems: A Survey of Twenty Languages. Cambridge: CUP, 96-111. Thorsen, N. 198. A study of the perception of sentence intonation -- evidence from Danish, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 114-13. van Heuven, V.J., Haan, J.. Phonetic correlates of statement versus question intonation in Dutch. In: Botinis, A. (ed.), Intonation: Analysis, Modelling and Technology. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 119-143. 16