Constructivist Learning and Teaching

Similar documents
LEARNING THEORIES Ausubel's Learning Theory

Current California Math Standards Balanced Equations

curriculum for excellence building the curriculum 2 active learning a guide to developing professional practice

Commutative Property Grade One

An Overview of the Developmental Stages in Children's Drawings

Five High Order Thinking Skills

Planning a Class Session

Running head: PERSONAL STATEMENT ON LEARNING AND INSTRUCTION 1. Personal Statement on Learning and Instruction. Jay A. Bostwick

Integer Operations. Overview. Grade 7 Mathematics, Quarter 1, Unit 1.1. Number of Instructional Days: 15 (1 day = 45 minutes) Essential Questions

Jean Piaget: A Cognitive Account of Development

Classroom Management Plan

MATHEMATICS: REPEATING AND GROWING PATTERNS First Grade. Kelsey McMahan. Winter 2012 Creative Learning Experiences

Circuits and Boolean Expressions

Math Games For Skills and Concepts

HarperOne Reading and Discussion Guide for The Problem of Pain. Reading and Discussion Guide for. C. S. Lewis

Geometry, Technology, and the Reasoning and Proof Standard inthemiddlegradeswiththegeometer ssketchpad R

Jean Piaget: Cognitive Theorist 1. Theorists from centuries ago have provided support and research about the growth of

Curriculum Alignment Project

Stages of development

Our research-based knowledge about good math. What Does Good Math Instruction Look Like?

Fostering Mathematical Thinking and Problem Solving:

Cognitive Psychology

Problem of the Month: Cutting a Cube

Alecia Hudson. St. Edward s University

Cognitive and Motor Development. Four Domains. Interaction. Affective Cognitive Motor Physical. Why organize into domains?

The development of number concepts is a major emphasis of mathematics instruction with

Teaching Place Value in First Grade: A Resource Guide

Inquiry, the Learning Cycle, & the 5E Instructional Model From the Guidelines for Lesson Planning from the Electronic Journal of Science Education:

What is Organizational Communication?

What Is Singapore Math?

Math vocabulary can be taught with what Montessorians call the Three Period Lesson.

Chapter 5, Learning to Think

Tom wants to find two real numbers, a and b, that have a sum of 10 and have a product of 10. He makes this table.

A Math Talk Community in Math Expressions Common Core

Teaching About Controversial Issues

Research on Graphic Organizers

Probability and Statistics

Cognitive Development

Independent samples t-test. Dr. Tom Pierce Radford University

Algorithms in Everyday Mathematics

The problem was written on the board

RACE TO CLEAR THE MAT

Cognitively Guided Instruction Developing Mathematical Reasoning Using Word Problems

COMMUNICATION UNIT: IV. Educational Technology-B.Ed Notes

Fractions as Numbers INTENSIVE INTERVENTION. National Center on. at American Institutes for Research

Guidance paper - The use of calculators in the teaching and learning of mathematics

Cognitive Development

Section 1.5 Exponents, Square Roots, and the Order of Operations

The Elementary School Math Project. Money Counts. Students will count and compare amounts of money less than or equal to one dollar.

Writing Thesis Defense Papers

Learning and Teaching

Reflections on Some Work of Jean Piaget. Samuel Otten. Michigan State University

Moral and Intellectual Development Through Play: How to Promote Children's Development Through Playing Group Games

Assessment Management

National Academy of Sciences Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism

Radius, Diameter, Circumference, π, Geometer s Sketchpad, and You! T. Scott Edge

Mathematics Task Arcs

1. I have 4 sides. My opposite sides are equal. I have 4 right angles. Which shape am I?

EDUC 2112 Foundations of Education

TEACHER IDENTITY AND DIALOGUE: A COMMENT ON VAN RIJSWIJK, AKKERMAN & KOSTER. Willem Wardekker VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Sample Fraction Addition and Subtraction Concepts Activities 1 3

Arguments and Dialogues

Addition of Multidigit Numbers

Technology in Early Childhood Education Debbie Meadows. California State University Bakersfield EDCI 604 Dr. Swenson

Local Government and Leaders Grade Three

Handouts for teachers

A STUDY GUIDE. Nonfiction Matters. Stephanie Harvey

Preschool. Assessment Kit. Activity probes for the Preschool Sequence critical skills packed in a teacher friendly assessment kit!

Christina Gawlik, Ph.D Texas Woman s University 1200 Frame Street, Denton, Texas cgawlik@twu.edu

Mathematical Knowledge as a Social Construct of Teaching- / Learning Processes The Epistemology Oriented Mathematical Interaction Research

Unifying Epistemologies by Combining World, Description and Observer

Proposal: Formal and informal HIV Health education at La Clinica. Antonio J. Marante

BUSINESS STRATEGY SYLLABUS

Ch. 13.2: Mathematical Expectation

Developing Base Ten Understanding: Working with Tens, The Difference Between Numbers, Doubling, Tripling, Splitting, Sharing & Scaling Up

Unit 1 Number Sense. In this unit, students will study repeating decimals, percents, fractions, decimals, and proportions.

Teaching Approaches Using Graphing Calculator in the Classroom for the Hearing-Impaired Student

Authentic Intellectual Work in Social Studies: Putting Performance Before Pedagogy

Writing Learning Objectives that Engage Future Engineers: Hands-on & Minds-on Learning Activities

Last May, philosopher Thomas Nagel reviewed a book by Michael Sandel titled

Maths Targets for pupils in Year 2

Integrating Technology into the Classroom. Trevor Moore. Western Oregon University

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY. effectiveness of, the psychology of teaching, and the social psychology of schools as

Guided Reading: Constructivism in Action. Donna Kester Phillips, Niagara University. Abstract

Standards for Mathematical Practice: Commentary and Elaborations for 6 8

the Median-Medi Graphing bivariate data in a scatter plot

STEP 5: Giving Feedback

What Is the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program?

INTRODUCTION TO CONCEPT MAPPING. Joseph D. Novak Professor of Education and Professor of Biological Sciences Cornell University

Does coaching work? Can mathematics coaches make a difference?

Kindergarten Math Content 1

Assessment of learning outcomes

LEARNERS UNDERSTANDING OF THE ADDITION OF FRACTIONS

Mathematical Modelling and New Theories of Learning. Jo Boaler, Stanford University.

Kindergarten Math Curriculum Course Description and Philosophy Text Reference:

Similar Triangles Grade Seven

ADDITION LESSON Excerpts

Hybrid Instructional Design Model Based on Constructivism for Web

Transcription:

Copyright 2009 by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Inc. www.nctm.org. All rights reserved. This material may not be copied or distributed electronically or in other formats without written permission from NCTM. Constructivist Learning and Teaching Douglas H. Clements and Michael T. Battista In reality, no one can teach mathematics. Effective teachers are those who can stimulate students to learn mathematics. Educational research offers compelling evidence that students learn mathematics well only when they construct their own mathematical understanding. (MSEB and National Research Council 1989, 58) Radical changes have been advocated in recent reports on mathematics education, such as NCTM s Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (National Council of Teachers of Mathematics 1989) and Everybody Counts (MSEB and National Research Council 1989). Unfortunately, many educators are focusing on alterations in content rather than the reports recommendations for fundamental changes in instructional practices. Many of these instructional changes can best be understood from a constructivist perspective. Although references to constructivist approaches are pervasive, practical descriptions of such approaches have not been readily accessible. Therefore, to promote dialogue about instructional change, each Research into Practice column this year * will illustrate how a constructivist approach to teaching might be taken for a specific topic in mathematics. What Is Constructivism? Most traditional mathematics instruction and curricula are based on the transmission, or absorption, view of teaching and learning. In this view, students passively absorb mathematical structures invented by others and recorded in texts or known by authoritative adults. Teaching consists of transmitting sets of established facts, skills, and concepts to students. Constructivism offers a sharp contrast to this view. Its basic tenets which are embraced to a greater or lesser extent by different proponents are the following: *1990 1991 1. Knowledge is actively created or invented by the child, not passively received from the environment. This idea can be illustrated by the Piagetian position that mathematical ideas are made by children, not found like a pebble or accepted from others like a gift (Sinclair, in Steffe and Cobb 1988). For example, the idea four cannot be directly detected by a child s senses. It is a relation that the child superimposes on a set of objects. This relation is constructed by the child by reflecting on actions performed on numerous sets of objects, such as contrasting the counting of sets having four units with the counting of sets having three and five units. Although a teacher may have demonstrated and numerically labeled many sets of objects for the student, the mental entity four can be created only by the student s thought. In other words, students do not discover the way the world works like Columbus found a new continent. Rather they invent new ways of thinking about the world. 2. Children create new mathematical knowledge by reflecting on their physical and mental actions. Ideas are constructed or made meaningful when children integrate them into their existing structures of knowledge. 3. No one true reality exists, only individual interpretations of the world. These interpretations are shaped by experience and social interactions. Thus, learning mathematics should be thought of as a process of adapting to and organizing one s quantitative world, not discovering preexisting ideas imposed by others. (This tenet is perhaps the most controversial.) 4. Learning is a social process in which children grow into the intellectual life of those around them (Bruner 1986). Mathematical ideas and truths, both in use and in meaning, are cooperatively established by the members of a culture. Thus, the constructivist classroom is seen as a culture in which students are involved not only in discovery and invention but in a social discourse involving explanation, negotiation, sharing, and evaluation. 6 Putting Research into Practice in the Elementary Grades: Readings from Journals of the NCTM

5. When a teacher demands that students use set mathematical methods, the sense-making activity of students is seriously curtailed. Students tend to mimic the methods by rote so that they can appear to achieve the teacher s goals. Their beliefs about the nature of mathematics change from viewing mathematics as sense making to viewing it as learning set procedures that make little sense. Two Major Goals Although it has many different interpretations, taking a constructivist perspective appears to imply two major goals for mathematics instruction (Cobb 1988). First, students should develop mathematical structures that are more complex, abstract, and powerful than the ones they currently possess so that they are increasingly capable of solving a wide variety of meaningful problems. Second, students should become autonomous and self-motivated in their mathematical activity. Such students believe that mathematics is a way of thinking about problems. They believe that they do not get mathematical knowledge from their teacher so much as from their own explorations, thinking, and participation in discussions. They see their responsibility in the mathematics classroom not so much as completing assigned tasks but as making sense of, and communicating about, mathematics. Such independent students have the sense of themselves as controlling and creating mathematics. Teaching and Learning Constructivist instruction, on the one hand, gives preeminent value to the development of students personal mathematical ideas. Traditional instruction, on the other hand, values only established mathematical techniques and concepts. For example, even though many teachers consistently use concrete materials to introduce ideas, they use them only for an introduction; the goal is to get to the abstract, symbolic, established mathematics. Inadvertently, students intuitive thinking about what is meaningful to them is devalued. They come to feel that their intuitive ideas and methods are not related to real mathematics. In contrast, in constructivist instruction, students are encouraged to use their own methods for solving problems. They are not asked to adopt someone else s thinking but encouraged to refine their own. Although the teacher presents tasks that promote the invention or adoption of more sophisticated techniques, all methods are valued and supported. Through interaction with mathematical tasks and other students, the student s own intuitive mathematical thinking gradually becomes more abstract and powerful. Because the role of the constructivist teacher is to guide and support students invention of viable mathematical ideas rather than transmit correct adult ways of doing mathematics, some see the constructivist approach as inefficient, free-for-all discovery. In fact, even in its least directive form, the guidance of the teacher is the feature that distinguishes constructivism from unguided discovery. The constructivist teacher, by offering appropriate tasks and opportunities for dialogue, guides the focus of students attention, thus unobtrusively directing their learning (Bruner 1986). Constructivist teachers must be able to pose tasks that bring about appropriate conceptual reorganizations in students. This approach requires knowledge of both the normal developmental sequence in which students learn specific mathematical ideas and the current individual structures of students in the class. Such teachers must also be skilled in structuring the intellectual and social climate of the classroom so that students discuss, reflect on, and make sense of these tasks. An Invitation Each article in this year s Research into Practice column will present specific examples of the constructivist approach in action. Each will describe how students think about particular mathematical ideas and how instructional environments can be structured to cause students to develop more powerful thinking about those ideas. We invite you to consider the approach and how it relates to your teaching to try it in your classroom. Which tenets of constructivism might you accept? How might your teaching and classroom environment change if you accept that students must construct their own knowledge? Are the implications different for students of different ages? How do you deal with individual differences? Most important, what instructional methods are consistent with a constructivist view of learning? Constructivist Learning and Teaching 7

References Bruner, Jerome. Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1986. Cobb, Paul. The Tension between Theories of Learning and Instruction in Mathematics Education. Educational Psychologist 23 (1988): 87 103. Mathematical Sciences Education Board (MSEB) and National Research Council. Everybody Counts: A Report to the Nation on the Future of Mathematics Education. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press, 1989. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics. Reston, Va.: NCTM, 1989. Steffe, Leslie, and Paul Cobb. Construction of Arithmetical Meanings and Strategies. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1988. 8 Putting Research into Practice in the Elementary Grades: Readings from Journals of the NCTM

Constructivism and First-Grade Arithmetic Constance Kamii and Barbara A. Lewis For arithmetic instruction in the first grade, we advocate the use of games and situations in daily living in contrast to the traditional use of textbooks, workbooks, and worksheets. Our position is supported by the research and theory of Jean Piaget, called constructivism, as well as by classroom research (Kamii 1985, 1990). Piaget s theory shows that children acquire number concepts by constructing them from the inside rather than by internalizing them from the outside. The best way to explain this statement is by describing children s reactions to one of the tasks Piaget developed with Inhelder (Inhelder and Piaget 1963). The pupil is given one of two identical glasses, and the teacher takes the other one. After putting thirty to fifty chips (or beans, buttons, etc.) on the table, the teacher asks the pupil to drop a chip into his or her glass each time she drops one into hers. When about five chips have thus been dropped into each glass with one-to-one correspondence, the teacher says, Let s stop now, and you watch what I am going to do. The teacher then drops one chip into her glass and says to the pupil, Let s get going again. The teacher and the pupil drop about five more chips into each glass with one-to-one correspondence, until the teacher says, Let s stop. The following is what has happened so far: Teacher: Pupil: 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 The teacher then asks, Do we have the same amount, or do you have more, or do I have more? Four-year-olds usually reply that the two glasses have the same amount. When we go on to ask, How do you know that we have the same amount? the pupils explain, Because I can see that we both have the same amount. (Some four-year-olds, however, reply that they have more, and when asked how they know that they have more, their usual answer is Because. ) The teacher goes on to ask, Do you remember how we dropped the chips? and four-year-olds usually give all the empirical facts correctly, including the fact that only the teacher put an additional chip into her glass at one point. In other words, four-yearolds remember all the empirical facts correctly and base their judgment of equality on the empirical appearance of the two quantities. By age five or six, however, most middle-class pupils deduce logically that the teacher has one more. When we ask these pupils how they know that the teacher has one more, they invoke exactly the same empirical facts as the four-year-olds. No one teaches five- and six-year-olds to give correct answers to these questions. Yet children all over the world become able to give correct answers by constructing numerical relationships through their own natural ability to think. This construction from within can best be explained by reviewing the distinction Piaget made among three kinds of knowledge according to their sources physical knowledge, logicomathematical knowledge, and social (conventional) knowledge. Physical knowledge, on the one hand, is knowledge of objects in external reality. The color and weight of a chip are examples of physical properties that are in objects in external reality and can be known empirically by observation. Logicomathematical knowledge, on the other hand, consists of relationships created by each individual. For instance, when we are presented with a red chip and a blue one and think that they are different, this difference is an example of logicomathematical knowledge. The chips are observable, but the difference between them is not. The difference exists neither in the red chip nor in the blue one, and if a person did not put the objects into this relationship, the difference would not exist for him or her. Other examples of relationships the individual can create between the chips are similar, the same in weight, and two. Physical knowledge is thus empirical in nature because it has its source partly in objects. Logicomathematical knowledge, however, is not Constructivism and First-Grade Arithmetic 9

empirical knowledge, as its source is in each individual s head. The ultimate sources of social knowledge are conventions worked out by people. Examples of social knowledge are the fact that Christmas comes on 25 December and that a tree is called tree. Words such as one, two, and three and numerals such as 1, 2, and 3 belong to social knowledge, but the numerical concepts necessary to understand these numerals belong to logicomathematical knowledge. Keeping the distinction among the three kinds of knowledge in mind, one can understand why most four-year-olds in the task described earlier said that the two glasses have the same amount. The four-yearolds had not yet constructed the logicomathematical relationship of number and could therefore gain only physical knowledge from the experience. From the appearance of the chips in the glasses, the pupils concluded that the amount was the same despite the fact that they remembered the way in which the chips had been dropped. Once the concept of number has developed, however, pupils will deduce from the same empirical facts that the teacher has one more chip regardless of the physical appearance. New Goals for Beginning Arithmetic Instruction If children develop mathematical understanding through their own natural ability to think, the goals of beginning arithmetic must be that pupils think and construct a network of numerical relationships. To add five and four, for example, pupils have to think (1 1 1 1 1) (1 1 1 1). This operation requires pupils to make two wholes (5 and 4) in their heads and then to make a higher-order whole (9) in which the original wholes (5 and 4) become parts. An example of a network of numerical relationships can be seen when pupils think about 5 4 as one more than 4 4 and as one less than 5 5. Addition thus involves a great deal of thinking, that is, the making of relationships rather than mere skills (such as penmanship). This definition of goals for instruction is very different from traditional instruction that focuses on correct answers and the writing of mathematical symbols. It is also very different from the assumption that pupils have to internalize addition facts, store them, and retrieve them in computerlike fashion. New Principles of Teaching The following principles of teaching flow from constructivism and the preceding goals: 1. Encourage pupils to invent their own ways of adding and subtracting numbers rather than tell them how. For example, if pupils can play a board game with one die, we simply introduce a second die and let them figure out what to do. 2. Encourage pupils to exchange points of view rather than reinforce correct answers and correct wrong ones. For example, if a pupil says that six minus two equals three, we encourage pupils to agree or disagree with each other. Pupils will eventually agree on the truth if they debate long enough because, in logicomathematical knowledge, nothing is arbitrary. 3. Encourage pupils to think rather than to compute with paper and pencil. Written computation interferes with pupils freedom to think and to remember sums and differences. Classroom Activities Paper-and-pencil exercises cause social isolation, mechanical repetition, and dependence on the teacher to know if an answer is correct. We, therefore, replace the textbook, workbook, and worksheets with two kinds of activities: games and situations in daily living. Games, such as a modification of old maid in which pupils try to make a sum of ten with two cards, are well known to be effective. Although games are typically used only as a reward for pupils who have finished their work, we use games as a staple of instruction. Games give rise to compelling reasons for pupils to think and to agree or disagree with each other. When it is useful to know that 5 5, 6 4, 7 3, and so on, all equal ten, pupils are much more likely to remember these combinations than when they write in workbooks to satisfy the teacher. Situations in daily living also offer meaningful opportunities for pupils to construct mathematical relationships. Taking attendance, voting, collecting money, and sending notes home are examples of situations the teacher can use to encourage pupils to think. If four people brought their lunch, eight 10 Putting Research into Practice in the Elementary Grades: Readings from Journals of the NCTM

ordered the special, and six ordered soup and sandwich, the teacher can ask if everybody present has been accounted for. Pupils care about real-life situations and think much harder about these questions than about those in workbooks. References Inhelder, Bärbel, and Jean Piaget, De I itération des actions à la récurrence Elémentaire. In La formation des raisonnements récurrentiels, edited by Pierre Greco, Bärbel Inhelder, B. Matalon, and Jean Piaget. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1963. Kamii, Constance. Constructivism and Beginning Arithmetic (K 2). In Teaching and Learning Mathematics in the 1990s, 1990 Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), edited by Thomas J. Cooney and Christian R. Hirsch, pp. 22 30. Reston, Va.: NCTM, 1990.. Young Children Reinvent Arithmetic. New York: Teachers College Press, 1985. Constructivism and First-Grade Arithmetic 11