CAUSAL RELATIONAL REASONING OF 5 TH GRADERS USING DENSITY IN EXPLAINING FLOATING-SINKING PHENOMENA

Similar documents
DOES OUR INITIAL TRAINING PROGRAM FOR PRIMARY TEACHERS REALLY WORK? DESIGN, IMPLEMENTATION AND ANALYSIS OF A QUESTIONNAIRE

DESIGNING VIRTUAL EXPERIMENTS IN ELECTRIC CIRCUITS BY HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida

Physics Teacher Education Program Web Site. Journal of Physics Teacher Education Online 10/22/11 7:53 AM 1

NATIONAL COMPETENCY-BASED TEACHER STANDARDS (NCBTS) A PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT GUIDE FOR FILIPINO TEACHERS

How Students Interpret Literal Symbols in Algebra: A Conceptual Change Approach

Chapter 2 Conceptualizing Scientific Inquiry

Science Notebooks. a tool for increasing student understanding of inquiry and science content a tool for enhancing literacy skills

Research into competency models in arts education

Designing Socio-Technical Systems to Support Guided Discovery-Based Learning in Students: The Case of the Globaloria Game Design Initiative

When students enter the classroom, they

THE SYSTEM OF TEACHER EDUCATION FOR MOTIVATIONAL TEACHING OF GIFTED STUDENTS

Instructional Systems Design

PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT: A VISION FOR SAUDI SCIENCE TEACHERS

Writing Learning Objectives

Joseph K. Torgesen, Department of Psychology, Florida State University

A Laboratory for Research, a Studio for Child Learning, and a Site for Student Clinical Experience in the UJ Institute for Childhood Education

PRIMARY SCIENCE. Syllabus for Primary Schools. Curriculum Department, Floriana

A. The master of arts, educational studies program will allow students to do the following.

CLASSROOM DISCOURSE TYPES AND STUDENTS LEARNING OF AN INTERACTION DIAGRAM AND NEWTON S THIRD LAW

The Creative Curriculum for Preschool: Objectives for Development & Learning

KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION

Table of Contents Section 8: How Children Learn

Critical Inquiry in Educational Research and Professional Practice

APPENDIX F Science and Engineering Practices in the NGSS

Morris College Teacher Education Curriculum Changes Elementary Education

EARLY CHILDHOOD TEACHERS VIEWS ABOUT SCIENCE TEACHING PRACTICES

Appendix B Data Quality Dimensions

NEW YORK STATE TEACHER CERTIFICATION EXAMINATIONS

An Engagement Model for Learning: Providing a Framework to Identify Technology Services

FOREIGN LANGUAGE TEACHING AN INTERVIEW WITH NINA SPADA

Analysing Technological Pedagogic Content Knowledge of Science Teacher Candidates According to Various Variables

How to Educate and Train Science Teachers in IBSE Experimentation

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Battery Bias

Curriculum Development: Deductive Models

STUDENT HANDBOOK. Master of Education in Early Childhood Education, PreK-4 and Early Childhood Education Certification Programs

CHEMISTRY LABORATORY TEACHING/LEARNING SEQUENCE ON ACIDS AND BASES WITH THE USE OF ICT (SPREADSHEETS, ARGUMENTATION AND COLLABORATION)

Technology Use of Mathematics Teachers at Urban Schools

Rover Races Middle School NGSS, Common Core, and 21 st Century Skills Alignment Document

Alignment of the Hawaii Preschool Content Standards With HighScope s Preschool Child Observation Record (COR), 2nd edition

Science teachers pedagogical studies in Finland

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF. Interpretive Report. Developed by. Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

LAGUARDIA COMMUNITY COLLEGE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS, ENGINEERING, AND COMPUTER SCIENCE

Research on Graphic Organizers

EDF 3214: Human Development and Learning Section 901 Meeting Time: Mondays from 5-9 Room: CPR 256

Abstract Title: Identifying and measuring factors related to student learning: the promise and pitfalls of teacher instructional logs

THE DESIGN AND CREATION OF AN ELECTRONIC LANGUAGE LEARNING ENVIRONMENT FOR GREEK AS FOREIGN/SECOND LANGUAGE

Learning through Computer Game Design: Possible Success (or Failure) Factors

THE IMPORTANCE OF CLASSROOM ASSESSMENT AND EVALUATION IN EDUCATIONAL SYSTEM

University of Kentucky Undergraduate Bulletin 1

JOINT MASTER OF ARTS IN LEADERSHIP AND EDUCATION CHANGE COURSE DESCRIPTIONS

The effects of beliefs about language learning and learning strategy use of junior high school EFL learners in remote districts

Roseberry Primary and Nursery School. Maths Policy

Implementing RTI Using Title I, Title III, and CEIS Funds

PROGRAMME SPECIFICATION

SKILL: What we want students to DO. I can count to 100 by 1 s and by 10s (K.CC.1) I can write the numbers 0-10 (K.CC.3)

A Proposed Program for Postgraduates in Egypt to Acquire the Skills and Techniques for Producing Concept Cartoons for Kindergarten Children

Adaptive information source selection during hypothesis testing

COMMUNITY UNIT SCHOOL DISTRICT 200. Course Description

Faculty of Engineering and Science Curriculum - Aalborg University

Educational research on teaching the nature and process of science

Whitnall School District Report Card Content Area Domains

Outcomes Data School Psychology EdS Program

Perimeter, Area and Volume What Do Units Tell You About What Is Being Measured? Overview

Koroška cesta Maribor, Slovenia

BOILING AND PEELING OF EGGS

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS. four research questions. The first section demonstrates the effects of the strategy

Please see current textbook prices at

Analyzing and Interpreting Data: What makes things sink or float?

The University of North Carolina at Pembroke Academic Catalog

PHILOSOPHY OF THE MATHEMATICS DEPARTMENT

Task-Model Driven Design of Adaptable Educational Hypermedia

DEGREE PROGRAMME IN EARLY CHILDHOOD EDUCATION CURRICULUM

ISAT Mathematics Performance Definitions Grade 4

Minnesota Academic Standards

Learning and Teaching Styles in Language, Science and Technology Education in Nigeria.

Five High Order Thinking Skills

Curriculum for the bachelor s programme in economics and business administration (HA)

Performance Assessment Task Which Shape? Grade 3. Common Core State Standards Math - Content Standards

PRIME. How to Select an Evidence-Based Intervention. A Guide

Italian Language & Culture Courses for Foreigners. ITALY Language Training

Characteristics of Effective and Sustainable Teaching Development Programs for Quality Teaching in Higher Education

LEARNING OUTCOMES FOR THE PSYCHOLOGY MAJOR

The test uses age norms (national) and grade norms (national) to calculate scores and compare students of the same age or grade.

A Correlation of Miller & Levine Biology 2014

Pedagogical Criteria for Successful Use of Wikis as Collaborative Writing Tools in Teacher Education

Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments

Master of Arts in Business Education (MA) 29 January Module 1 Introduction to Business Education (6 ECTS) Content. Learning Outcomes F01 BE01

Opportunity Document for STEP Literacy Assessment

TEACHING AND LEARNING IN COMPETENCY-BASED EDUCATION

A Hands-On Exercise Improves Understanding of the Standard Error. of the Mean. Robert S. Ryan. Kutztown University

Mobile Learning contexts for problem-solving competence assessment at higher education

Utah State Office of Education Elementary STEM Endorsement Course Framework Nature of Science and Engineering

ELL Considerations for Common Core-Aligned Tasks in English Language Arts

Running head: SCHOOL COMPUTER USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE. Using the U.S. PISA results to investigate the relationship between

How To Write A Curriculum Paper On Arts Education In Nsw

Research Basis for Catchup Math

VOLUME MEASUREMENT AND CONSERVATION IN LATE PRIMARY SCHOOL CHILDREN IN CYPRUS

4 Square Writing Method. Developed by Judith Gould Madalyn Jira Taylors Elementary

Transcription:

CAUSAL RELATIONAL REASONING OF 5 TH GRADERS USING DENSITY IN EXPLAINING FLOATING-SINKING PHENOMENA A. Zoupidis 1, D. Pnevmatikos 1, A. Spyrtou 1 and P. Kariotoglou 1 tzoupidis@gmail.com, dpnevmat@uowm.gr, aspirtou@uowm.gr, pkariotog@uowm.gr 1 School of Education, University of Western Macedonia, Florina, Greece Abstract: The aim of the present study was to investigate if students use causal relational reasoning using density in explaining floating/sinking (F/S) phenomena before, during and after the implementation of a Teaching/Learning Sequence (TLS). The study is part of a cross-national research-based curriculum project on Materials Science. One of the theoretical bases for the design of the TLS was focused on a model-based learning approach. The TLS was designed and developed by researchers working in close collaboration with school teachers comprising a University School partnership. Its implementation took place in two science classrooms of forty-one 5 th graders (mean age 10.9; SD =.376) in total. Participants answered 5 tasks (pre, during, just after and seven months after the implementation) on explaining F/S phenomena using the concept of density. A significant shift on participants conceptual understanding was evident; participants grasped the scientific concept of density after the intervention. In addition, it seems that a significant number of participants could successfully explain F/S, using causal relational reasoning. Finally, participants who managed to use density in a causal relational reasoning had a significantly better understanding of density as a property of materials than those who used density in a causal linear reasoning. Keywords: Model-based learning, Causal relational reasoning, Concept acquisition, Density, Floating/Sinking phenomena BACKGROUND, FRAMEWORK, AND PURPOSE Model-based reasoning can be thought of as a continuum in which teachers begin with students basic representational capacities and try to end up near the practices of scientists (Nersessian, 2008). An important constrain in this transition is the narrow range of students causal models. Most of them tend towards simple causal models in explaining phenomena. For instance, when they try to explain F/S of an object, they focus only on the object, giving answers such as it sinks because it is heavy (Kohn, 1993). However, many concepts and theories in science depend on substantially more complex causal models (Perkins & Grotzer, 2005). Causal relational reasoning, being such a complex model, in the case of F/S provides interpretations such as if an object has bigger density than a liquid then it sinks. Perkins & Grotzer (2005) argued that cultivating greater complexity in students causal models leads to better understanding of phenomena and relevant concepts that are used to interpret them. Moreover, there is a considerable agreement that density is an abstruse notion for students. Students, before being exposed to the systematic teaching at schools, have already developed a conceptual framework about matter and material kind, which differs from the scientific one (Smith, Snir & Grosslight 1992; Hardy et al. 2006; Wiser & Smith 2008). Within students conceptual framework, the concepts of weight and density are undistinguished

(Fassoulopoulos, Kariotoglou & Koumaras, 2003). Moreover, students by referring to the density of an object, they focus on only one feature of the object, either on the weight, the size or the shape, (Smith, Snir & Grosslight, 1992). Smith et al. (1992) also argued that the necessary condition to conclude that students have grasped the concept of density, in an elementary level, is to realize that the heaviness of the kind of material is a property of that material. In addition, they claim that this is a prerequisite in order to argue that students can distinguish the concepts of weight and density. RATIONALE Although the concept of density is considered among the most difficult concepts for primary school students, there is little empirical evidence of (TLSs) (Μéheut & Psillos 2004), that use the model-based reasoning to help students to abandon the constraints of the linear causal reasoning ( Perkins & Grotzer, 2005). In this study, we were keen to answer the following research questions: (a) In what extent do students acquire density as a property of materials? (b) Do students use causal relational reasoning in explaining F/S? (c) Does the use of complex causal reasoning help students to understand the concept of density? For this purpose a five unit TLS on density as a materials property (Spyrtou, Zoupidis & Kariotoglou, 2008) has been developed. Fig. 1: The visual dots-in-a-box model of several materials More specifically, in the 1 st unit of the TLS, participants were introduced to the technological problem of the salvage of the Sea Diamond s shipwreck through a video which included a description of the accident and a discussion about its environmental consequences. Furthermore, the participants were familiarized with F/S phenomena and the relevant concepts through several activities, such as real experiments, working in a Predict-Observe- Explore (POE) approach. In the 2 nd unit, the participants working in groups, followed the POE approach in real and simulated experiments in order to identify and test possible variables that affect F/S, for instance, the weight of an object and the kind of the liquid s or the object s material. In the 3 rd unit the participants were introduced to a visual model of density as a property of material, the dots-in-a-box model (Smith, et al., 1992, Figure 1). Using this representation in relevant simulated experiments we expected that the participants would acquire a causal relational reasoning (Perkins & Grotzer, 2005) in order to explain and predict F/S phenomena for homogenous objects. In the 4 th unit, participants used the visual model to come to a conclusion about the F/S of two-materials composite objects, for instance a bottle filled with air or a bottle filled with water. By doing this, we aimed to help participants to understand that the density of a composite object, which consists of two materials, lies between the densities of the two materials. Finally, in the 5 th unit, participants had the opportunity to work in groups in a simulated environment and to investigate the F/S of the Sea Diamond cruise ship, in order to argue about its salvage. METHOD

This study was conducted in two 5 th grade science classrooms. Forty one 5 th graders (mean age 10.9, SD =.38) participated in the study. Each of the five units of the TLS lasted for 2 teaching periods of approximately 80 min in total. The implementation took place during normal daily courses and the female class teacher had great experience in teaching science. Data Collection: Data were collected from multiple sources. For the sake of conciseness, in this paper we will be limited in presenting the results only of the analysis of the learning of density as a property of materials (Tasks 1, 2 and 3), as well as the use of causal relational reasoning in explaining F/S (Tasks 4 and 5) of the pre (just before implementation), interim (between 4 th and 5 th unit), post (just after implementation) and post-post (seven months after implementation) phases of evaluation. More specifically, task 1 asked participants to write a sentence including the words density and material. Task 2 asked participants to choose and explain their choice, on which of two objects of the same amount and material, but of different shape has bigger density, or if they have the same density. Task 3 asked participants to choose and explain their choice, on which of two objects of the same material, but of different size has bigger density, or if they have the same density. In task 4 participants were asked to draw a big sphere and a small triangle in their final position in a liquid, taking into account the densities that are given in dots-in-a-box representation (sphere s material: two dots, triangle s material: six dots and liquid s material: four dots). They were also asked to explain their drawings. Finally, in task 5, participants were asked to choose and explain their choice, about the final position of two identical spheres in a liquid, taking into account the densities that are given in dots-in-a-box representation (object s A material: two dots, object s B material: three dots and liquid s material: four dots). The first choice was that the two objects are floating at the same level, the second that object B floats in a higher level than object A and the third choice is that object A floats in a higher level than object B. Tasks 1, 2 and 3 were addressed to participants in each of the four phases (pre, interim, post and post - post) of the TLS, while tasks 4 and 5, only in the last three phases. Coding: Answers in tasks 1, 2 and 3 were classified in four categories, and in tasks 4 and 5 in two categories, by two independent judges, with 85% agreement which after a discussion was increased to 100%. The same categories were used in the tasks 1, 2, and 3. Phenomenological, incomplete or no answers were classified in the no answer category (0). Answer s that accepted density as dependent on the object s size, (e.g. the big object has bigger density ), were classified in the category extensive idea and they were credited with one (1). Answers that accepted density as a property of materials and dependent on the object s size, (e.g. the objects have the same density because they are made from the same material and the same quantity ), were classified in the category transitional idea and they were credited with two (2). Finally, answers that considered density as a property of materials, (e.g. both objects have the same density because they are from the same material ), were classified in the category intensive idea and they were credited with three (3). The means of the classification in these three tasks were calculated and rounded for each student. Answers that were based on causal relational reasoning to explain F/S, (e.g. object A floats because its density is smaller than the liquid s and object B sinks because its density is higher than the liquid s ), in task 4, were classified as causal relational and credited with one (1). Additionally, answers that used causal linear reasoning, (e.g. object A will float because it is light and object B will sink because it is heavy ), were classified as causal linear and were credited with zero (0). Answers that used causal linear reasoning in task 5, ( e.g. they both float because they are light ), were classified as causal linear and were credited with zero (0). Additionally, answers that used causal relational reasoning to explain F/S, (e.g. I choose the third picture because both objects have smaller density than the liquid and object B has bigger density than object A ), in task 5, were classified as causal relational and were

credited with one (1). The means of the categorization in the last two tasks were also calculated and rounded for each student. RESULTS Table 1 presents the participants progress in understanding the concept of density as a property of materials. As it was expected, the number of participants that considered density as a property of materials increased significantly after the first and second part of our implementation (interim and post measure), while the number of participants answering irrelevant, phenomenological or incomplete answers decreased significantly from the pre to the post phase of evaluation (Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test, z=4.8, p <.001). Furthermore, the pattern of answers remained unchanged from post to post-post evaluation (Wilcoxon Signed- Rank Test, z=.29, p =.770). Nevertheless, a small though considerable number of participants were still assuming that density is an extensive quantity (category 1). Table1. Frequencies of participants found in each category in each time-point concerning learning of density as a property of materials Tasks 1, 2 and 3 Time-points Categories pre interim post post-post 3: Density as a property of materials (intensive idea) 0 10 13 11 2: Density as a property of materials and dependent on the shape, weight or size of the object (transitional idea) 1 7 4 7 1: Density dependent on the shape, weight or size of the object (extensive idea) 8 13 17 15 0: Phenomenological, incomplete or no answers 32 11 7 8 Total answers 41 41 41 41 The results concerning participants explanations of F/S phenomena are shown in table 2. These results are based on participants answers in Task 4 and 5, which were conducted only in interim, post and post-post phase of evaluation. Table2. Frequencies of participants found in each category in each time-point concerning participants explanations of F/S phenomena Tasks 4 and 5 Time-points Categories interim post post-post 1: Use of density in a causal relational reasoning 17 22 15 0: Use of density in a causal linear reasoning 24 19 26 Total 41 41 41

A considerable number of participants (seventeen out of forty-one) were able to use density in a causal relational reasoning after the first phase of our implementation in the intermediate phase of evaluation (see Table 2). This number was significantly increased in the end of the TLS where almost one out of two participants used density in a causal relational reasoning (Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test, z=2.7, p <.05). However, this improvement lost at the post-post time point, seven months later, and participants started using density in a linear reasoning again (Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test, z=2.4, p <.05). Mann-Whitney non-parametric analysis was used in order to test our hypothesis that cultivating causal relational reasoning in explaining F/S phenomena could lead to better understanding of density as a property of materials. More specifically, we divided our participants in two groups; those who in each time-point understood and used causal relational reasoning and those that used causal linear reasoning in explaining F/S. For each group of participants we calculated the means corresponding to their conceptual understanding of density as a property of materials. As we expected, significantly more participants who used causal relational reasoning, understood density as a property of materials, than participants who used causal linear reasoning in interim ( p =.040), post ( p =.007), and post-post (p =.001) phase of testing. CONCLUSIONS The results showed statistically significant increase of the participants who understood density as a property of materials, which remained stable seven months later (Table 1). Furthermore, there were a considerable number of participants who used density in a causal relational reasoning to explain F/S phenomena (Table 2). In parallel, the number of participants who used causal relational explanations in the post phase of evaluation significantly increased, in comparison with the interim one. One possible explanation for this improvement could be the use of the Sea Diamond s cruise ship simulation, in the 5 th unit. In this unit participants used the Sea Diamond cruise ship simulation in order to investigate the conditions under which a ship sinks and floats, in the frame of the technological problem solving of the salvage of the sunken ship. Nevertheless, this improvement did not remain seven months later. An explanation of that could be the short period given to such activities, that is, only a part of the last unit of the TLS. Last but not least, we noticed that participants who managed to use density in a causal relational reasoning had a better understanding of density as a property of materials, in a statistically significant degree, than those who used density in a causal linear reasoning. We consider that this result is in favor of Perkins & Grotzer (2005) claim, that cultivating complex causal reasoning can enhance participants concepts understanding. ACKNOWLEDGMENT Work presented in this paper has been supported by the EU through the ECRDG in the project Materials Science University-school partnerships for the design and implementation of research-based ICT-enhanced modules on Material Properties, Science and Society Programme, FP6, SAS6-CT -2006-042942.

REFERENCES Fassoulopoulos, G., Kariotoglou, P., & Koumaras, P. (2003). Consistent and inconsistent pupil s reasoning about intensive quantities: the case of density and pressure. Research in Science Education, 33, 71-87. Retrieved from http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/klu/rise/ Hardy, I., Jonen, A., Moeller, K., & Stern, E. (2006). Effects of instructional support within constructivist learning environments for elementary school students' understanding of floating and sinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 307-326. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.307 Kohn, A. S. (1993). Preschooler s reasoning about density: will it float? Child Development 64(6), 1637-1650. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1131460 Méheut, M., & Psillos, D. (2 004). Teaching-learning sequences: aims and tools for science education research. International Journal of Science Education, 26(5), 515-535. doi:10.1080/09500690310001614762 Nersessian, N. (2008). Model-based reasoning in scientific practice. In R. A. Duschl & R. E. Grandy (Eds.), Teaching Scientific Inquiry: Recommendations for Research and Implementation (pp. 57-79). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers. Perkins, D., & Grotzer, T. (2005). Dimensions of causal understanding: the role of complex causal models in students' understanding of science. Studies in Science Education, 41, 117-166. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03057260508560216 Smith, C., Snir, J., & Grosslight, L. (1992). Using conceptual models to facilitate conceptual change: the case of weight-density differentiation, Cognition and Instruction, 9(3), 221-283. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0903_3 Spyrtou, A., Zoupidis, A., & Kariotoglou, P. (2008). The design and development of an ICT- Enhanced Module concerning density as a property of materials applied in floating-sinking phenomena. Ιn: C. P. Constantinou & N. Papadouris (Eds.), GIREP International Conference, Physics Curriculum Design, Development and Validation (pp. 391-407). Selected Papers, ISBN 978-9963-689-20-0. Wiser, M., & Smith, C. (2008). Learning and teaching about matter in grades K -8: when should the atomic-molecular theory be introduced? In S. Vosniadou (Eds.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (pp. 205-239). New York: Routledge.