Sex Differences and Self-Reported Attention Problems During Baseline Concussion Testing

Similar documents
Immediate Post-Concussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing (ImPACT) Normative Data

Neuropsychological Assessment in Sports-Related Concussion: Part of a Complex Puzzle

Concussion: Sideline Assessment

Validity of ImPACT 1. Running Head: VALIDITY OF ImPACT. Validity of ImPACT for Measuring Attention & Processing Speed

Computerized Neurocognitive Testing for the Management of Sport-Related Concussions

Traumatic Brain Injury in Young Athletes. Andrea Halliday, M.D. Oregon Neurosurgery Specialists

Concussion in Athletes: Identification, Evaluation and Educational Considerations Mary Ann Dugan, MSN, CRNP, FNP-BC

Utility of Repeated Assessment After Invalid Baseline Neurocognitive Test Performance

Highlights from the 2008 Consensus Statement on Concussion in Sport

The last decade has witnessed the rapid CONCUSSION IN PROFESSIONAL FOOTBALL: RECOVERY CLINICAL STUDIES

The effect of effort on baseline neuropsychological test scores in high school football athletes

Neuropsychological Assessment in Sports- Related Concussion: Part of a Complex Puzzle

Defined as a complex process affecting the brain, induced by traumatic biomechanical forces.

Get a Heads Up on Concussion in Sports Policies

Recovery From Mild Concussion in High School Athletes Journal of Neurosurgery 2003;98:

Concussion Management Program for Red Bank Catholic High School Athletic Department

Sport Concussion in New Zealand ACC National Guidelines

LIBERTY UNIVERSITY CONCUSSION PROTOCOL Page 1

THE MANAGEMENT OF CONCUSSION IN AUSTRALIAN FOOTBALL

Impaired Executive Function in Concussed Athletes

Revised Bethel Park s Sports Concussion and Closed Head Injury Protocol and Procedures for Student-Athletes

Evaluation of the Zurich Guidelines and Exercise Testing for Return to Play in Adolescents Following Concussion

HEADS UP ONCUSSION IN HIGH SCHOOL SPORTS U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES CENTERS FOR DISEASE CONTROL AND PREVENTION

Clinician Portal: Enabling a Continuity of Concussion Care

In-Office Evaluation of Sports Concussion: Components of Assessment, Treatment and Rehabilitation

Concussion in Children Lecture Summary Sports Medicine Australia

Neuropsychological Assessment of Sport- Related Concussion

Online publication date: 19 May 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Psychometric issues associated with computerised neuropsychological assessment of concussed athletes.

Published online: 17 Jun 2010.

The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

Sports Neuropsychology: Definition, Qualifications, and Training Guidelines

The FacTs: * All concussions are serious. A Fact Sheet for School Nurses

NFL Head, Neck and Spine Committee s Protocols Regarding Diagnosis and Management of Concussion

sports medicine and concussion services Taking Care of Athletes of All Ages, from Recreational to Professional

Michael J. Sileo, MD. Orthopedic Associates of Long Island (OALI)

Sport Concussion in New Zealand:

Concussion Rates per Sport

How To Know If You Have A Concussion

Approximately 300,000 sport-related concussions are SEX DIFFERENCES IN NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL FUNCTION AND POST-CONCUSSION SYMPTOMS CLINICAL STUDIES

A Parent s Guide to Choosing a Sports Concussion Specialist Brain Injury Alliance of New Jersey Concussion in Youth Sports Committee

CURRICULUM VITAE. Work Address: 3334 Capital Medical Blvd. Suite #400 Tallahassee, FL 32308

EYE-TRAC Advance. A research study funded by the Department of Defense

Concussion & Post-Traumatic Headache in the Female Athlete

SOUTHWEST FOOTBALL LEAGUE CONCUSSION MANAGEMENT PROTOCOL

Daring Greatly: How the Courage to Be Vulnerable Transforms the Way We Live, Love, Parent, and Lead. Click for updates

Concussion (Mild Traumatic Brain Injury) and the Team Physician: A Consensus Statement *

Group Versus Individual Administration Affects Baseline Neurocognitive Test Performance

IF IN DOUBT, SIT THEM OUT.

SUMMARY OF THE WHO COLLABORATING CENTRE FOR NEUROTRAUMA TASK FORCE ON MILD TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

Epidemiology of Concussions Among United States High School Athletes in 20 Sports

mtbi Concussion, Post Concussion Syndrome and mtbi

Objectives. Definition. Epidemiology. The journey of an athlete

What is a concussion? What are the symptoms of a concussion? What happens to the brain during a concussion?

A GUIDE TO IN RUGBY UNION

Mild head injury: How mild is it?

A PEEK INSIDE A CLOSED HEAD INJURY CLAIM... 1

Concussion Symptom Inventory (CSI):

INTERNATIONAL RUGBY BOARD Putting players first

Skate Australia Concussion Guidelines

Concussion Management Return to Play Protocol

NASPE Sets the Standard

NEW ZEALAND RUGBY LEAGUE CONCUSSION / HEAD INJURY POLICY

Early Response Concussion Recovery

MYBRAINTEST. Computerized Concussion Testing

Creating an Academic Concussion Management Plan

Concussion Guidance for the General Public

Model Policy and Guidance for Pennsylvania Schools for Sports-Related Concussion/Mild Traumatic Brain Injury

IHSA Sports Medicine Acknowledgement & Consent Form. Concussion Information Sheet

Outline. What is a concussion? A brief history of concussion. Neurometabolic Cascade 4/27/2016

Sport-related concussion: Evaluation and management

CONCUSSION AND HEAD INJURY AWARENESS POLICY TRAINING C I T Y O F S O U T H J O R D A N 9 /

Medical Affairs Policy

Traumatic brain injury (TBI)

California Published online: 09 Jun 2014.

Emerging Frontiers in Concussion:

Functional Brain Abnormalities Are Related to Clinical. Recovery and Time to Return to Play in Athletes

Recovering from a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (MTBI)

Concussion in Sports

This document is a revision to the team physician

Laurel School Sports Medicine Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI)/Concussion Management Document

Facts for Physicians. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

Heads Up. Facts for Physicians. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) may improve the home behavior of children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD).

Concussion Knowledge in High School Football Players. Janie Cournoyer, MSc, ATC, CAT(C); Brady L. Tripp, PhD, LAT, ATC. Key Points

Cognitive Remediation of Brain Injury

continuing Medical Education Specialized Care of the Young Athlete, Attorneys

Sports Concussion Management Plan

Florida State University-Sports Medicine. Concussion and Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (mtbi) Management Plan

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) as Screening tool for cognitive impairment in mtbi.

8 th European Conference on Psychological Assessment

National Football League Head, Neck and Spine Committee s Protocols Regarding Return to Participation Following Concussion

POLICY TO ADDRESS SUSPECTED SPORTS RELATED HEAD INJURY/CONCUSSION

Cognitive Assessment and Rehabilitation in mtbi Patients. Shannon E. Auxier, MS CCC-SLP Judy M. Mikola, PhD CCC-SLP

Brain Injury Litigation. Peter W. Burg Burg Simpson Eldredge Hersh & Jardine, P.C.

Traumatic Brain Injury for VR Counselors Margaret A. Struchen, Ph.D. and Laura M. Ritter, Ph.D., M.P.H.

Coaches Course Syllabus HB 2038 ~ Natasha s Law

The Military Acute Concussion Evaluation (MACE)

Individualized Education Plans

Concussion (Mild Traumatic Brain Injury)

Transcription:

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/275660959 Sex Differences and Self-Reported Attention Problems During Baseline Concussion Testing ARTICLE APRIL 2015 DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2014.1003066 Source: PubMed READS 77 5 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Brian L Brooks The University of Calgary 95 PUBLICATIONS 1,259 CITATIONS Joseph E Atkins Colby College 9 PUBLICATIONS 118 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE Ross Zafonte Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital 237 PUBLICATIONS 4,977 CITATIONS Paul Berkner Colby College 12 PUBLICATIONS 37 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately. Available from: Paul Berkner Retrieved on: 06 January 2016

This article was downloaded by: [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] On: 29 April 2015, At: 09:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Applied Neuropsychology: Child Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapc20 Sex Differences and Self-Reported Attention Problems During Baseline Concussion Testing Brian L. Brooks abc, Grant L. Iverson def, Joseph E. Atkins g, Ross Zafonte hf & Paul D. Berkner i a Neurosciences Program (Brain Injury and Rehabilitation), Alberta Children's Hospital, Calgary, Alberta, Canada b Departments of Pediatrics and Clinical Neurosciences, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada c Alberta Children's Hospital Research Institute, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada d Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Harvard Medical School Click for updates e Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, MassGeneral Hospital for Children Sports Concussion Program f Red Sox Foundation and Massachusetts General Hospital Home Base Program, Boston, Massachusetts g Department of Psychology, Colby College, Waterville, Maine h Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School i Health Services and Department of Biology, Colby College, Waterville, Maine Published online: 29 Apr 2015. To cite this article: Brian L. Brooks, Grant L. Iverson, Joseph E. Atkins, Ross Zafonte & Paul D. Berkner (2015): Sex Differences and Self-Reported Attention Problems During Baseline Concussion Testing, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2014.1003066 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2014.1003066 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD, 0: 1 8, 2015 Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 2162-2965 print=2162-2973 online DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2014.1003066 Sex Differences and Self-Reported Attention Problems During Baseline Concussion Testing Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 Brian L. Brooks Neurosciences Program (Brain Injury and Rehabilitation), Alberta Children s Hospital, and Departments of Pediatrics and Clinical Neurosciences, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, and Alberta Children s Hospital Research Institute, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada Grant L. Iverson Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Harvard Medical School, Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, MassGeneral Hospital for Children Sports Concussion Program, and Red Sox Foundation and Massachusetts General Hospital Home Base Program, Boston, Massachusetts Joseph E. Atkins Department of Psychology, Colby College, Waterville, Maine Ross Zafonte Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Brigham and Women s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, and Red Sox Foundation and Massachusetts General Hospital Home Base Program, Boston, Massachusetts Paul D. Berkner Health Services and Department of Biology, Colby College, Waterville, Maine Amateur athletic programs often use computerized cognitive testing as part of their concussion management programs. There is evidence that athletes with preexisting attention problems will have worse cognitive performance and more symptoms at baseline testing. The purpose of this study was to examine whether attention problems affect assessments differently for male and female athletes. Participants were drawn from a database that included 6,840 adolescents from Maine who completed Immediate Postconcussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing (ImPACT) at baseline (primary outcome measure). The final sample included 249 boys and 100 girls with self-reported attention problems. Each participant was individually matched for sex, age, number of past concussions, and sport to a control participant (249 boys, 100 girls). Boys with attention problems BLB receives funding from a test publisher (Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.), book royalties from Oxford University Press, and in-kind test credits for research from another computerized test publisher (CNS Vital Signs). GLI has received past research funding from several test publishing companies, including ImPACT Applications, Inc., CNS Vital Signs, and Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. He has not received research support from ImPACT Applications, Inc., in the past 3 years. He receives royalties for a neuropsychological test and for books relating to neuropsychological assessment. None of the authors have a financial interest in the ImPACT. Address correspondence to Brian L. Brooks, Ph.D., Neurosciences Program, Alberta Children s Hospital, 2888 Shaganappi Trail NW, Calgary, Alberta, Canada T3B 6A8. E-mail: brian.brooks@albertahealthservices.ca

2 BROOKS ET AL. had worse reaction time than boys without attention problems. Girls with attention problems had worse visual-motor speed than girls without attention problems. Boys with attention problems reported more total symptoms, including more cognitive-sensory and sleep-arousal symptoms, compared with boys without attention problems. Girls with attention problems reported more cognitive-sensory, sleep-arousal, and affective symptoms than girls without attention problems. When considering the assessment, management, and outcome from concussions in adolescent athletes, it is important to consider both sex and preinjury attention problems regarding cognitive test results and symptom reporting. Key words: adolescents, baseline, children, gender, return to play, TBI Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 INTRODUCTION It is estimated that 1 out of every 10 injuries sustained in high school athletic programs is a concussion (Gessel, Fields, Collins, Dick, & Comstock, 2007). More than 700,000 concussions were estimated to have occurred in U.S. high school athletics during a 5-year longitudinal study, with 13% of these injuries being deemed as recurrent (Castile, Collins, McIlvain, & Comstock, 2012). It has been suggested that children and adolescents might take longer to recover from a concussion (Collins, Iverson, Gaetz, Meehan, & Lovell, 2012; Collins, Lovell, Iverson, Ide, & Maroon, 2006; Field, Collins, Lovell, & Maroon, 2003; Guskiewicz & Valovich McLeod, 2011; Makdissi et al., 2013; McClincy, Lovell, Pardini, Collins, & Spore, 2006; Pellman, Lovell, Viano, & Casson, 2006; Sim, Terryberry-Spohr, & Wilson, 2008), and there have been clarion calls for more research with youth athletes (Choe, Babikian, DiFiori, Hovda, & Giza, 2012; McCrory et al., 2009, 2013; Purcell, 2009; Zemek, Farion, Sampson, & McGahern, 2013). During the past few years, every state in the United States has passed legislation relating to the management of concussions in amateur athletics. Despite conflicting opinions in the literature on the pros (Van Kampen, Lovell, Pardini, Collins, & Fu, 2006) and cons (Echemendia et al., 2012; Kirkwood, Randolph, & Yeates, 2012; Randolph, 2011) of computerized cognitive testing, amateur athletic programs continue to use this assessment method as part of their concussion management program, including preseason assessments. There is some evidence, however, that adolescent athletes with a history of attention problems may have different results on baseline computerized testing compared with those athletes without a history of attention problems (Elbin et al., 2013; Zuckerman, Lee, Odom, Solomon, & Sills, 2013). Notably, adolescent athletes with attention problems have worse performance at baseline on measures of memory, visual-motor speed, reaction time, and impulse control, as well as higher endorsement of symptoms compared with athletes without attention problems (Elbin et al., 2013; Zuckerman et al., 2013). A recent consensus statement on the management of sport concussion deemed developmental conditions, such as preinjury attention problems, a modifier that may influence the investigation, management, and potentially the outcome from concussion (McCrory et al., 2013). Considering that a significant minority of adolescent athletes have attention problems (Putukian et al., 2011; White, Harris, & Gibson, 2014), it is important to better understand how this developmental condition may affect the assessment of functional abilities (i.e., cognitive skills, symptom reporting) even in the absence of an injury. Despite recent evidence that cognitive testing and symptom reporting used for concussion management are different in those with attention problems (Elbin et al., 2013; Zuckerman et al., 2013), it is not known whether there are sex differences on preseason testing in those with or without preinjury attention problems. The consensus guidelines suggest that sex may be another potential modifier of concussion management and outcome (McCrory et al., 2013). The goals of this study were to: (a) examine the effects of preinjury attention problems on baseline cognitive performance and symptom reporting, and (b) determine if there are sex differences related to preinjury attention problems and baseline testing. It was hypothesized that boys and girls with preinjury attention problems would perform more poorly on cognitive testing than those without preinjury attention problems and that the effect size differences would be similar (i.e., the magnitude of the difference would not differ between the sexes). In addition, it was hypothesized that girls in both groups would report more symptoms than boys and that boys and girls with preinjury attention problems would report more symptoms than adolescents without preinjury attention problems. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS Participants for this study were derived from a large database of 6,840 adolescent athletes aged 13 to 18 years

Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 old (boys, n ¼ 3,905; girls, n ¼ 2,935) in Maine who completed Immediate Postconcussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing (ImPACT) as part of their baseline evaluations for sport participation in 2010. Because these are baseline evaluations, all athletes were cleared for participation in sport and their data would be considered prior to an injury sustained in the season. Participants were excluded for the following reasons (and were excluded if they were missing data for any of these reasons): (a) if their primary language was not English (n ¼ 327 were identified as speaking another primary language and n ¼ 118 had missing information on language; total excluded n ¼ 445); (b) if they reported a history of neurological problems (e.g., epilepsy=seizures, n ¼ 70; meningitis, n ¼ 27; and brain surgery, n ¼ 8; n ¼ 877 had missing data on neurological problems; total excluded n ¼ 982); and=or (c) if they were flagged by the ImPACT program as having invalid test scores (n ¼ 263). The final sample consisted of 2,860 boys and 2,290 girls who were eligible for inclusion. The ImPACT health survey asked the athlete whether he or she had had problems with ADD=hyperactivity, with the response being either yes or no. It is important to emphasize that responses to this question are not equivalent to identifying those with a confirmed diagnosis of an attention disorder but do apply directly to those who would endorse having a problem with attention. Self-reported attention problems based on this response were present in 249 boys (8.7% of the included sample; 6.4% of the initial total sample before exclusions) and 100 girls (4.4% of the included sample; 3.4% of the initial total sample before exclusions). Control participants from this database were individually matched to the participants with attention problems on sex, age, number of self-reported past concussions, and sport. The ImPACT is a brief computerized screen of cognitive abilities that has been used previously in several studies investigating neurocognitive functioning following concussive injuries (Brooks et al., 2013, 2014; Covassin, Elbin, Larson, & Kontos, 2012; Iverson, Brooks, Collins, & Lovell, 2006; Iverson, Brooks, Lovell, & Collins, 2006; Iverson, Echemendia, Lamarre, Brooks, & Gaetz, 2012; Iverson, Lovell, & Collins, 2003; Maroon et al., 2000; Schatz, 2010; Thomas et al., 2011). The ImPACT includes six tests=modules (i.e., Word Discrimination, Design Memory, Xs and Os, Symbol Match, Color Match, Three Letters) that yield four primary composite scores (i.e., Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Visual-Motor, Reaction Time) and one secondary composite score (i.e., Impulse Control). The ImPACT also contains a 22-item symptom scale, with each symptom being rated from 0 (none) to 6 (severe). Based on factor analysis (Kontos et al., 2012), the Postconcussion Symptom Scale can be divided into four ATTENTION PROBLEMS, SEX, AND BASELINE TESTING 3 subscales: (a) Cognitive-Sensory (e.g., sensitivity to light, difficulty concentrating); (b) Sleep-Arousal (e.g., fatigue, trouble falling asleep); (c) Vestibular-Somatic (e.g., headache, balance problems); and (d) Affective Symptoms (e.g., irritability, sadness). Statistical analyses were completed using IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Statistics for Windows Version 19.0 (IBM Corporation, 2010). Group comparisons (control vs. attention problems) for performance on the four primary ImPACT composite scores (i.e., Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Visual-Motor, and Reaction Time) used multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) with univariate followup group comparisons for each of the domain scores. Group comparisons for Impulse Control, symptom ratings, and subtest scores along with any variable that violated assumptions of the general linear model used nonparametric techniques such as Mann-Whitney U tests. Experiment-wise alpha was set at p.01 for all analyses. Cohen s d effect sizes were also computed (conventional interpretation of effect sizes: d ¼ 0.2, small; d ¼ 0.5, medium; and d ¼ 0.8, large). RESULTS The boys in this study (n ¼ 249 without attention problems, n ¼ 249 with attention problems) were a mean age of 15.9 years (SD ¼ 1.3 years, range ¼ 13 18 years) and had 9.1 mean years of education (SD ¼ 1.4 years) and on average 0.4 previous concussions (SD ¼ 0.8, range ¼ 0 4, median ¼ 0). The most common sports for boys were football (34.5%), soccer (21.7%), and ice hockey (10.4%). For the boys with self-reported attention problems, 34% reported that they were on a medication for this condition, 10% did not list a medication for attention problems but did list a medication for another health problem (e.g., allergies), and 56% had missing data on medications. The girls in this study (n ¼ 100 without attention problems, n ¼ 100 with attention problems) were a mean age of 16.0 years (SD ¼ 1.3 years, range ¼ 13 18 years) and had 9.4 mean years of education (SD ¼ 1.8 years) and on average 0.4 previous concussions (SD ¼ 0.9, range ¼ 0 5, median ¼ 0). The most common sports for girls were soccer (21%), cheerleading (21%), and field hockey (13%). For the girls with self-reported attention problems, 41% reported that they were on a medication for this condition, 12% did not list a medication for attention problems but did list a medication for another health problem, and 47% had missing data on medications. Because of the matching design of this study, there were not significant differences with age, education, prior concussions, or proportions in various sports

4 BROOKS ET AL. Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 between those with and without attention problems for either the boys or girls. There were not significant differences in the use of medications for attention problems between boys and girls, v 2 (1) ¼ 1.29, p ¼.257. Compared with youth without attention problems, boys and girls with attention problems were more likely to have a learning disability, v 2 (1) ¼ 26.08, p <.001 for boys, v 2 (1) ¼ 26.98, p <.001 for girls; to attend a special education class, v 2 (1) ¼ 26.32, p <.001 for boys, v 2 (1) ¼ 15.87, p <.001 for girls; and to repeat a grade, v 2 (1) ¼ 22.26, p <.001 for boys, v 2 (1) ¼ 5.70, p ¼.017 for girls. However, there were no sex differences when comparing proportions of boys and girls with a learning disability attention problems, v 2 (1) ¼ 1.88, p ¼.171; controls, v 2 (1) ¼ 3.02, p ¼.082 or those who attended a special education class attention problems, v 2 (1) ¼ 0.51, p ¼.474; controls, v 2 (1) ¼ 3.44, p ¼.064 although boys with attention problems were more likely to have repeated a grade compared with girls with attention problems, v 2 (1) ¼ 5.26, p ¼.022; controls, v 2 (1) ¼ 2.51, p ¼.113. Cognitive Performance Performance on the ImPACT is presented in Table 1. Comparison of the four primary ImPACT composite scores in boys with or without attention problems was done using a MANOVA. However, Box s M Test was significant (p ¼.034; suggesting the covariance matrices differed significantly across the dependent variables), and Levene s test for equality of error variances was significant for three out of four variables, so group ImPACT Scores comparisons were completed using univariate parametric (analysis of variance [ANOVA] for Visual-Motor Speed) and nonparametric analyses (Mann-Whitney U tests for Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Reaction Time, Impulse Control, and the Postconcussion Scale). Group comparisons revealed that boys with attention problems had significantly worse performance than boys without attention problems on the Reaction Time composite (p ¼.005, Cohen s d ¼ 0.27). The effect size was small. The groups did not differ significantly on the Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Visual-Motor Speed, or Impulse Control composite scores. Girls with and without attention problems were compared on the four primary composite scores using MANOVA followed by univariate ANOVAs. Box s M Test was not significant (p ¼.184), indicating that the covariance matrices did not significantly differ across the dependent variables, and Levene s tests for equality of error variances were nonsignificant for all four variables. There was a significant multivariate effect, Wilks s Lambda ¼.90, F(4, 195) ¼ 5.16, p ¼.001, partial eta squared ¼.096. The univariate ANOVAs revealed significantly worse performance for girls with attention problems on the Processing Speed composite (p <.001, d ¼ 0.57). Symptom Ratings Group comparisons revealed that boys with attention problems reported significantly more baseline symptoms (p <.001, d ¼ 0.26) compared with boys without attention problems. On the four symptom subscales, boys TABLE 1 ImPACT Performance and Symptom Ratings at Baseline in Boys and Girls With or Without Attention Problems Boys Girls No Attention Problems Attention Problems No Attention Problems Attention Problems Mean SD Mean SD p Values Cohen s d Effect Sizes Mean SD Mean SD p Values Cohen s d Effect Sizes Composites Verbal Memory 82.5 9.0 81.0 10.5.170 0.15 84.7 9.6 83.4 9.3.344 0.14 Visual Memory 72.8 13.0 70.2 14.7.067 0.19 71.3 11.8 67.6 12.7.031 0.31 Visual-Motor Speed 35.3 8.0 33.9 8.1.043 0.18 37.8 6.9 33.9 6.4.001 0.57 Reaction Time^ 0.60 0.08 0.62 0.09.004 0.27 0.60 0.09 0.62 0.08.160 0.24 Impulse Control^ 8.1 5.4 9.0 5.5.033 0.17 6.7 4.8 8.7 5.7.033 0.44 Symptoms Total Symptoms^ 4.9 9.0 7.3 10.3 <.001 0.26 7.4 11.6 13.3 15.1.001 0.44 -Cognitive-Sensory Symptoms^ 1.2 2.9 2.3 4.1 <.001 0.31 1.8 3.9 3.5 4.7 <.001 0.40 -Sleep-Arousal Symptoms^ 1.7 3.1 2.6 3.5.001 0.27 2.5 3.6 4.3 5.0.005 0.42 -Vestibular-Somatic Symptoms^ 0.7 2.0 0.8 2.4.675 0.05 1.0 2.4 1.7 3.1.062 0.25 -Affective Symptoms^ 1.0 2.2 1.2 2.6.498 0.08 1.8 3.3 3.5 4.9.001 0.42 Note. Scores marked with a ^ indicate that lower raw scores suggest better performance or fewer symptoms. Bolded p values are interpreted as significant group differences (p.01). By convention, Cohen s d effect sizes are interpreted as follows: d ¼ 0.2, small; d ¼ 0.5, medium; and d ¼ 0.8, large. ImPACT ¼ Immediate Postconcussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing; SD ¼ standard deviation.

ATTENTION PROBLEMS, SEX, AND BASELINE TESTING 5 Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 with attention problems rated more problems with cognitive-sensory (p <.001, d ¼ 0.31) and sleep-arousal symptoms (p ¼.001, d ¼ 0.27) compared with boys without attention problems. They did not differ on vestibular-somatic (p ¼.675, d ¼ 0.05) or affective (p ¼.498, d ¼ 0.08) symptoms. The most commonly reported symptoms for boys with attention problems were difficulty concentrating (38.2%), trouble falling asleep (30.5%), sleeping less than usual (28.5%), and drowsiness (25.3%; see Table 2). When considering the prevalence of elevated symptom reporting in boys (i.e., having a total symptom score of 13 or greater; Lovell et al., 2006), 10.4% of boys without attention problems and 21.7% of boys with attention problems were deemed to meet the criteria, v 2 (1) ¼ 11.68, p <.001. Girls with attention problems reported significantly more baseline symptoms (p <.001, d ¼ 0.44) compared with girls without attention problems. On the four symptom subscales, girls with attention problems rated more problems with cognitive-sensory (p <.001, d ¼ 0.40), sleep-arousal (p ¼.005, d ¼ 0.42), and affective (p ¼.001, d ¼ 0.42) symptoms compared with girls without attention problems. There was not a significant group difference for vestibular-somatic symptoms (p ¼.062, d ¼ 0.25). The most commonly reported symptoms for girls with attention problems were difficulty concentrating (59%), trouble falling asleep (49%), sleeping less than usual (42%), nervousness (39%), headache (35%), fatigue (34%), feeling more emotional (32%), drowsiness (32%), and sadness (25%; see Table 2). When considering the prevalence of elevated symptom reporting in girls (i.e., having a total symptom score of 21 or greater; Lovell et al., 2006), 14% of girls without attention problems and 23% of girls with attention problems were deemed to meet the criteria, v 2 (1) ¼ 2.69, p ¼.101. Sex Differences In the control participants, girls had significantly better performance on the Visual-Motor composite score (p ¼.007, d ¼.32) compared with boys (Box s M test for MANOVA was significant so group comparisons were completed using ANOVA and the Mann-Whitney U Test). Group differences were not found for Verbal Memory (p ¼.040, d ¼ 0.24), Visual Memory (p ¼.287, d ¼ 0.13), Reaction Time (p ¼.960, d ¼ 0.01), or Impulse Control (p ¼.024, d ¼ 0.27). There was no group difference on Total Symptom score (p ¼.057, d ¼ 0.26), but the girls did report more problems with cognitivesensory symptoms (p ¼.03, d ¼ 0.41) compared with boys. There was also a trend for more sleep-arousal TABLE 2 Individual Symptom Reporting by Sex and Presence=Absence of Attention Problems Boys No Attention Problems Attention Problems No Attention Problems Attention Problems Headache 19.7 20.1 24.0 35.0 Nausea 4.4 6.4 8.0 11.0 Vomiting 1.2 2.0 1.0 0.0 Balance Problems 5.2 6.0 4.0 11.0 Dizziness 7.6 9.6 14.0 16.0 Fatigue 20.5 24.5 29.0 34.0 Trouble Falling Asleep 20.9 30.5 24.0 49.0 Sleeping More Than Usual 9.6 10.0 6.0 10.0 Sleeping Less Than Usual 19.7 28.5 33.0 42.0 Drowsiness 19.7 25.3 22.0 32.0 Sensitivity to Light 7.2 9.6 16.0 22.0 Sensitivity to Noise 4.8 10.8 8.0 17.0 Irritability 16.5 20.9 20.0 38.0 Sadness 8.0 10.0 16.0 25.0 Nervousness 18.1 15.7 24.0 39.0 Feeling More Emotional 10.4 10.0 25.0 32.0 Numbness or Tingling 6.0 4.4 1.0 2.0 Feeling Slowed Down 9.6 11.6 8.0 15.0 Feeling Mentally Foggy 8.8 9.2 9.0 14.0 Difficulty Concentrating 16.1 38.2 26.0 59.0 Difficulty Remembering 9.2 15.3 12.0 18.0 Visual Problems 4.8 8.0 9.0 9.0 Elevated symptom reporting 10.4 21.7 14.0 23.0 Girls Note. The percentages of students who endorsed the symptom as present is presented (i.e., score of 1 or greater). Elevated symptom reporting for boys: percent of sample with a total score of 13 or greater. Elevated symptom reporting for girls: percent of sample with a total score of 21 or greater. Source: Lovell et al. (2006).

Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 6 BROOKS ET AL. symptoms in girls (p ¼.05, d ¼ 0.45) and a medium effect size for more affective symptoms in girls (p ¼.126, d ¼ 0.45). In adolescents with attention problems, there was not a significant multivariate effect, Wilks s Lambda ¼.97, F(4, 344) ¼ 2.89, p ¼.022, partial eta squared ¼.096, Box s M test, p ¼.283, for the four primary composite scores and there was no group difference on the Impulse Control composite score (p ¼.675, d ¼ 0.05). Symptom ratings, however, were significantly higher in girls with attention problems than in boys with attention problems (p <.001, d ¼ 0.51). Girls with attention problems reported significantly more problems with cognitive-sensory (p ¼.005, d ¼ 0.28), sleep-arousal (p ¼.005, d ¼ 0.43), vestibular-somatic (p <.001, d ¼ 0.32), and affective (p <.001, d ¼ 0.69) symptoms compared with boys with attention problems. DISCUSSION There is worldwide interest in the assessment, management, and treatment of concussive injuries in amateur athletics. The Concussion in Sport Group, by consensus, has considered both sex and attention problems to be potential modifiers of concussion outcome or management (McCrory et al., 2013). However, little is known about how these modifiers can potentially predict prolonged or persistent problems or how they might influence the assessment and management of youth following a concussion. If attention problems alter baseline (preinjury) assessments of cognition and symptom reporting and these alterations differ for boys and girls, then this could in turn affect the assessment and management of athletes following a concussive injury. The primary goals of this study were to examine how self-reported attention problems impact baseline performance on cognitive testing and symptom reporting and to determine whether the impact was different in adolescent boys versus girls. The present study replicates and supports the existing literature showing that adolescent athletes with attention problems have different baseline performance, on average, compared with those without attention problems (Elbin et al., 2013; Zuckerman et al., 2013). In fact, it has long been known that youth with attention problems have neuropsychological deficits when compared with children and adolescents without this neurodevelopmental disorder (Wasserman & Wasserman, 2012). However, the effect sizes in the present study were smaller than anticipated, especially for boys. Boys with attention problems had lower scores on the ImPACT Reaction Time composite compared with boys without attention problems, but the effect size was small (d ¼ 0.27). Girls with attention problems had lower scores on the ImPACT Visual-Motor Speed composite compared with girls without attention problems, and the effect size for this difference was medium (d ¼ 0.57) and more clinically meaningful. In regards to symptom reporting, girls reported more symptoms than boys, and youth with attention problems reported more symptoms than youth without attention problems. Many studies (Brooks et al., 2014; Dougan, Horswill, & Geffen, 2014; Frommer et al., 2011; Kontos, Dolese, Elbin, Covassin, & Warren, 2011; Kontos et al., 2012), although not all (Covassin, Elbin, Harris, Parker, & Kontos, 2012; Piland, Ferrara, Macciocchi, Broglio, & Gould, 2010; Zuckerman et al., 2012), support sex differences associated with symptom reporting. Examples of symptoms reported more commonly by girls than by boys (both without attention problems) are difficulty concentrating (26% in girls, 16% in boys), sleeping less than usual (33% in girls, 20% in boys), sadness (16% in girls, 8% in boys), and feeling more emotional (25% in girls, 10% in boys). Boys with attention problems reported more symptoms than boys without attention problems in the cognitivesensory and sleep-arousal domains, although the effect sizes were small. Examples of symptoms reported more commonly by boys with attention problems than boys without attention problems were trouble falling asleep (31% in boys with attention problems, 21% in boys without attention problems) and difficulty concentrating (38% in boys with attention problems, 16% in boys without attention problems). Girls with attention problems reported more symptoms than girls without attention problems in the cognitive-sensory, sleep-arousal, and affective domains, with effect sizes that were more clinically meaningful. Examples of symptoms reported more commonly by girls with attention problems than girls without attention problems are trouble falling asleep (49% in girls with attention problems, 24% in girls without attention problems), difficulty concentrating (59% in girls with attention problems, 26% in girls without attention problems), and irritability (38% in girls with attention problems, 20% in girls without attention problems). Although the present study provides similar support as Elbin et al. (2013) and Zuckerman et al. (2013) regarding the impact of attention problems on baseline performance, the sex differences highlight unique and interesting aspects. Elbin et al. only found very small effect sizes on the cognitive composite scores (d ¼ 0.11 0.19) and a small effect size on total symptoms (d ¼ 0.32) in relation to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Cohen s d were calculated based on the data presented in Elbin et al., 2013). Zuckerman et al. (2013) found small-to-medium effect sizes (d ¼ 0.27 0.42) with all cognitive composite scores and total symptom ratings (d ¼ 0.45) in relation to having ADHD (Cohen s d was calculated based on the data presented in Zuckerman et al., 2013). With the boys in

Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 the present study, there were very small to small effect sizes associated with attention problems. In girls, most effect sizes were small, with a medium effect size for visual-motor speed. These results suggest that the effect of attention problems on both cognitive performance and symptom reporting may be more pronounced in girls than in boys. There are some limitations with the present data. First, attention problems were self-reported by the youth who participated in the baseline testing, and there is some evidence that self-reported histories on the ImPACT health survey do not always match up with parental report (McKay, Schneider, Brooks, Mrazik, & Emery, 2014). However, it is important to note that often the self-reported answers during the health survey are the only source of information available for these baseline and rapid screening assessments. Second, the youth were asked if they had problems with ADD= hyperactivity, so endorsement is not diagnostic and does not confirm that a diagnosis has been made. And third, the participants indicated whether they were taking medication for attention problems, but there was no confirmation that the medication was taken prior to the testing. Therefore, it is assumed that medication would be taken as per usual. Sex differences and the effect of attention problems on obtained data may need to be considered when managing a concussion, determining a return to baseline, and guiding return to play (McCrory et al., 2013). Considering the studies to date (Elbin et al., 2013; Zuckerman et al., 2013), the effect sizes of differences between those with attention problems versus controls on the ImPACT have been mostly small and occasionally medium. At present, there are no representative normative data for students with attention problems, and there are no specific guidelines or recommendations for how to interpret cognitive test scores differently in these students. Clinicians and amateur athletic programs need to be aware that having attention problems is associated with lower scores on ImPACT domains (i.e., slower reaction time in boys; slower visual-motor speed in girls) and with more symptoms being endorsed (i.e., cognitive and sleep symptoms for boys; cognitive, sleep, and emotional symptoms for girls). As well, the relation between attention problems and symptom reporting, in particular, appears greater in girls than in boys. These findings might have an impact on the management of athletes following a concussion, particularly when determining if cognitive problems or symptoms have resolved and considering return to play. FUNDING This work was funded in part by the Goldfarb Center for Public Policy and Civic Engagement=Colby College ATTENTION PROBLEMS, SEX, AND BASELINE TESTING 7 and the Bill and Joan Alfond Foundation. RZ was supported in part by the Harvard Integrated Program to Protect and Improve the Health of National Football League Players Association Members. GLI and RZ acknowledge support from the Mooney-Reed Charitable Foundation. REFERENCES Brooks, B. L., McKay, C., Mrazik, M., Barlow, K. M., Meeuwisse, W., & Emery, C. A. (2013). Subjective, but not objective, lingering effects of multiple past concussions in adolescents. Journal of Neurotrauma, 30, 1469 1475. Brooks, B. L., Mrazik, M., Barlow, K. M., McKay, C. D., Meeuwisse, W., & Emery, C. A. (2014). Absence of differences between male and female adolescents with a prior sport concussion. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 29, 257 264. Castile, L., Collins, C. L., McIlvain, N. M., & Comstock, R. D. (2012). The epidemiology of new versus recurrent sports concussions among high school athletes, 2005 2010. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 46, 603 610. Choe, M. C., Babikian, T., DiFiori, J., Hovda, D. A., & Giza, C. C. (2012). A pediatric perspective on concussion pathophysiology. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 24, 689 695. Collins, M. W., Iverson, G. L., Gaetz, M., Meehan, W. P., & Lovell, M. R. (2012). Sport-related concussion. In N. D. Zasler, D. I. Katz, & R. D. Zafonte (Eds.), Brain injury medicine: Principles and practice (2nd ed., pp. 498 516). New York, NY: Demos Medical Publishing. Collins, M., Lovell, M. R., Iverson, G. L., Ide, T., & Maroon, J. (2006). Examining concussion rates and return to play in high school football players wearing newer helmet technology: A three-year prospective cohort study. Neurosurgery, 58, 275 286. Covassin, T., Elbin, R. J., Harris, W., Parker, T., & Kontos, A. (2012). The role of age and sex in symptoms, neurocognitive performance, and postural stability in athletes after concussion. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 1303 1312. Covassin, T., Elbin, R. J., 3rd, Larson, E., & Kontos, A. P. (2012). Sex and age differences in depression and baseline sport-related concussion neurocognitive performance and symptoms. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 22, 98 104. Dougan, B. K., Horswill, M. S., & Geffen, G. M. (2014). Athletes age, sex, and years of education moderate the acute neuropsychological impact of sports-related concussion: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(1), 64 80. Echemendia, R. J., Bruce, J. M., Bailey, C. M., Sanders, J. F., Arnett, P., & Vargas, G. (2012). The utility of post-concussion neuropsychological data in identifying cognitive change following sports-related MTBI in the absence of baseline data. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26, 1077 1091. Elbin, R. J., Kontos, A. P., Kegel, N., Johnson, E., Burkhart, S., & Schatz, P. (2013). Individual and combined effects of LD and ADHD on computerized neurocognitive concussion test performance: Evidence for separate norms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28, 476 484. Field, M., Collins, M. W., Lovell, M. R., & Maroon, J. (2003). Does age play a role in recovery from sports-related concussion? A comparison of high school and collegiate athletes. Journal of Pediatrics, 142, 546 553. Frommer, L. J., Gurka, K. K., Cross, K. M., Ingersoll, C. D., Comstock, R. D., & Saliba, S. A. (2011). Sex differences in concussion symptoms of high school athletes. Journal of Athletic Training, 46, 76 84.

Downloaded by [Education Resource Centre], [Brian L. Brooks] at 09:27 29 April 2015 8 BROOKS ET AL. Gessel, L. M., Fields, S. K., Collins, C. L., Dick, R. W., & Comstock, R. D. (2007). Concussions among United States high school and collegiate athletes. Journal of Athletic Training, 42, 495 503. Guskiewicz, K. M., & Valovich McLeod, T. C. (2011). Pediatric sports-related concussion. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 3, 353 364. IBM Corporation. (2010). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 19.0) [Computer software]. Armonk, NY: Author. Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., Collins, M. W., & Lovell, M. R. (2006). Tracking neuropsychological recovery following concussion in sport. Brain Injury, 20, 245 252. Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., Lovell, M. R., & Collins, M. W. (2006). No cumulative effects for one or two previous concussions. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 72 75. Iverson, G. L., Echemendia, R. J., Lamarre, A. K., Brooks, B. L., & Gaetz, M. B. (2012). Possible lingering effects of multiple past concussions. Rehabilitation Research and Practice, 2012, 316575. Iverson, G. L., Lovell, M. R., & Collins, M. W. (2003). Interpreting change on ImPACT following sport concussion. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 460 467. Kirkwood, M. W., Randolph, C., & Yeates, K. O. (2012). Sportrelated concussion: A call for evidence and perspective amidst the alarms. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 22, 383 384. Kontos, A. P., Dolese, A., Elbin, R. J., Covassin, T., & Warren, B. L. (2011). Relationship of soccer heading to computerized neurocognitive performance and symptoms among female and male youth soccer players. Brain Injury, 25, 1234 1241. Kontos, A. P., Elbin, R. J., Schatz, P., Covassin, T., Henry, L., Pardini, J., & Collins, M. W. (2012). A revised factor structure for the Post-Concussion Symptom Scale: Baseline and postconcussion factors. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 2375 2384. Lovell, M. R., Iverson, G. L., Collins, M. W., Podell, K., Johnston, K. M., Pardini, D.,... Maroon, J. C. (2006). Measurement of symptoms following sports-related concussion: Reliability and normative data for the Post-Concussion Scale. Applied Neuropsychology, 13, 166 174. Makdissi, M., Davis, G., Jordan, B., Patricios, J., Purcell, L., & Putukian, M. (2013). Revisiting the modifiers: How should the evaluation and management of acute concussions differ in specific groups?. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 47, 314 320. Maroon, J. C., Lovell, M. R., Norwig, J., Podell, K., Powell, J. W., & Hartl, R. (2000). Cerebral concussion in athletes: Evaluation and neuropsychological testing. Neurosurgery, 47, 659 672. McClincy, M. P., Lovell, M. R., Pardini, J., Collins, M. W., & Spore, M. K. (2006). Recovery from sports concussion in high school and collegiate athletes. Brain Injury, 20, 33 39. McCrory, P., Meeuwisse, W. H., Aubry, M., Cantu, R. C., Dvorak, J., Echemendia, R. J.,...Davis, G. A. (2013). Consensus statement on concussion in sport: The 4th International Conference on Concussion in Sport, Zurich, November 2012. Journal of Athletic Training, 48, 554 575. McCrory, P., Meeuwisse, W., Johnston, K., Dvorak, J., Aubry, M., Molloy, M., & Cantu, R. (2009). Consensus statement on concussion in sport 3rd International Conference on Concussion in Sport held in Zurich, November 2008. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 19, 185 200. McKay, C. D., Schneider, K. J., Brooks, B. L., Mrazik, M., & Emery, C. A. (2014). Baseline evaluation in youth ice hockey players: Comparing methods for documenting prior concussions, attention problems, and learning difficulties. Journal of Orthopedic and Sports Physical Therapy, 44, 329 335. Pellman, E. J., Lovell, M. R., Viano, D. C., & Casson, I. R. (2006). Concussion in professional football: Recovery of NFL and high school athletes assessed by computerized neuropsychological testing Part 12. Neurosurgery, 58, 263 274. Piland, S. G., Ferrara, M. S., Macciocchi, S. N., Broglio, S. P., & Gould, T. E. (2010). Investigation of baseline self-report concussion symptom scores. Journal of Athletic Training, 45, 273 278. Purcell, L. (2009). What are the most appropriate return-to-play guidelines for concussed child athletes? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(Suppl. 1), i51 i55. Putukian, M., Kreher, J. B., Coppel, D. B., Glazer, J. L., McKeag, D. B., & White, R. D. (2011). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and the athlete: An American Medical Society for Sports Medicine position statement. Clinical Journal of Sports Medicine, 21, 392 401. Randolph, C. (2011). Baseline neuropsychological testing in managing sport-related concussion: Does it modify risk?. Current Sports Medicine Reports, 10, 21 26. Schatz, P. (2010). Long-term test retest reliability of baseline cognitive assessments using ImPACT. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 47 53. Sim, A., Terryberry-Spohr, L., & Wilson, K. R. (2008). Prolonged recovery of memory functioning after mild traumatic brain injury in adolescent athletes. Journal of Neurosurgery, 108, 511 516. Thomas, D. G., Collins, M. W., Saladino, R. A., Frank, V., Raab, J., & Zuckerbraun, N. S. (2011). Identifying neurocognitive deficits in adolescents following concussion. Academic Emergency Medicine, 18, 246 254. Van Kampen, D. A., Lovell, M. R., Pardini, J. E., Collins, M. W., & Fu, F. H. (2006). The value added of neurocognitive testing after sports-related concussion. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 34(10), 1630 1635. Wasserman, T., & Wasserman, L. D. (2012). The sensitivity and specificity of neuropsychological tests in the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 1, 90 99. White, R. D., Harris, G. D., & Gibson, M. E. (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and athletes. Sports Health, 6, 149 156. Zemek, R. L., Farion, K. J., Sampson, M., & McGahern, C. (2013). Prognosticators of persistent symptoms following pediatric concussion: A systematic review. JAMA Pediatrics, 167, 259 265. Zuckerman, S. L., Lee, Y. M., Odom, M. J., Solomon, G. S., & Sills, A. K. (2013). Baseline neurocognitive scores in athletes with attention deficit-spectrum disorders and=or learning disability. Journal of Neurosurgery Pediatrics, 12, 103 109. Zuckerman, S. L., Solomon, G. S., Forbes, J. A., Haase, R. F., Sills, A. K., & Lovell, M. R. (2012). Response to acute concussive injury in soccer players: Is gender a modifying factor? Journal of Neurosurgery Pediatrics, 10, 504 510.