The effects of television advertising on materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness: A review of research

Similar documents
Television Viewing and Consumer Behavior: The Effect of Personality Traits and Demographic Variables on Children s Consumer Socialization.

COMPONENT DESCRIPTION AND RELATED EVIDENCE

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Consumed by Consumer Culture? Advertising s Impact on Children s Materialism and Life Satisfaction S.J. Opree

How do we know what we know?

Chapter 2. Sociological Investigation

MATHEMATICS AS THE CRITICAL FILTER: CURRICULAR EFFECTS ON GENDERED CAREER CHOICES

Three Theories of Individual Behavioral Decision-Making

Chapter 11. Unobtrusive Research. What are the topics appropriate for content analysis? What are the procedures for content analysis?

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

Running head: THE EFFECTS OF EXTRA-CURRICULAR ACTIVITIES

Expectancy Value Theory: Motivating Healthcare Workers

Advertising value of mobile marketing through acceptance among youth in Karachi

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. Here in the Philippines, we believe in the saying of our national hero Dr.

NON-PROBABILITY SAMPLING TECHNIQUES

Written Example for Research Question: How is caffeine consumption associated with memory?

Association Between Variables

GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT

Evaluation: Designs and Approaches

Public Housing and Public Schools: How Do Students Living in NYC Public Housing Fare in School?

Rapid Communication. Who Visits Online Dating Sites? Exploring Some Characteristics of Online Daters

Restaurant Tips 1. Restaurant Tips and Service Quality: A Weak Relationship or Just Weak Measurement? Michael Lynn. School of Hotel Administration

High School Students Attitude towards Learning English Language

Turn Off TV Turn On the Possibilities. TV Turn-Off Week. What is TV Turn-Off Week?

IMPACT OF TELEVISION PROGRAMS AND ADVERTISEMENTS ON SCHOOL GOING ADOLESCENTS: A CASE STUDY OF BAHAWALPUR CITY, PAKISTAN

Chapter 5. Socialization

Teenage Pregnancy and Sexual Health Marketing Strategy November 2009

IMPACT OF TRUST, PRIVACY AND SECURITY IN FACEBOOK INFORMATION SHARING

Comparison of Research Designs Template

Impact of T.V Advertisements on Buying Pattern of Adolescent Girls

Presented by: Deborah Bourne C/O Hope Enterprises Ltd. 25 Burlington Ave., KGN 10, Jamaica W.I

Analysis of Parenting Styles and Interpersonal Relationship among Adolescents

Consumer Behavior. Attitude Formation and Change: High Consumer Effort Part 1

BILINGUALISM AND LANGUAGE ATTITUDES IN NORTHERN SAMI SPEECH COMMUNITIES IN FINLAND PhD thesis Summary

Scholarly research examining children s consumer behavior

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal

KNOWing Tobacco and the Media Deconstructing Tobacco Advertising

CHAPTER FIVE CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Chapter 2 Quantitative, Qualitative, and Mixed Research

Introducing Social Psychology

Child Selection. Overview. Process steps. Objective: A tool for selection of children in World Vision child sponsorship

Teens, Social Media And Body Image:

Barriers & Incentives to Obtaining a Bachelor of Science Degree in Nursing

Case Formulation in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy. What is Case Formulation? Rationale 12/2/2009

Criteria for a Children s Program

REPORT OF THE APA TASK FORCE ON ADVERTISING AND CHILDREN

FAMILY LIFE EDUCATION ACPS Fifth Grade

Theoretical perspectives: Eccles expectancy-value theory Julie Partridge, Robert Brustad and Megan Babkes Stellino

November 2008 / Common Sense Media. Media + Child and Adolescent Health: A Systematic Review

Supply and Demand in the Market for Money: The Liquidity Preference Framework

Infant Behavior and Development

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender,

Topic 1.1.2: Influences on your healthy, active lifestyle

IPDET Module 6: Descriptive, Normative, and Impact Evaluation Designs

Job Design from an Alternative Perspective

Goodheart-Willcox Publisher

Predicting Box Office Success: Do Critical Reviews Really Matter? By: Alec Kennedy Introduction: Information economics looks at the importance of

A Theoretical Framework for Assessing Effects of User Generated Content on a Company s Marketing Outcomes

An Introduction to Path Analysis. nach 3

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS

Understanding healthy childhood sexual development plays a key role in child sexual abuse

RESEARCH METHODS IN PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 381 FALL 2013 T & TH 12:10 p.m. to 1:25 p.m. (GRPS 1) SATURDAY 9/14 & 10/19 (8:00 a.m. to 12:00 noon Tech 101)

Non-personal communication

The Influence of Stressful Life Events of College Students on Subjective Well-Being: The Mediation Effect of the Operational Effectiveness

The Influence of Parent-Child Attachment on Romantic Relationships

Adult Child Ratio in Child Care

WHAT IS A JOURNAL CLUB?

Scottish Parliament Health and Sport Committee s Inquiry into Teenage Pregnancy in Scotland Evidence from CHILDREN 1 ST

Impact of Television Advertisements on Buying Pattern of Adolescent - A Study of Punjab

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 12

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CLASSROOM MANAGEMENT STYLES OF TEACHERS AND THE OCCURRENCE OF DISCIPLINE PROBLEMS IN INTERNATIONAL SCHOOLS BANGKOK, THAILAND

Marketing: Advertising and Sales Promotion

Self-Concept and Self-Esteem in Adolescents

Adolescent Pregnancy and Parenting

Emotional Intelligence Why does it matter?

Living a happy, healthy and satisfying life Tineke de Jonge, Christianne Hupkens, Jan-Willem Bruggink, Statistics Netherlands,

NHS Staff Management and Health Service Quality

Asset 1.6 What are speech, language and communication needs?

Barriers to Parental Involvement in Early Childhood Education Classrooms in Mumbai Slums as Perceived by Parents

Why do people publish weblogs? An online survey of weblog authors in Japan

INTRODUCING LANGUAGE TEACHER COGNITION

Aspirations Index. Scale Description

Organizational Change and Anxiety: Developing an Instrument for the 5Rs Model

Cognitive Therapies. Albert Ellis and Rational-Emotive Therapy Aaron Beck and Cognitive Therapy Cognitive-Behavior Therapy

Chapter 2. Theories of Psychosocial and Cognitive Development

Influence of Gender and Age in Aggressive Dream Content of Spanish Children and Adolescents

Linear Models in STATA and ANOVA

Direct-to-Consumer Prescription Drug Advertising: Understanding Its Consequences

An Automated Test for Telepathy in Connection with s

Impact factors model of internet adoption and use: Taking the college students as an example

Chapter Five Socialization. Human Development: Biology and Society. Social Isolation

Bloomsburg University Midterm and Final Competency Field Evaluation. Task Supervisor (if appropriate) :

The Mozart effect Methods of Scientific Research

1. Ethics in Psychological Research

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley

Multivariate Analysis of Variance. The general purpose of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is to determine

Empirical Methods in Applied Economics

Long-term impact of childhood bereavement

Interview studies. 1 Introduction Applications of interview study designs Outline of the design... 3

Transcription:

Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 The effects of television advertising on materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness: A review of research Moniek Buijzen*, Patti M. Valkenburg Amsterdam School of Communications Research ASCoR, University of Amsterdam, Kloveniersburgwal 48, 1012 CX, Amsterdam 1012, The Netherlands Abstract In this article, we introduce a model on the unintended effects of advertising. This model describes the existing hypotheses about the impact of advertising on (a) materialism, (b) parent child conflict, and (c) unhappiness. The validity of each of these hypotheses was investigated using a vote-counting analysis. Our analyses yielded a small to moderate effect size for the relation between advertising and materialism as well as a small to moderate effect size for the relation between advertising and parent child conflict. However, support for the hypothesized relation between advertising and unhappiness was not found. The outcome of our vote-counting analysis provided several working hypotheses for further research as well as identified possible moderator variables that should be taken into account in future research. D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Advertising; Materialism; Parent child conflict; Unhappiness; Children; Review 1. Introduction Television advertising aimed at children has been the subject of considerable public concern, both in the United States and in Europe. In the United States, policies restricting advertising aimed at children were formed as early as the 1970s. Two decades later, * Corresponding author. Tel.: +31-20-525-2426; fax: +31-20-525-3681. E-mail address: buijzen@pscw.uva.nl (M. Buijzen). 0193-3973/$ see front matter D 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/s0193-3973(03)00072-8

438 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 several European countries implemented rules or regulatory policies concerning advertising directed at children. Belgium for example, issued the Five-minute rule in 1995, a decree which states that children s programs may not be immediately preceded or followed by children s advertisements. In the same period, Greece proclaimed a partial ban on advertising, prohibiting toy advertising between 7 a.m. and 10 p.m. Finally, in 1995, Norway introduced a total ban on television advertising directed at children, a policy also adopted by Sweden in 1996. These restrictions on television advertising directed at children are based on either ethical concerns among parents and policymakers about the unfairness of advertising to children or on beliefs that television advertising has harmful effects on children. Although the possible aversive effects of advertising have been debated since television was introduced, there is still no consensus, however, about what these effects are, and whether they are supported by empirical research. The main aim of this review study is to identify and analyze the existing hypotheses regarding the negative effects of television advertising and to examine the extent to which these hypotheses are supported by empirical research findings. Our literature review focuses on three possible harmful effects of television advertising: the effects on materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness. Studies on the effects of advertising directed toward children are generally based on two paradigms: the paradigm of the empowered child and the paradigm of the vulnerable child. In the empowered child paradigm, children are viewed as skilled consumers capable of critically processing commercial messages. Research based on this paradigm, mostly marketing research, generally focuses on the intended effects of advertising. Intended advertising effects refer to children s brand awareness, brand attitudes, and purchase intentions. Advertising-effects research based on the vulnerable child paradigm assumes that children lack the cognitive skills to protect themselves against advertising messages. It is believed that children, more than adults, are susceptible to the seductive influences of commercials. Research within the vulnerable child paradigm usually investigates the extent to which advertising is harmful for children and addresses issues such as whether children should be protected from these possibly harmful effects. This research focuses on the unintended effects of advertising, for example, on family conflict and materialistic values. In other words, unintended effects are the secondary, usually negative, effects of children s exposure to advertising. Several earlier reviews have discussed the unintended effects of advertising (Atkin, 1980; Kunkel, 2001; Young, 1990). The present review differs from these reviews in two respects. First, unlike earlier reviews, which were narrative in nature, we conducted a votecounting analysis. A vote-counting analysis is a formalized count of the number of studies that either produce or fail to produce statistically significant findings in the hypothesized direction. A vote-counting analysis is considered the most suitable and accepted method of research synthesis when a literature is small and heterogeneous, as in the case of the advertising-effects literature (Bushman, 1994; Cooper, 1990). Second, unlike earlier narrative reviews, our vote-counting analysis provides a comprehensive review of the

M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 439 research literature. Earlier narrative reviews on the unintended effects of advertising have usually discussed a selection of the research, without reporting any criteria on how they selected the empirical studies that were included in their reviews. Nonsystematic literature searches can easily lead to biased samples and, as a result, to biased conclusions (Davies, 2000; Johnson & Eagly, 2000). In preparing this vote-counting analysis, we collected all relevant references from the standard computer-searchable databases (Econlit, ERIC, PsycInfo, Sociological Abstracts, and Social Sciences Citation Index), including studies published up to the fall of 2002. In addition, references from these publications were examined to trace articles, chapters, and unpublished reports that were not recorded in these databases. 1.1. Hypotheses about the relations between advertising, materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness In the past four decades, several hypotheses regarding the impact of advertising on materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness have been put forward in the literature. For reasons of clarity, we have modeled the hypothesized relations between advertising and these three unintended effects in Fig. 1. The model describes all the advertising-effects hypotheses with regard to materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness that have received research attention. 1.1.1. Advertising enhances materialism (path 1) Several authors suggest that advertising stimulates materialistic values in children (Greenberg & Brand, 1993; Liebert, 1986; Pollay, 1986; Wulfemeyer & Mueller, 1992). According to these authors, advertising enhances materialism because it is designed to arouse desires for products that would not otherwise be salient. Advertising propagates the ideology that possessions are important and that desirable qualities, such as beauty, success, Fig. 1. Model for the three unintended effects of advertising. The arrows represent the hypotheses with regard to the effect of advertising on materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness that have received research attention. The dashed square box represents a variable that has been investigated but not explicitly operationalized.

440 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 and happiness can be obtained only by material possessions (Pollay, 1986; Wulfemeyer & Mueller, 1992). 1.1.2. Advertising causes parent child conflict (path 2) A second hypothesis states that exposure to advertising leads to parent child conflict (see path 2; Atkin, 1980; Robertson, 1979). Although it is possible that advertising exposure directly influences parent child conflict, it seems more plausible that this relation between exposure to advertising and parent child conflict is mediated by children s purchase requests and parental denial of these requests. According to this mediational hypothesis, advertising (a) leads to an increased number of requests for advertised products on the part of children, which (b) leads to an increased number of product denials (given that not all requests can be granted), which then (c) causes conflict between parent and child. These mediated relations are represented by paths 2 a, 2 b, and 2 c in Fig. 1. The product denial variable is represented by a dashed square box in Fig. 1. This dashed box indicates that studies investigating the relation between advertising exposure and parent child conflict only implicitly assume that this relation is mediated by parental denial. However, none of these studies have actually operationalized and/or assessed product denial as a mediating variable. 1.1.3. Advertising makes children unhappy (path 3) Finally, several authors argue that advertising may cause unhappiness in children. Most of these authors have investigated a direct relation between exposure to commercials and unhappiness, represented by path 3 a (e.g., Martin & Kennedy, 1993; Richins, 1991). One explanation for this direct relation stems from social-comparison theory. Advertising is assumed to depict a world with beautiful people and desirable products. When children watch television advertising and compare their own situation with this idealized world in the commercials, the discrepancy between the two worlds might cause unhappiness (Atkin, 1980; Richins, 1991). Another explanation for the direct advertising unhappiness relationship has to do with the way in which products and brands are depicted in commercials. By using special camera and editing techniques (e.g., close-ups, moving images of toys that are unable to move by themselves), commercials can create unrealistic expectations regarding the performance and quality of products. It is assumed that young children are not yet capable of seeing these techniques as representations that are not entirely consistent with reality. After the purchase, when the product does not meet the child s expectations, the child becomes frustrated, dissatisfied, and unhappy (Atkin, 1980; Robertson, Rossiter, & Ward, 1985; Ward, Wackman, & Wartella, 1977). Finally, there are hypotheses about one or more mediated paths between advertising exposure and unhappiness. A first mediated hypothesis is that advertising exposure (a) leads to materialism, which (b) leads to unhappiness because materialistic people are assumed to be less happy (path 3 b ). For instance, it has been argued that materialistic people consider objects as an important means to gain happiness (Sirgy, 1998). When the

M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 441 products fail to yield the promised state of happiness, disappointment, and unhappiness will follow (Richins, 1991). A second mediated hypothesis states that parental denial of children s purchase requests causes unhappiness (path 3 c ). It is assumed that greater exposure to advertising causes children to make more purchase requests to their parents. When children do not receive the requested products, they may become disappointed, dissatisfied, and hence, unhappy (Atkin, 1980; Goldberg & Gorn, 1978; Sheikh & Moleski, 1977). 1.2. Hypotheses about developmental differences in unintended advertising effects It is generally assumed that younger children are more susceptible to advertising effects than older children are (Gunter & Furnham, 1998; Young, 1990). It has been shown that children in early childhood (ages two to seven) are more vulnerable to persuasive information, because they have less experience and domain-specific knowledge that they can use while processing commercials (Roedder, 1981). They are, therefore, less able to come up with critical thoughts and counterarguments while watching commercials (Brucks, Armstrong, & Goldberg, 1988). As children mature, they develop greater cognitive skills, which result in a better understanding of the persuasive intent of commercials. Consequently, in middle childhood (ages 8 to 12), children become progressively more critical about, and thereby less susceptible to advertising messages. Although these advertising processing theories suggest that younger children are more susceptible to unintended advertising effects than older children are, no specific hypotheses about developmental differences in advertising-induced materialism and unhappiness have been formulated. However, in the case of parent child conflict, theories of parent child interaction do suggest that younger children more often come into conflict with their parents about advertised products than older children do. Various explanations can be provided to support this hypothesis. First, younger children have been shown to make more advertising-induced purchase requests than older children do, which increases the chance of parent child conflict (Robertson & Rossiter, 1974; Ward & Wackman, 1972; Ward et al., 1977). Second, younger children more often have difficulty delaying gratifications than older children have (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999). If young children see some item as attractive, they focus all their attention on the enticing aspects of this stimulus and find it very difficult to resist, which may also increase the chance of parent child conflict. A final explanation for a stronger effect of advertising on parent child conflict among younger children is children s growing ability to apply sophisticated persuasion techniques on their parents. Research has shown that young children quite often ask, whine, and show anger to persuade their parents. Older children, in contrast, tend to use more sophisticated persuasion techniques, such as negotiation, flattery, and white lies (Williams & Burns, 2000). Such sophisticated persuasion strategies have been shown to lead to less parent child conflict than the persuasion strategies of younger children do (Atkin, 1978; Kuczynski, Kochanska, Radke-Yarrow, & Girnius-Brown, 1987; Mangleburg, 1990; Williams & Burns, 2000).

442 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 2. Empirical evidence: Materialism A total of eight studies examined the direct relation between television advertising and materialism in children (path 1 in Fig. 1): Five correlational studies (Atkin, 1975a, 1975b; Churchill & Moschis, 1979; Moschis & Churchill, 1978; Ward & Wackman, 1971), one causalcorrelational study (Moschis & Moore, 1982), one quasi-experiment (Greenberg & Brand, 1993), and one laboratory experiment (Goldberg & Gorn, 1978). Five of these eight studies were conducted among adolescents and three among children in early and middle childhood. 2.1. Definition and operationalization of materialism Materialism is considered as a preoccupation with money and possessions (Belk, 1985; Rossiter, 1980) and the idea that personal wealth and material possessions are the key to success and well-being (Fournier & Richins, 1991). Most correlational studies on the relation between advertising and materialism have used Ward and Wackman s (1971) operationalization of materialism (Churchill & Moschis, 1979; Moschis & Churchill, 1978; Moschis & Moore, 1982). In these studies, a 4- or 5-item scale was used with items such as It is really true that money can buy happiness, and My dream in life is to be able to own expensive things. In Goldberg and Gorn s (1978) experiment, materialism was operationalized as a preference for toys over friends. 2.2. Correlational studies The specific characteristics (sample size, age of children, control variables, and moderator variables) of the correlational studies on the relation between television advertising and materialism are presented in Table 1. The two right-hand columns in Table 1 present the direction and strength of the observed relations. The coefficients (r, pr, b) are included in the table to give an indication of the effect sizes that have been found. Four studies reported zero-order correlations (Atkin, 1975a, 1975b; Moschis & Churchill, 1978; Moschis & Moore, 1982). In each of these studies, the reported correlations were positive and significant, ranging from r =.13 (Moschis & Churchill, 1978) to r =.32 (Moschis & Moore, 1982). In three out of the four studies, the relation between viewing frequency and materialism was still significant when controls were added for age and sex, socioeconomic status, school performance, ethnicity, parent and peer communication about consumption, and birth order. The significant correlation observed by Moschis and Churchill (1978) disappeared when they simultaneously controlled for nine variables, including family communication about consumption and motivations for watching commercials. Similarly, Ward and Wackman (1971) did not find a significant correlation between television exposure and materialism when controlling for motivation for watching advertising. However, it is questionable whether exposure and motivation variables should be included simultaneously in a regression analysis. It is possible that motivation measures are correlated highly with exposure measures. Highly correlated predictors can lead to serious estimation problems and spurious relations (Lewis-Beck, 1980; Stevens, 1996), and hence may lead to questionable results.

M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 443 2.3. Quasi-experimental and experimental studies Two quasi-experimental studies investigated the relation between advertising exposure and materialism. Greenberg and Brand (1993) studied 782 adolescents and examined differences in materialism between teenagers enrolled in schools receiving the commercial school program Channel One and teenagers attending schools without this program. Teenagers attending schools with Channel One were more materialistic than teenagers from schools without the program, r =.11. 1 Goldberg and Gorn (1978) exposed 231 four- to five-year-old children to a 10-min preschool program. The children were randomly assigned to three groups. The first group watched a program into which two identical toy commercials were edited. A second group was exposed to the same program and commercials on two successive days. A control group saw the program without the commercials. After the television program, all children were shown two photographs. The first photograph showed a boy holding the advertised toy. The second one pictured another boy, who was not holding a toy. The children were told that the boy who was holding the advertised toy was a not-so-nice boy. The children were then asked whether they would like to play with the not-so-nice boy with the toy or with the nice boy without the toy. More than half of the children who were exposed to the commercials chose to play with the not-so-nice boy with the advertised toy, whereas in the control group only 30% of the children chose to play with the not-so-nice boy with the toy. In the same experiment, Goldberg and Gorn (1978) used a second operationalization of materialism. The children were asked whether they would like to play with the advertised toy or with their friends in the sandbox. More than half of the children who had watched the commercials preferred the toy over their friends, whereas only 30% of the children in the control group preferred the toy. However, it is doubtful whether this experiment has really demonstrated that advertising makes children materialistic. It is also conceivable that the attractive toy commercial temporarily aroused children s curiosity, and that it is curiosity (or some other factor) and not materialism that caused the children to choose a toy over a friend. 2.4. Moderator variables With the exception of Atkin (1975b), the majority of studies that investigated developmental differences in the effect of advertising on materialism did not yield significant results (Atkin, 1975a; Moschis & Moore, 1982; Ward & Wackman, 1971; see Table 1). These studies also did not find a moderating effect for sex, SES, or peer communication. 1 Greenberg and Brand (1993) have reported t values for the differences between the two experimental groups (t [825] = 3.1, p <.005). The correlation size reported in the present article was computed with the formula r = M (t 2 /t 2 + df ); see Rosenthal (1991).

444 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 Table 1 Correlational studies on the relation between advertising exposure and materialism Study Ward and Wackman (1971) Age in years 13 15; 15 18 N Measures of exposure 1094 TV viewing frequency Control variables SES, IQ, money, family communication, motivations for watching commercials, magazine readership Moderator variables Direction of relation 0 Age 0 (TV viewing frequency) Younger children Older children Strength of relation a b =.01 ns b =.07 ns Atkin 9 11; 775 TV None and 0 Age + (TV r =.24 (1975a) 11 13 viewing age, sex, 0 Sex viewing pr =.18 frequency SES, school 0 SES frequency) performance Advertising 0 Age + (Advertising r =.22 viewing 0 Sex viewing pr =.13 frequency 0 SES frequency) Atkin 4 6; 738 Saturday None and age, + Age + (Saturday r =.25 (1975b) 6 9; 9 12 morning viewing frequency sex, ethnicity, school performance Moschis and Churchill 12 18 806 TV viewing None and age, sex, SES, family (1978) frequency communication, peer communication, motivations for watching commercials and TV programs, newspaper readership, consumer education Churchill and 12 18 806 TV Age, sex, SES, Moschis viewing family (1979) frequency communication, peer communication, Moschis and Moore (1982) 12 15; 15 18 211 Advertising viewing frequency + Sex morning viewing frequency) + (Only for boys and 6+ children) None None + (TV viewing frequency) 0 (TV viewing frequency) + (TV viewing frequency) birth order None 0 Age Cross-sectional results: + (Advertising viewing frequency) pr =.10 r =.13 b =.07 ns b =.11 r =.32

Table 1 (continued) Study M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 445 Age in years N Measures of exposure Control variables Moderator variables Direction of relation Strength of relation a Idem Idem Idem Idem Age, sex, SES, 0 Age Longitudinal pr =.24 ethnicity, family + Materialism results: pr =.22 communication, at year 0 + (Advertising peer communication, + Family communication viewing frequency; materialism year 0. 0 Peer communication only for adolescents initially low on materialism) + (Advertising viewing frequency; only for adolescents whose parents do not discuss consumption matters) a r = product-moment correlation coefficient; pr = partial correlation coefficient, b = standardized multiple correlation coefficient. In the causal-correlational study of adolescents by Moschis and Moore (1982), two moderator effects were found: The effect for advertising on materialism held only for (a) adolescents who initially scored low on materialism, and (b) adolescents from families who do not discuss consumption matters. The first moderator effect was probably due to a ceiling effect. After all, when children already score high on materialism at the outset of the study, it is less likely that advertising can still contribute to this materialistic attitude. The second moderator effect could be attributed to the mediating role of parents. A series of studies have shown that parents can reduce or even counteract negative television effects (Nathanson, 1999; Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, & Marseille, 1999). Moschis and Moore s (1982) findings demonstrate that this also holds true for advertising effects on materialism. 3. Empirical evidence: Parent child conflict Eleven studies have examined one or more causal paths linking advertising to parent child conflict (see path 2, 2 a, 2 b, and 2 c in Fig. 1): Nine correlational studies (Atkin, 1975a, 1975b; Buijzen & Valkenburg, 2000; Galst & White, 1976; Isler, Popper, & Ward, 1987; Robertson & Rossiter, 1976, 1977; Robertson, Ward, Gatignon, & Klees, 1989; Ward & Wackman, 1972) and two experiments (Goldberg & Gorn, 1978; Stoneman &

446 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 Brody, 1981). 2 All studies on the relation between advertising and parent child conflict were conducted among children 12 years old or younger. 3.1. Definition and operationalization of parent child conflict Parent child conflict has been operationalized in two ways. In correlational research, children or parents have been asked to indicate how often they had a conflict with the other party about product purchases. In experimental research, diary studies, and observational research, parent child conflict has been indicated by children s negative reactions to parental denial of a purchase request. 3.2. Correlational studies Eight studies investigated the relationship between advertising exposure and purchase requests (path 2 a ; Atkin, 1975a, 1975b; Buijzen & Valkenburg, 2000; Galst & White, 1976; Isler et al., 1987; Robertson & Rossiter, 1976, 1977; Robertson et al., 1989), four studies the relation between purchase requests and parent child conflict (path 2 b/c ; Atkin, 1975a, 1975b; Robertson et al., 1989; Ward & Wackman, 1972), and two studies examined the direct relation between exposure to advertising and parent child conflict (path 2; Atkin, 1975a, 1975b). The specific characteristics (sample size, age of children, control variables, and moderator variables) of the correlational studies on the relations between television advertising and parent child conflict are presented in Table 2. The three right-hand columns of the table represent the coefficients of the relations between exposure and purchase requests (path 2 a ), between purchase requests and parent child conflict (path 2 b/c ), and between advertising exposure and parent child conflict (path 2), respectively. The correlational studies between advertising exposure and purchase requests show without exception that advertising exposure is positively related to children s purchase requests. The zero-order correlations vary from r =.18 (Isler et al., 1987) to r =.41 (Atkin, 1975b). In the majority of studies the zero-order correlations were around r =.30. The correlations between purchase requests and parent child conflict (path 2 b/c ) were investigated most often when possible third variables were controlled. These studies yielded partial correlations varying from pr =.25 (Atkin, 1975b) to pr =.72 (Robertson et al., 1989). Finally, the two studies that investigated the direct relation (path 2) between advertising exposure and parent child conflict reported relations of r =.08 and r =.20. 2 Many studies refer to Sheikh and Moleski (1977) to show that advertising enhances parent child conflict. In our opinion, this study does not provide evidence that advertising causes conflict in the family, because (a) no relation has been reported between exposure to advertising and conflict, and (b) no experimental comparison has been made between children who were or were not exposed to advertising. Therefore, the study conducted by Sheikh and Moleski (1977) has not been included in this portion of our review.

Table 2 Correlational studies on the relation between advertising exposure and parent child conflict Study Age in N Control Moderator Investigated relation a years variables variables Advertising exposure purchase requests (path 2 a ) Purchase requests parent child conflict (path 2 b/c ) Advertising exposure parent child conflict (path 2) Atkin 9 11; 506 None and Age r =.32 pr =.46 r =.20 (1975a) 11 13 age, sex, Sex pr =.27 pr =.13 SES, school performance SES No moderator effects Moderator effect sex: boys r =.23, girls r =.03 Atkin 4 6; 738 None and Age r =.41 pr =.25 r =.08 (1975b) 6 9; age, sex, Sex pr =.29 pr =.10 9 12 ethnicity, school performance No moderator effects Robertson and Rossiter (1976) Galst and White (1976) Isler et al. (1987) Buijzen and Valkenburg (2000) Robertson et al. (1989) Ward and Wackman (1972) M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 447 6 11 289 None None r =.24 4 6 41 None None r =.31 3 11 250 None None r =.18 7 12 250 None and age, sex None r =.31, pr =.29 3 10 267 Age, demanding behavior, communicating behavior, independent behavior None b =.25 b =.72 5 12 109 None None r =.18 Moderator effect sex: boys r =.14, girls r =.07 Moderator effect age: 4 6 years pr =.02, 6 9 years pr =.11, 9 11 years pr =.28 a r = product-moment correlation coefficient; pr = partial correlation coefficient, b = standardized multiple correlation coefficient.

448 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 3.3. Experimental studies Two experiments investigated the influence of advertising exposure on purchase requests (path 2 a ; Stoneman & Brody, 1981) and parent child conflict (path 2; Goldberg & Gorn, 1978). Stoneman and Brody (1981) demonstrated that exposure to advertising increased the number of children s purchase requests. This study examined the interaction between 36 mothers and their 3- to 5-year-old preschoolers in the store. First, the mothers and children were shown a cartoon. Half of the mothers and children saw just the cartoon, while the other half saw the cartoon with six commercials for candy and chips edited into it. After exposure to the cartoon, the mothers went shopping with their children. The children who had seen the candy and toy commercials made approximately 50% more purchase requests than children who had not seen the commercial. Moreover, children who had been exposed to the commercials asked for the advertised products twice as often (M = 4.4) as the children who had not been exposed (M = 1.9). Goldberg and Gorn (1978) investigated the effect of advertising on parent child conflict. They showed 231 four- and five-year-olds a television program, either with or without commercials. After exposure to the program, children were asked which product they preferred, a tennis ball or the advertised toy. They were also told that their mother preferred the tennis ball over the advertised toy. Children who had been exposed to the commercial were significantly more often opposed to their mother s wish (45.8%) than children who had not seen the commercial (21.3%). In the same experiment, Goldberg and Gorn (1978) used a second operationalization of conflict. After exposure to the preschoolers program described previously, children were told a story about a boy who asked his father for the advertised toy, but did not receive it. Then the children were presented with pictures representing two hypothetical responses of the boy. The first picture showed a child happily hugging his father, while the second showed a child glumly walking away from his father. The children were asked how they thought the boy in the story would react. The results showed that children who had seen the commercial more often chose the picture of the boy walking away from his father, although the differences between the two experimental groups were not statistically significant. 3.4. Moderator variables The moderating influence of age has been addressed in the correlational studies by Atkin (1975a, 1975b. No moderating effect was found in the first study (Atkin, 1975a). In the second study, the correlation between advertising exposure and parent child conflict applied only to 6- to 9-year-olds and 9- to 11-year-olds, and not to 4- to 6-year-olds. This greater advertising effect on parent child conflict for older rather than younger children was surprising. After all, research on parent child interaction has often found that younger children have difficulty delaying gratifications relatively often (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999), and are less able to apply sophisticated persuasion strategies than older children (Kuczynski

M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 449 et al., 1987), which are both characteristics that easily result in parent child conflict (McNeal, 1992). The moderating effect of gender has also been established in two correlational studies (Atkin, 1975a, 1975b). In both studies, the effect of advertising on parent child conflict held only for boys and not for girls. This moderating influence is consistent with general theories on gender differences in parent child interactions (e.g., Maccoby, 1990). It has been shown that boys are on average less compliant than girls to the requests and demands of their parents (Cowan & Avants, 1988). Boys also rely on forceful or demanding strategies more often when trying to persuade their parents to comply with them, whereas girls are more likely to rely on tact and polite suggestions (Cowan & Avants, 1988). These gender differences can explain why the relation between advertising exposure and parent child conflict holds mainly for boys. 4. Empirical evidence: Unhappiness 4.1. Definition and operationalization of unhappiness Unhappiness can occur as a short-term, temporary emotional state, but it can also be a longer term, enduring state of mind. Long-term unhappiness has been investigated in studies on the direct effect of advertising (path 3 a ), and in studies on the indirect effect of advertising, mediated through materialism (path 3 b ). In these two types of research, unhappiness has been conceptualized as dissatisfaction with one s life situation or with oneself (e.g., Richins, 1991). Short-term unhappiness has been investigated in studies into the consequences of parental denial of children s purchase requests (path 3 c ). In these studies, unhappiness has been operationalized as crying and being sad or disappointed (Atkin, 1975b; Goldberg & Gorn, 1978). 4.2. Evidence of path 3 a : The direct relation between advertising exposure and unhappiness Two types of studies have addressed the direct relation between exposure to advertising and unhappiness. The first type investigated whether people become unhappy because they compare themselves unfavorably with the beautiful world of advertising. This type of research has not been conducted with children in early or middle childhood. The existing research has focused on the impact of beautiful models in print advertising on the selfperceptions of female adolescents (Durkin & Paxton, 2002; Martin & Kennedy, 1993; Stice, Spangler, & Agras, 2001), female graduate students (Richins, 1991), and male graduate students (Gulas & McKeage, 2000). These studies have produced mixed results. Three experiments (Durkin & Paxton, 2002; Gulas & McKeage, 2000; Richins, 1991) found that exposure to print advertisements containing idealized images of physical attractiveness and financial success negatively affected males and females self-perceptions. These findings were corroborated in a longitudinal experiment among 13- to 17-year-old girls (Stice et al., 2001), but the

450 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 advertising exposure effect held only for girls that initially scored high on perceived pressure to be thin and body dissatisfaction. However, in an experiment among 9- to 18- year-old girls, Martin and Kennedy (1993) did not find an effect for advertising on the selfperception of physical attractiveness. All studies were restricted to print advertising; therefore, the question whether television advertising negatively affects children s or teenagers self-perception is still open. The second type of research on the direct relation between advertising and unhappiness has concentrated on the influence of commercials on disappointment with the advertised product after purchase. Only one correlational study has investigated this relation (Robertson et al., 1985). In this survey, among 253 six- to eleven-year-old boys, a small though significant negative relation was found between exposure to advertising and satisfaction with the product (r =.06). However, this finding applied only to the youngest children in the sample. 4.3. Evidence of path 3 b : Does materialism lead to unhappiness? One of the hypotheses modeled in Fig. 1 states that materialism mediates the relation between advertising and unhappiness. Although there is empirical evidence to support the relation between advertising exposure and materialism (path 1), the path between materialism and unhappiness has never been investigated among children. There is, however, evidence for this relation among adults. A meta-analysis on the relation between materialism and life satisfaction has yielded a negative relation of r =.25 (Wright & Larsen, 1993), suggesting that adults who are more materialistic are unhappier than adults who are less materialistic. Although it is conceivable that this finding also applies to children, no empirical evidence exists to confirm this proposition. 4.4. Evidence of path 3 c : Does product denial lead to unhappiness? Two correlational studies have investigated the relation between purchase requests and unhappiness. Atkin (1975b) asked 211 mothers of 3- to 11-year-old children how often their children became unhappy after they had denied a purchase request. He found a significant positive correlation between advertising and unhappiness after denial of requests for cereals (r =.14) and toys (r =.20). Both correlations remained positive when controlling for age, sex, ethnicity, and school performance. A survey by Robertson et al. (1985) among 253 six- to eleven-year-old boys yielded a smaller, though still significant, correlation between advertising exposure and disappointment after denial of a purchase request (r =.08). In Goldberg and Gorn s (1978) experiment, 4- and 5-year-olds were exposed to a preschoolers television program. One group of children watched the program with commercials and a second group watched the same program without commercials. After the program, the children were asked how they thought a hypothetical child would react when a request for the advertised product had been denied. More than half of the children who had been exposed to the commercials thought that the child would be sad, compared to 35% of the control group.

5. Discussion M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 451 The main aim of this vote-counting analysis was to evaluate the current state of the empirical research on three unintended advertising effects among children: materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness. The impact of advertising on children is a controversial issue, which often provokes contradictory opinions. A systematic and integrative review of the empirical research on this topic is important, because such a review enables us to determine the extent to which the prevailing claims and assumptions are supported by empirical research. In this discussion, we will first evaluate the validity of each of the different hypotheses. In the final section, we will make some suggestions for further research. 5.1. The validity of the hypotheses 5.1.1. Advertising enhances materialism The majority of the correlational studies presented in Table 1 showed coefficients that Cohen (1988) would identify as small to moderate effect sizes. Relations between advertising exposure and materialism were established among children in early and middle childhood, as well as adolescents. Most of these studies still showed a positive relation between advertising and materialism when the influence of possible third variables, such as age, sex, socioeconomic status, and school performance was controlled. These correlational findings were confirmed by the experimental studies by Greenberg and Brand (1993) and Goldberg and Gorn (1978). Finally, the causal-correlational study conducted by Moschis and Moore (1982) suggests that the influence of advertising applies only to children who initially scored low on materialism and children from families who do not discuss consumption matters. However, the causal-correlational and experimental studies only investigated adolescents and children in early childhood. There is as yet no evidence for a causal relation among children in middle childhood. Although most research is correlational and does not permit causal conclusions, the weight of evidence is in favor of the hypothesis that advertising makes children materialistic. The causal-correlational and experimental studies that have been conducted are in support of the hypothesis that the causal direction points from advertising to materialism and not the other way around. Therefore, although more research is needed to arrive at decisive conclusions, our vote-counting analysis clearly suggests that exposure to television advertising stimulates materialistic values in children. 5.1.2. Advertising causes parent child conflict All correlational studies on one or more paths between advertising and parent child conflict yielded small to moderate positive correlations. These correlational results were confirmed partially by the experiment by Goldberg and Gorn (1978). The two studies that have investigated moderator variables suggest that the relation between advertising exposure and parent child conflict holds only for children older than six and for boys.

452 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 However, because of the paucity of studies that have investigated moderator variables, these results can, at most, be regarded as an indication for future research. The results of the correlational and experimental studies are in agreement with the hypothesis that advertising enhances parent child conflict. However, the majority of the studies are correlational. The sole experiment that has been conducted focused exclusively on children in early childhood, and resulted in only a partial confirmation of the hypothesis. Therefore, the scientifically conservative conclusion is that advertising enhances parent child conflict, but that decisive evidence is still lacking. 5.1.3. Advertising makes children unhappy Hypotheses on the relation between advertising and unhappiness show that there are four ways by which exposure to advertising can lead to unhappiness: (1) via social comparison, (2) via materialism, (3) by leading to disappointment about the quality of the advertised product, and (4) by leading to sadness after parental denial of purchase requests. With regard to the first two hypotheses, there is either a lack of studies that investigate the hypothesis among children (in the case of materialism), or the empirical evidence does not apply to television advertising (in the case of social comparison). However, there is some evidence to support the latter two hypotheses. The only study on the effect of advertising on unhappiness through disappointment about the advertised product yielded a small positive relation. The three studies on the effects of advertising on unhappiness caused by denial of purchase requests suggest a small to moderate effect size. In summary, the empirical research on the advertising unhappiness relationship is not developed to such an extent that it allows of causal conclusions. Our analysis does suggest, however, that each of the four hypotheses on the advertising unhappiness relationship that we distinguished can be used as a viable working hypothesis in future research. 5.2. Suggestions for further research Traditionally, most studies on the unintended effects of advertising have focused on the direct relation between the independent variable (advertising exposure) and one of the three dependent variables (materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness). None of these studies has explored the mechanisms underlying the relations between advertising exposure and each of the three dependent variables. Most research investigating the unintended advertising effects is characterized by an input output orientation, which has neglected the specific ways in which advertising can lead to materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness. An important aim of our review was to reveal some of these underlying mechanisms (or mediating variables). Our vote-counting review has yielded a number of working hypotheses for future research, and it identified several moderator variables, such as age, gender, socioeconomic status, and parent child communication. In most empirical studies, these moderator variables were

M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 453 included on an exploratory basis, without guidance of specific theories on the nature and direction of the moderating influence of these variables. In our view, a choice of moderator variables can only be valuable when a priori hypotheses are formulated about how and why certain moderators affect the relation between advertising exposure and materialism, parent child conflict, and unhappiness. Although it has not been decisively established how the moderator variables identified in this review influence advertising effects, they may serve as a basis or guideline for future research. Although further research is needed to arrive at decisive conclusions, our review clearly indicates that advertising aimed at children can have certain undesirable consequences for the family, such as increased parent child conflict, and materialistic and disappointed children. Even though most Western countries have tightened their rules and regulations on child-directed advertising, the lion s share of the responsibility of dealing with negative advertising effects is still shouldered by parents, who are usually the first to experience inconvenience as a result of advertising. This review study suggests that parents are able to counteract undesired effects of advertising by interacting with their children about advertising and consumer matters. However, it is advisable that those working in applied fields, such as marketers and advertisers, also take on part of the responsibility of mitigating negative effects. Responsible advertising aimed at children does not only serve an ethical purpose, but also a commercial one. After all, it is progressively acknowledged among child marketers that parental approval is a necessary condition for successful marketing and advertising towards children (Del Vecchio, 1998; Guber & Berry, 1993; McNeal, 1992, 1999). Parents are eventually the ones who will select and buy the advertised products for their children. It is no surprise, therefore, that the most successful children s products in the past decades were those that met the approval of parents (McNeal, 1999). An important way to avoid or reduce negative advertising effects is to prevent advertising being misleading to children. One way to accomplish this is to test a new commercial among children in order to examine whether the product is portrayed accurately and whether the benefits communicated are not overpromised. Another way is to test the commercial among parents to get their opinions about a certain advertising strategy or tactic. Not only can parents indicate how other parents will react to the commercial, but they also have a keen sense of what their children appreciate and understand (McNeal, 1992). In sum, testing marketing and advertising efforts among children and parents can help applied professionals to find ways of making child-directed advertising as effective, responsible, and publicly acceptable as possible. References References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the vote-counting analysis *Atkin, C. K. (1975a). Survey of pre-adolescent s responses to television commercials: The effects of television advertising on children (Report No. 6). East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED116820.

454 M. Buijzen, P.M. Valkenburg / Applied Developmental Psychology 24 (2003) 437 456 *Atkin, C. K. (1975b). Survey of children s and mothers responses to television commercials. The effects of television advertising on children (Report No. 8). East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED123675. Atkin, C. K. (1978). Observation of parent child interaction in supermarket decision-making. Journal of Marketing, 42, 41 45. Atkin, C. K. (1980). Effects of television advertising on children. In E. L. Palmer, & A. Dorr (Eds.), Children and the faces of television: Teaching, violence, selling ( pp. 287 305). New York: Academic Press. Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects of living in the material world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 265 280. Brucks, M., Armstrong, G. M., & Goldberg, M. E. (1988). Children s use of cognitive defenses against television advertising: A cognitive response approach. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 471 482. *Buijzen, M., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2000). The impact of television advertising on children s Christmas wishes. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 44, 456 470. Bushman, B. J. (1994). Vote-counting procedures in meta-analysis. In H. M. Cooper, & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis ( pp. 193 213). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. *Churchill, G. A., & Moschis, G. P. (1979). Television and interpersonal influences on adolescent consumer learning. Journal of Consumer Research, 6, 23 35. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cooper, H. M. (1990). Integrating research: A guide for literature reviews (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cowan, G., & Avants, S. K. (1988). Children s influence strategies: Structure, sex differences, and bilateral mother child influence. Child Development, 59, 1303 1313. Davies, P. (2000). The relevance of systematic reviews to educational policy and practice. Oxford Review of Education, 26, 365 378. Del Vecchio, G. (1998). Creating ever-cool: A marketer s guide to a kid s heart. Gretna, LA: Pelican Publishing. *Durkin, S. J., & Paxton, S. J. (2002). Predictors of vulnerability to reduced body image dissatisfaction and psychological well being in response to exposure to idealized female images in adolescent girls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 995 1005. Fournier, S., & Richins, M. L. (1991). Some theoretical and popular notions concerning materialism. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 141 403. *Galst, J., & White, M. (1976). The unhealthy persuader: The reinforcing value of television and children s purchase influencing attempts at the supermarket. Child Development, 47, 1089 1096. *Goldberg, M. E., & Gorn, G. J. (1978). Some unintended consequences of TV advertising to children. Journal of Consumer Research, 5(1), 22 29. *Greenberg, B. S., & Brand, J. E. (1993). Television news and advertising in schools: The Channel One controversy. Journal of Communication, 43(1), 143 151. Guber, S. S., & Berry, J. (1993). Marketing to and through kids. New York: McGraw-Hill. *Gulas, C. S., & McKeage, K. (2000). Extending social comparison: An examination of the unintended consequences of idealized advertising imagery. Journal of Advertising, 29(2), 17 28. Gunter, B., & Furnham, A. (1998). Children as consumers: A psychological analysis of the young people s market. London: Routledge. *Isler, L., Popper, E. T., & Ward, S. (1987). Children s purchase requests and parental responses: Results from a diary study. Journal of Advertising Research, 27(5), 29 39. Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research. In H. T. Reis, & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology ( pp. 496 528). London: Cambridge University Press. Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children s noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23, 799 806. Kunkel, D. (2001). Children and television advertising. In D. Singer, & J. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of children and the media (pp. 375 394). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lewis-Beck, M. S. (1980). Applied regression: An introduction. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.