English prepositional passive constructions



Similar documents
Non-nominal Which-Relatives

What s in a Lexicon. The Lexicon. Lexicon vs. Dictionary. What kind of Information should a Lexicon contain?

A Beautiful Four Days in Berlin Takafumi Maekawa (Ryukoku University)

Chapter 13, Sections Auxiliary Verbs CSLI Publications

Constraints in Phrase Structure Grammar

Scrambling in German - Extraction into the Mittelfeld

An HPSG analysis of a beautiful two weeks * 1

Paraphrasing controlled English texts

LESSON THIRTEEN STRUCTURAL AMBIGUITY. Structural ambiguity is also referred to as syntactic ambiguity or grammatical ambiguity.

How the Computer Translates. Svetlana Sokolova President and CEO of PROMT, PhD.

Index. 344 Grammar and Language Workbook, Grade 8

Double Genitives in English

I have eaten. The plums that were in the ice box

Outline of today s lecture

Movement and Binding

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006

Rethinking the relationship between transitive and intransitive verbs

COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH. FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr

A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English. with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs

Syntactic Theory. Background and Transformational Grammar. Dr. Dan Flickinger & PD Dr. Valia Kordoni

A Writer s Reference, Seventh Edition Diana Hacker Nancy Sommers

Syntactic and Semantic Differences between Nominal Relative Clauses and Dependent wh-interrogative Clauses

COMPUTATIONAL DATA ANALYSIS FOR SYNTAX

Methodological Issues for Interdisciplinary Research

Chapter 1. Introduction Topic of the dissertation


Sentence Structure/Sentence Types HANDOUT

The finite verb and the clause: IP

English Syntax: An Introduction

Syntactic Theory on Swedish

L130: Chapter 5d. Dr. Shannon Bischoff. Dr. Shannon Bischoff () L130: Chapter 5d 1 / 25

UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI DELL AQUILA CENTRO LINGUISTICO DI ATENEO

Annotation Guidelines for Dutch-English Word Alignment

MATRIX OF STANDARDS AND COMPETENCIES FOR ENGLISH IN GRADES 7 10

Structure of Clauses. March 9, 2004

The compositional semantics of same

INF5820 Natural Language Processing - NLP. H2009 Jan Tore Lønning jtl@ifi.uio.no

Czech Verbs of Communication and the Extraction of their Frames

Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada

Semantics and Generative Grammar. Quantificational DPs, Part 3: Covert Movement vs. Type Shifting 1

Torben Thrane* The Grammars of Seem. Abstract. 1. Introduction

PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR

SYNTAX: THE ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE STRUCTURE

The Passive Voice. Forms and Functions. Noelia Malla García. Complutense University of Madrid Spain

EAP Grammar Competencies Levels 1 6

Introduction to formal semantics -

WRITING A CRITICAL ARTICLE REVIEW

Morphology. Morphology is the study of word formation, of the structure of words. 1. some words can be divided into parts which still have meaning

Introduction to Semantics. A Case Study in Semantic Fieldwork: Modality in Tlingit

Course Description (MA Degree)

CONSTRUCTIONS AND RESULT: ENGLISH PHRASAL VERBS AS ANALYSED IN CONSTRUCTION GRAMMAR ANNA L. OLSON A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF

English Descriptive Grammar

Overview of the TACITUS Project

Structure of the talk. The semantics of event nominalisation. Event nominalisations and verbal arguments 2

Use the Academic Word List vocabulary to make tips on Academic Writing. Use some of the words below to give advice on good academic writing.

Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) Certificate Programs

MARY. V NP NP Subject Formation WANT BILL S

Appendix to Chapter 3 Clitics

Phrase Structure Rules, Tree Rewriting, and other sources of Recursion Structure within the NP

Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent

Syntax: Phrases. 1. The phrase

Francis Y. LIN Alex X. PENG (School of Foreign Languages and Literatures / Beijing Normal University)

Syntax-driven bindings of Spanish clitic pronouns

Language Meaning and Use

Impossible Words? Jerry Fodor and Ernest Lepore

IP PATTERNS OF MOVEMENTS IN VSO TYPOLOGY: THE CASE OF ARABIC

Artificial Intelligence Exam DT2001 / DT2006 Ordinarie tentamen

Appendix B Data Quality Dimensions

THE BACHELOR S DEGREE IN SPANISH

COURSE SYLLABUS ESU 561 ASPECTS OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE. Fall 2014

How do I understand standard and inverted word order in sentences?

Application of Natural Language Interface to a Machine Translation Problem

How To Understand A Sentence In A Syntactic Analysis

Register Differences between Prefabs in Native and EFL English

Interactive Dynamic Information Extraction

An Approach to Handle Idioms and Phrasal Verbs in English-Tamil Machine Translation System

How To Write A Task For Ielts

Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar

Albert Pye and Ravensmere Schools Grammar Curriculum

Constituency. The basic units of sentence structure

Parts of Speech. Skills Team, University of Hull

Phase 2 of the D4 Project. Helmut Schmid and Sabine Schulte im Walde

English Grammar Passive Voice and Other Items

Mixed Sentence Structure Problem: Double Verb Error

Introduction. 1.1 Kinds and generalizations

KSE Comp. support for the writing process 2 1

Oxford Dictionary of Current Idiomatic English

Advanced Grammar in Use

University of Lisbon Department of Informatics. A Computational Grammar for Deep Linguistic Processing of Portuguese: LXGram, version A.4.

KEY CONCEPTS IN TRANSFORMATIONAL GENERATIVE GRAMMAR

SENTENCE FRAGMENTS. 1. Complete sentences

Slot Grammars. Michael C. McCord. Computer Science Department University of Kentucky Lexington, Kentucky 40506

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation

Online Tutoring System For Essay Writing

Complex Predications in Argument Structure Alternations

Frames and Commonsense. Winston, Chapter 10

PS I TAM-TAM Aspect [20/11/09] 1

Reaching Up and Reaching Out: Preposition Processing Hollis Thomann, Linguistics, College of Science, Advisor: Dr. Heather Littlefield

CINTIL-PropBank. CINTIL-PropBank Sub-corpus id Sentences Tokens Domain Sentences for regression atsts 779 5,654 Test

[Refer Slide Time: 05:10]

Transcription:

English prepositional constructions An empirical overview of the properties of English prepositional s is presented, followed by a discussion of formal approaches to the analysis of the various types of prepositional s in HPSG. While a lexical treatment is available, the significant number of technical and conceptual difficulties encountered point to an alternative approach relying on constructional constraints. 1 Syntactic and non-syntactic constraints In addition to the ordinary alternation involving transitive verbs (1a), English allows prepositional s (sometimes referred to as pseudos ), where the subject in the structure corresponds to the object of a preposition in the related active structure (1b c). (1) a. Kim planted the tree. The tree was planted by Kim. b. Kim looked after the tree. The tree was looked after by Kim. c. Kim sat under the tree. The tree was sat under by Kim. As noted by Huddleston and Pullum (00, p. 1433), prepositional s can be divided into two classes, depending on the syntactic function of the PP. In Type I prepositional s, the PP is a complement whose prepositional head is idiomatically selected by the verb, as in (1b); in Type II prepositional s as in (1c), the preposition is not part of a verbal idiom. Huddleston and Pullum, suggest that the availability of Type I prepositional s is ultimately an idiosyncratic lexical property that must be indicated in the dictionary entries of verbal idioms (although this is rarely the case). Type II s, on the other hand are subject to primarily pragmatic constraints. The linguistic literature on prepositional s confirms this basic description, while offering a more complex picture of the kinds of constraints involved. It is clear that the prepositional is much more restricted than the ordinary, which applies quite systematically to all transitive verbs, with a handful of lexical exceptions. Whether a given verb + PP combination will give rise to an acceptable prepositional depends on various, poorly understood syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic factors. Context, usage and frequency effects, and lexical idiosyncrasies also play a crucial role. Previous accounts of the phenomenon rely on notions like affectedness or role prominence of the subject (Riddle and Sheintuch, 1983; Bolinger, 1977, 1978). These proposals are intuitively appealing, but it remains unclear how they can be satisfactorily formalized. Many authors argue that a high degree of cohesion between the verb and the stranded preposition is a necessary condition for the well-formedness of the prepositional. One version of this approach suggests that V and P are in fact reanalyzed as a complex predicate (e.g., Hornstein and Weinberg, 1981). The fact that V and P typically appear immediately adjacent to one another is taken as evidence for reanalysis. The well-known exception that certain idiomatic direct objects can intervene between V and P in the prepositional () is not necessarily problematic, nor are the examples of phrasal verbs in (3). () Kim made a fool of / kept tabs on Sandy. Sandy was made a fool of / kept tabs on. (3) Kim put up with / looked down on / got rid of Sandy. Sandy was put up with / looked down on / gotten rid of. The possibility of other kinds of intervening elements, however, does call the reanalysis hypothesis into question. Some marginally acceptable examples of non-idiomatic direct objects can be found in the literature (4), and modifiers and specifiers can also appear between V and P with varying degrees of acceptability (5): 1 (4) a.?to be whispered such dirty innuendoes about was enough to break any girl s heart. b.?this fork has been eaten spaghetti with. 1 Example (4a) is from Bolinger (1977). Example (4b) is from Davison (1980), who treats it as ungrammatical, while acknowledging that at least one informant accepts it (p. 49). 1

c.?i have never been knit a sweater for in my life. (5) The bridge was sailed right under / walked completely across. The contrasts illustrated in (6) also shed some light on the nature of the relevant constraint: (6) a. This bed was once napped in by Charlemagne. /??This bed was once taken a nap in by Charlemagne. b. This sofa was once sat on by Hadrian. / *This sofa was once had a seat on by Hadrian. The highly cohesive light verb constructions take a nap and have a seat might be expected to allow reanalysis in the same way as () above, but the is in fact very bad, compared to the versions with single verb synonyms. Examples like () and (4) show that there is no strict constraint against the appearance of a direct object in the prepositional, and that V and P are not required to be adjacent. In fact, if a direct object is involved, then it must intervene between V and P. Any attempt to extract or extrapose this NP results in total ungrammaticality: (7) a. *How much of a fool was Sandy made of? b. *I have never been knit for such an amazing technicolor dream sweater. Lexical approaches to passivization Early generative analyses treated the ordinary formally as a transformation applying to the complete syntactic structure of an active sentence. In non-transformational approaches, with richer lexical representations, the can be analyzed as a lexical process involving only the verb, and no actual syntactic structure. A verb whose basic (active) subcategorization frame is transitive can systematically give rise to a verb with the appropriate demotion and promotion of the (as yet unrealized) subject and object. In HPSG, there are several ways of implementing this idea, the most familiar being the lexical rule approach. (8) HEAD ARG-ST PHON VFORM base MORPH NP i, NP j acc 1 HEAD ARG-ST PSP VFORM NP j 1 (PP i by) This (simplified) rule constructs a lexical entry, given a base verb that selects a direct object (accusative NP as second element of the ARGUMENT-STRUCTURE list). The output lexical entry has the appropriate morphophonological form (past participle), it is identified as (for external selection, e.g. by the auxiliaries be, get), and it has a new ARG-ST list with the original elements permuted just as required. The rule in (8) does not mention the semantic content of the verb, which is therefore assumed to remain unchanged. The verbal relation in both Kim likes Sandy and Sandy is liked by Kim is like(k, s). Only the syntactic configuration of the two arguments is different. This kind of analysis has been standard in HPSG since Pollard and Sag (1987). It can be extended to Type I prepositional s, in which the preposition is lexically selected by the verb (via PFORM selection). For an underspecification-based account of the alternation, see?.

(9) HEAD VFORM base ARG-ST NP i, 1 (NPcanon), PP j pform 3 PHON 4 MORPH PSP 4 HEAD VFORM ARG-ST NP j, 1, P PFORM 3 (PP i by) COMPS NP j The construction of the ARG-ST list is more complicated in this case, because of the stranded preposition. Whereas the active verb selects a saturated PP argument, the verb selects a COMPSunsaturated prepositional argument. The rule allows an intervening direct object, specified as canonical to account for the data in (), (4), and (7). 3 Like the original lexical rule (8), this rule assumes that the semantics of the verb remains unchanged. It should be noted that this analysis requires a further assumption that the preposition in Type I prepositional s is semantically empty, cf. the treatment of case-marking prepositions in Pollard and Sag (1994). This makes the index of the prepositional object j visible on the verb s ARG- ST list and available for semantic role assignment in the verbal relation. For example, Kim looks after Sandy (and its version Sandy is looked after by Kim) expresses a single semantic relation lookafter(k, s), rather than some conjunction of a look relation and an after relation. This analysis seems correct for this example, but in general the possibility of a preposition being both syntactically selected via PFORM and contributing its own semantics cannot be excluded (Tseng, 001), and such cases are potentially problematic for the rule in (9) (see below). An undesirable consequence of adopting the case-marking analysis for syntactically selected prepositions is that the stranded preposition in the output of rule (9) still has nominal semantics, corresponding to the content of its unrealized complement. This means that it is subject to binding principles. Given the coindexation indicated in (9), we must conclude that the preposition is technically reflexive, by Principle A. Alternatively, the stranded preposition could be assigned an expletive index instead (no longer coreferent with the subject). Neither of these options appears to have any empirical motivation, since the preposition itself is not a nominal element. 4 Turning now to Type II prepositional s, where the preposition is not selected idiomatically by the verb, the lexical approach runs into problems. There are two cases to consider. If the PP is a complement (e.g., the directional complement of a verb of motion), then the prepositional involves a reconfiguration of the ARG-ST list along the same lines as (9), but complicated by the fact that the preposition is semantically contentful. In Kim drove past the monument, for example, there must be a drive relation and a past relation, and the monument does not receive a semantic role directly from the verb. Assuming the same semantics for the sentence The monument was driven past by Kim, we have a problem because the verb driven selects a referential subject, but assigns it no semantic role a violation of the theta criterion, or in HPSG, the Raising Principle. A verb assigns no role to a referential argument if and only if this argument is inherited (raised) from another element on the verb s ARG-ST list. To satisfy this principle, the lexical rule for Type II prepositional s (involving complement PPs) must be formulated in terms of raising of the stranded preposition s complement to subject position (that is, synsem-sharing rather than just coindexation, as in (9)). A more unified analysis might be achieved by modifying (9) to analyze Type I prepositional s as instances of raising as well, but this would technically violate the Raising Principle in a different way, because in Type I examples, the verb does assign a role to the subject. This NP does not receive 3 The phrasal verb examples in (3) are not accommodated in this simplified formulation. 4 A third possibility would be to treat the preposition as intransitive, like a phrasal verb particle, but this is problematic for forms like of and for that are never used intransitively elsewhere. 3

an additional role from the case-marking preposition, however, so the Raising Principle could perhaps be reformulated to permit this configuration. An additional complication introduced by the raising analysis is the nominative vs accusative case mismatch between subject position and prepositional object position. This problem could be solved by assuming that, if an argument appears on more than one ARG-ST list, case assignment principles apply only to the highest occurrence (Przepiórkowski, 1999). 3 A constructional approach Type II prepositional s involving PP adjuncts, such as The tree was sat under (by Kim), present serious difficulties for a lexical account (e.g., by lexical rule). In principle, adjuncts are not selected by the verb and are not visible in the lexical description of the verb. It would seem impossible, at first sight, to derive a lexical entry for the verb sat starting from the intransitive verb sit, since the subject of sat originates in an inaccessible PP modifier. A technical solution is available, in the form of the DEPENDENTS list, or extended argument structure, of Bouma et al. (1998). This attribute was introduced to allow lexical heads to impose constraints on their adjuncts, by treating adjuncts effectively as syntactically (but not semantically) selected complements. This move has been controversial within HPSG (see Levine 003, and the response by Sag 005), and could be challenged from a conceptual point of view for abandoning conventional notions of selection and argument structure, making too much information accessible at the lexical level. If we accept this adjuncts-as-complements analysis, the lexical rule approach sketched in the previous section can be straightforwardly extended to all Type II prepositional s. The Raising Principle would have to be modified to apply to the DEPS list, rather than to ARG-ST, since the subject otherwise receives no semantic role from the verb or from any of the verb s arguments. This is an apparently minor change, but one whose effects would need to be checked, and which would no doubt be objectionable to some on conceptual grounds. The remainder of the paper is therefore devoted to an alternative analysis of adjunct-based prepositional s as instances of a special construction, adjunct-prep--cx. The relevant constraint is responsible for licensing the VP consisting of the participle, the stranded preposition, and any intervening elements (certain direct objects, phrasal verb particles, specifiers of P). (10) adj-prep-pass-cx VFORM NP 1 SUBJ COMPS (PP par) VFORM psp SUBJ NP P MOD 3 3 V COMPS NP 1 COMPS SLASH { } The first thing to notice is that the verb is actually an active past participle (psp), not a verb form. Morphologically, English past participles and and participles are identical in form, and they have the same semantic content (linked in different ways to the syntactic arguments). Type II prepositional s can involve intransitive verbs like go that never participate in the ordinary ; on the other hand, all verbs have a past participle form. Using this form also sidesteps the problem of constructing a participle that would violate the theta-criterion/hpsg Raising Principle. The COMPS and SLASH values of this V daughter are empty, ensuring that the direct object (if any) is realized canonically. 5 5 Additional constraints need to be incorporated to block the realization of other kinds of complements, like PPs, but more empirical work needs to be done to reveal the nature of these constraints. 4

The other daughter of the construction is specified to be a COMPS-unsaturated prepositional projection (possibly including modifiers or a specifier) that modifies the verb. At the constructional level, the semantic indices of the verb s unrealized subject and of the preposition s unrealized complement are used to construct the valence requirements of the entire construction (note the value of VFORM). The resulting phrase is a VP that can appear in all contexts and be coordinated with other VPs (here, a Type I and an ordinary ): (11) The birthday cake was sat on, set fire to, and thrown away by Kim. It is worth considering extending this constructional approach to the other types of prepositional s, involving complement PPs, since the lexical treatment of those structures is not wholly unproblematic, and the common properties of all three types of prepositional s studied in this paper can be captured with a small constructional hierarchy. The non-syntactic factors that determine the wellformedness of the prepositional (context, modality, pragmatic and stylistic effects) are also more appropriately accounted for at the constructional level than in the lexical entry of the verb. At the same time, of course, the constructional contraints can also be made sensitive to lexical properties. References Bolinger, D. (1977). Transitivity and spatiality: The of prepositional verbs. In A. Makkai, V. B. Makkai, and L. Heilmann, editors, Linguistics at the Crossroads, pages 57 78. Jupiter Press, Lake Bluff, IL. Bolinger, D. (1978). Passive and transitivity again. Forum Linguisticum, 3, 5 8. Bouma, G., Malouf, R., and Sag, I. (1998). A unified theory of complement, adjunct, and subject extraction. In G. Bouma, G.-J. M. Kruijff, and R. T. Oehrle, editors, Proceedings of the Joint Conference on Formal Grammar, Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, and Categorial Grammar, pages 83 97, Saarbrücken. Davison, A. (1980). Peculiar s. Language, 56, 4 66. Hornstein, N. and Weinberg, A. (1981). Case theory and preposition stranding. Linguistic Inquiry, 1, 55 91. Huddleston, R. and Pullum, G. K. (00). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Levine, R. D. (003). Adjunct valents: cumulative scoping adverbial constructions and impossible descriptions. In J.-B. Kim and S. Wechsler, editors, Proceedings of the 9th International HPSG Conference, pages 09 3. CSLI Publications, Stanford, CA. Pollard, C. and Sag, I. A. (1987). Information-Based Syntax and Semantics, Volume 1: Fundamentals. CSLI Publications, Stanford, CA. Pollard, C. and Sag, I. A. (1994). Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar. CSLI Publications, Stanford, CA. Distributed by University of Chicago Press. Przepiórkowski, A. (1999). Case Assignment and the Complement-Adjunct Dichotomy: A Non- Configurational Constraint-Based Approach. Ph.D. thesis, Universität Tübingen. Riddle, E. and Sheintuch, G. (1983). A functional analysis of pseudo-s. Linguistics and Philosophy, 6, 57 563. Sag, I. A. (005). Adverb extraction and coordination: a reply to Levine. In S. Müller, editor, Proceedings of the 1th International Conference on HPSG, pages 3 34. CSLI Publications, Stanford, CA. Tseng, J. (001). The Representation and Selection of Prepositions. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh. 5