Verb-cluster variations: A Harmonic Grammar analysis



Similar documents
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. At the completion of this study there are many people that I need to thank. Foremost of

Continuous Acceptability, Categorical Grammaticality, and Experimental Syntax

Evaluating Competition-based Models of Word Order

Syntactic Theory. Background and Transformational Grammar. Dr. Dan Flickinger & PD Dr. Valia Kordoni

Simple maths for keywords

A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English. with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs

Testing Data-Driven Learning Algorithms for PoS Tagging of Icelandic

The finite verb and the clause: IP

Chapter 5. Phrase-based models. Statistical Machine Translation

African American English-Speaking Children's Comprehension of Past Tense: Evidence from a Grammaticality Judgment Task Abstract

Points of Interference in Learning English as a Second Language

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics

The Michigan State University - Certificate of English Language Proficiency (MSU- CELP)

Non-Parametric Tests (I)

Doctoral School of Historical Sciences Dr. Székely Gábor professor Program of Assyiriology Dr. Dezső Tamás habilitate docent

Real-Time Identification of MWE Candidates in Databases from the BNC and the Web

Stefan Engelberg (IDS Mannheim), Workshop Corpora in Lexical Research, Bucharest, Nov [Folie 1]

PSC-CUNY Research Awards (Traditional A)

Scandinavian Dialect Syntax Transnational collaboration, data collection, and resource development

English for Academic Skills Independence [EASI]

Differences in linguistic and discourse features of narrative writing performance. Dr. Bilal Genç 1 Dr. Kağan Büyükkarcı 2 Ali Göksu 3

Guidelines for Masters / Magister / MA Theses

The Effect on Housing Prices of Changes in Mortgage Rates and Taxes

The Michigan State University - Certificate of English Language Proficiency (MSU-CELP)

Comma checking in Danish Daniel Hardt Copenhagen Business School & Villanova University

Search and Information Retrieval

German Language Support Package

The structure of the English Sentence

Complex Predications in Argument Structure Alternations

31 Case Studies: Java Natural Language Tools Available on the Web

Research Assistant in the Research Group: Diversity and Inclusion, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Potsdam.

Modalverben Theorie. learning target. rules. Aim of this section is to learn how to use modal verbs.

Berlin-Brandenburg Academy of sciences and humanities (BBAW) resources / services

FOR TEACHERS ONLY The University of the State of New York

Vernacular Language Loyalty and Social Network

AP WORLD LANGUAGE AND CULTURE EXAMS 2012 SCORING GUIDELINES

How the Computer Translates. Svetlana Sokolova President and CEO of PROMT, PhD.

SYNTAX: THE ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE STRUCTURE

Stefan Engelberg (IDS Mannheim), Workshop Corpora in Lexical Research, Bucharest, Nov [Folie 1]

Scrambling in German - Extraction into the Mittelfeld

Least-Squares Intersection of Lines

Online Appendix to Stochastic Imitative Game Dynamics with Committed Agents

CS 533: Natural Language. Word Prediction

Two Correlated Proportions (McNemar Test)

Negation and (finite) verb placement in a Polish-German bilingual child. Christine Dimroth, MPI, Nijmegen

Frequency Response of Filters

Statistical Machine Translation: IBM Models 1 and 2

MARY. V NP NP Subject Formation WANT BILL S

A User-Based Usability Assessment of Raw Machine Translated Technical Instructions

Pronouns: A case of production-before-comprehension

Why are thesis proposals necessary? The Purpose of having thesis proposals is threefold. First, it is to ensure that you are prepared to undertake the

TS3: an Improved Version of the Bilingual Concordancer TransSearch

The last three chapters introduced three major proof techniques: direct,

LEJ Langenscheidt Berlin München Wien Zürich New York

Analysis of Data. Organizing Data Files in SPSS. Descriptive Statistics

Constraints in Phrase Structure Grammar

Multilingual and mixed-lingual TTS applications

CINTIL-PropBank. CINTIL-PropBank Sub-corpus id Sentences Tokens Domain Sentences for regression atsts 779 5,654 Test

Feifei Ye, PhD Assistant Professor School of Education University of Pittsburgh

Sentence Blocks. Sentence Focus Activity. Contents

Statistical Machine Translation

AP GERMAN LANGUAGE AND CULTURE EXAM 2015 SCORING GUIDELINES

INTERMEDIATE STUDENT S BOOK B1+ Adrian Doff, Craig Thaine Herbert Puchta, Jeff Stranks, Peter Lewis-Jones with Rachel Godfrey and Gareth Davies

Genetic Algorithms commonly used selection, replacement, and variation operators Fernando Lobo University of Algarve

MATRIX OF STANDARDS AND COMPETENCIES FOR ENGLISH IN GRADES 7 10

Structure of Clauses. March 9, 2004

Linguistic Universals

Accelerated Professional Program (APP) Absolute Beginner Varies (Typically 50+ units)

Facebook Friend Suggestion Eytan Daniyalzade and Tim Lipus

Programming Risk Assessment Models for Online Security Evaluation Systems

Extended Projections of Adjectives and Comparative Deletion

Definition 8.1 Two inequalities are equivalent if they have the same solution set. Add or Subtract the same value on both sides of the inequality.

Inglés IV (B-2008) Prof. Argenis A. Zapata

bound Pronouns

Syntax: Phrases. 1. The phrase

Vectors. Objectives. Assessment. Assessment. Equations. Physics terms 5/15/14. State the definition and give examples of vector and scalar variables.

English auxiliary verbs

GCE EXAMINERS' REPORTS

WHY DO TEACHERS OF ENGLISH HAVE TO KNOW GRAMMAR?

International Statistical Institute, 56th Session, 2007: Phil Everson

International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research, Volume 4, Issue 11, November ISSN

SmartFocus Article 1 - Technical approach

The Effects of Lag Time on Interpreter Errors

Continued Fractions and the Euclidean Algorithm

PÁZMÁNY PÉTER KATOLIKUS EGYETEM BÖLCSÉSZETTUDOMÁNYI KAR

GCE German (2660) Candidate Exemplar Work: Additional Candidate Exemplar Work Unit 1 (Autumn 2008)

What Makes a Good Online Dictionary? Empirical Insights from an Interdisciplinary Research Project

POWER SYSTEM HARMONICS. A Reference Guide to Causes, Effects and Corrective Measures AN ALLEN-BRADLEY SERIES OF ISSUES AND ANSWERS

Annotation Guidelines for Dutch-English Word Alignment

Introduction to Semantics. A Case Study in Semantic Fieldwork: Modality in Tlingit

A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z. Letter

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

Discourse Markers in English Writing

2-3 Automatic Construction Technology for Parallel Corpora

Education and Assessment Regulations Language and Communication Research Master s Programme Tilburg University

: : Solutions to Additional Bonding Problems

ANALYSIS OF LEXICO-SYNTACTIC PATTERNS FOR ANTONYM PAIR EXTRACTION FROM A TURKISH CORPUS

German Language Support Package

Guidelines for Students when Writing s. Raymond Hickey English Linguistics

SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS OF CAUSAL DENN IN GERMAN TATJANA SCHEFFLER

Transcription:

Verb-cluster variations: A Harmonic Grammar analysis Markus Bader Goethe-Universität Frankfurt New Ways of Analyzing Syntactic Variation Radboud University Nijmegen November 15.-17, 2012

Introduction In German, verbs normally select their dependent elements to the left. This is true for objects... (1)... dass Peter [ein Buch schreibt]. that P. a book writes...and also for verbs selected by another verb. (2) a.... dass er [es geschrieben hat]. that he it written has b.... dass er [es geschrieben haben that he it written have...that he might have written it. c.... dass [es geschrieben worden that it written been...that it might have been written. könnte]. could sein be könnte]. could

Introduction The general pattern thus looks as in (3): (3) a. V 2 V 1 b. V 3 V 2 V 1 c. V 4 V 3 V 2 V 1 There are certain well-known exceptions to (3): For V 1 = Aux perfective and V 2 = Modal, the auxiliary must be fronted to the cluster initial position according to normative grammars of Standard German: (4) Aux 1 V 3 Mod 2 dass er es [hat [schreiben wollen]]. that he it has write want that he wanted to write it.

Introduction However......we find a lot of variation across German dialects and varieties: (5) a. Certain variants of Austrian and Bavarian: dass er es [[schreiben wollen] hat]. b. Pattern typical for Austrian and Bavarian: dass er es [schreiben [hat wollen]]. c. Standard German: dass er es [hat [schreiben wollen]]. d. Pattern typical for Swiss German: dass er es [hat [wollen schreiben]]. V-Mod-Aux V-Aux-Mod Aux-V-Mod Aux-Mod-V Furthermore, it is reported that dialects often allow for more than one order.

Introduction In fact, variation is also found for non-dialect speakers: A series of experimental investigations of verb cluster formation has shown that non-dialect speakers of German do not adhere strictly to the Standard German pattern: Native speakers of Colloquial German are more liberal than prescriptive grammars of Standard German in a precisely defined way. Question addressed in this talk: How are variation as observed in acceptability judgments and variation as observed in corpus frequencies related to each other.

3-verb clusters: Introduction Topics Order among verbs within 3-verb clusters Comparison of acceptaility and frequency Two different methods to assess the acceptability/grammaticality of sentences: Speeded Grammaticality Judgments (SGJ): Participants judge sentences as either grammatical or ungrammatical under controlled and timed conditions. Magnitude Estimation (ME): Participants evaluate sentences relative to a reference sentence on a continuous scale. The experimental results discussed in this section are taken from Bader & Schmid (2009) and Bader & Häussler (2010).

3-verb clusters: Expectation (6) dass Peter ein Buch (hat) lesen (hat) müssen (hat). that P. a book has read has must has (7) dass Peter ein Buch (hat) müssen (hat) lesen (hat). that P. a book has read has must has Aux = 1 Aux = 2 Aux = 3 V < Mod Aux-V-Mod V-Aux-Mod V-Mod-Aux Mod < V Aux-Mod-V Mod-Aux-V Mod-V-Aux Expectation based on normative grammar: high percentages of judgments grammatical for order Aux-V-Mod low percentages for the remaining five orders

3-verb clusters: Procedures Speeded Grammaticality Judgments Word-by-word presentation in the middle of the screen Presentation time for each word: 225 ms plus an additional 25 ms per character End-of-sentence judgments with a deadline of 2000 ms Magnitude Estimation First, a reference item is presented to which the participant assigns an arbitrary numeric value (> 0). All further items are judged in proportion to the reference item on a continuous numerical scale. Each individual data point is divided by the reference value and the resulting ratio is log-transformed.

3-verb clusters: Representative results Left: Speeded grammticality judgments Right: Magnitude estimation % grammatical 0 20 40 60 80 100 Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 V<M Mean acceptability 0.3 0.1 0.1 0.3 M<V Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 V<Mod Mod<V Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 SGJ (%) 86 69 26 12 3 2 ME (log) 0.51 0.20-0.27-0.29-0.58-0.66

3-verb clusters: Summary of acceptability In all experiments, the Standard German order Aux-V-Mod received the best judgments. Independently of regional background, the partially inverted order V-Aux-Mod was judged only somewhat worse, despite being ungrammtical according to normative grammar. The remaining orders were judged as unacceptable, but with differing degrees: When the modal verb preceded the lexical verb, the aux-initial order Aux-MOD-V was judged better then the other two orders (MOD-Aux-V and MOD-V-Aux).

3-verb clusters: Expectation (8) dass Peter ein Buch hat lesen müssen. that P. a book has read must (9) dass Peter ein Buch lesen hat müssen. that P. a book read has must Aux-V-Mod V-Aux-Mod Aux = 1 Aux = 2 Aux = 3 V < Mod Aux-V-Mod V-Aux-Mod V-Mod-Aux Mod < V Aux-Mod-V Mod-Aux-V Mod-V-Aux (10) a. The Mod-V Constraint The complement of a modal verb precedes the modal verb. b. The Aux-Mod Restriction Restriction When the perfect auxiliary selects a modal verb, it must precede it.

On Gradience and Frequency Table: Experimental data for word order in 3-verb clusters. V>Mod Aux>Mod ME SGJ Constr. Constr. Exp. Exp. V<Mod Aux = 1.22 79 Aux = 2.09 61 Aux = 3 * -.12 28 Mod<V Aux = 1 * -.13 25 Aux = 2 * * -.25 5 Aux = 3 * * -.29 4 The two orders which violate neither of the two constraints are judged best. The two orders which violate only a single constraint are judged worse, but are not rejected completely. The two orders which violate both constraints are rejected completely.

On Gradience and Frequency Frequency of verb-final clusters in complementizer-initial embedded clauses in the Tiger Corpus (Version 2), a treebank containing about 50.000 sentences (ca. 888.000 tokens) from the German newspaper Frankfurter Rundschau. (11) 1 2 3 4 5 V[fin] - final 3959 2635 319 1 - V[fin] - initial - - 11 1 - Newspaper Corpus: the newspaper corpus made available by the Institute for German Language (IDS) in Mannheim/Germany (http://www.ids-mannheim.de) Web Corpus: the dewac corpus of German internet texts made available by the University of Bologna Baroni et al. (2009)

On Gradience and Frequency Table: Corpus and experimental data for word order in 3-verb clusters. V>Mod Aux>Mod Newspaper Web ME SGJ Constr. Constr. Corpus Search Exp. Exp. V<Mod Aux = 1 99.5 96.2.22 79 Aux = 2 0.5 3.8.09 61 Aux = 3 * 0 <.1 -.12 28 Mod<V Aux = 1 * 0 0 -.13 25 Aux = 2 * * 0 0 -.25 5 Aux = 3 * * 0 0 -.29 4 The frequency distribution is much more skewed than the acceptability distribution: Small variations in acceptability go hand in hand with large variations in frequency. Orders with zero or near-zero frequency can still differ with regard to acceptability.

On Gradience and Frequency Orders with zero or near-zero frequency can still differ with regard to acceptability. This argues against theories like Stochastic OT (Boersma & Hayes, 2001) which tie acceptability to frequency on the level of whole structures. What is needed instead is a way to relate frequency to acceptability on the level of individual constraints. The frequency distribution is much more skewed than the acceptability distribution. Frequency and acceptability must be related to each other in a non-linear way.

On Gradience and Frequency Relating frequency to acceptability: Recent versions of Harmonic Grammar (Pater, 2009; Boersma & Pater, 2008 have the desired properties (see also Goldwater & Johnson, 2003; Jäger, 2003; Jäger & Rosenbach, 2006): The weight of individual constraints is learned by frequency. The relationship between frequency and constraint weight is non-linear. Constraint weights can be related to acceptability in the way of Linear OT: Keller (2000, 2006)

On Gradience and Frequency Constraints for modelling: (12) a. Align(V, L) (V > Mod) b. Align(Aux Mod, L) (Aux Mod > Mod) Experiments with the praat program indicate that contraint weights accounting for acceptability can be learned from frequency. Open question: How to model the advantage of Aux-V-Modover V-Aux-Mod? Align(Aux Mod, Initial)? Making Align(Aux Mod, L) a gradable constraint?

4-verb clusters The final part of this talk presents new experimental evidence as well as corpus data on 4-verb clusters. 3-verb cluster (13) dass jemand die Schubkarre hätte 1 reparieren 3 müssen 2. that someone the wheelbarrow had repair must... that someone should have repaired the wheelbarrow. 4-verb clusters (14) dass die Schubkarre hätte 1 repariert 4 werden 3 müssen 2. that the wheelbarrow had repaired be must... that the wheelbarrow should have been repaired. (15) dass Max die Schubk. hätte 1 reparieren 4 lassen 3 müssen 2. that M. the wheelb. had repaired let must...that Werner should have let repair the wheelbarrow.

4-verb clusters (16) dass die Schubkarre hätte 1 repariert 4 werden 3 müssen 2. that the wheelbarrow had repaired be must... that the wheelbarrow should have been repaired. (17) V-Pass < Mod Mod < V-Pass Aux = 1 Aux 1 V 4 Pass 3 Mod 2 Aux 1 Mod 2 Pass 3 V 4 Aux = 2 V 4 Aux 1 Pass 3 Mod 2 Mod 2 Aux 1 V 4 Pass 3 Aux = 3 V 4 Pass 3 Aux 1 Mod 2 Mod 2 V 4 Aux 1 Pass 3 Aux = 4 V 4 Pass 3 Mod 2 Aux 1 Mod 2 V 4 Pass 3 Aux 1

4-verb clusters Experiment 1: 4-verb clusters Method: Magnitude estimation Ich glaube, dass die Schubkarre schon letzte Woche... I think that the wheelbarrow already last week V < Mod Aux=1... hätte repariert werden müssen had repaired be must Aux=2... repariert hätte werden müssen Aux=3... repariert werden hätte müssen Aux=4... repariert werden müssen hätte Mod < V Aux=1... hätte müssen repariert werden Aux=2... müssen hätte repariert werden Aux=3... müssen repariert hätte werden Aux=4... müssen repariert werden hätte Translation for all conditions: I think that the wheelbarrow had to be repaired already last week.

4-verb clusters Experiment 2: 4-verb clusters Method: Magnitude estimation Ich glaube, dass Max die Schubkarre sofort... I think that M. the wheelbarrow immediately V < Mod Aux=1... hätte reparieren lassen müssen had repair let must Aux=2... reparieren hätte lassen müssen Aux=3... reparieren lassen hätte müssen Aux=4... reparieren lassen müssen hätte Mod < V Aux=1...hätte müssen reparieren lassen Aux=2... müssen hätte reparieren lassen Aux=3... müssen reparieren hätte lassen Aux=4... müssen reparieren lassen hätte Translation for all conditions: I think that the wheelbarrow had to be repaired already last week.

4-verb clusters Mean acceptability 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.2 V<M M<V Mean acceptability 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.2 V<M M<V Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 Aux=4 Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 Aux=4 Figure: Results of Experiment 1 and 2

4-verb clusters % grammatical 0 20 40 60 80 100 V<M M<V Mean acceptability 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.2 V<M M<V Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 Aux=4 Aux=1 Aux=2 Aux=3 Aux=4 Figure: Results of Experiment 4 of Bader & Schmid (2009) and results of Experiment 1

4-verb clusters Table: Corpus and experimental data for word order in 3-verb clusters. V>Mod Aux>Mod Newspaper Web Exp 1 Exp 2 Constr. Constr. Corpus Search (ME) (ME) V<Mod Aux = 1 100 95.1.26.28 Aux = 2 0 4.0.17.12 Aux = 3 0 0.8.04.11 Aux = 4 * 0 0 -.31 -.11 Mod<V Aux = 1 * 0 0 -.08 -.01 Aux = 2 * * 0 0 -.34 -.24 Aux = 3 * * 0 0 -.43 -.28 Aux = 4 * * 0 0 -.45 -.34 The results are similar to those for 3-verb clusters: All orders that violate neither of the two constraints receive positive acceptability scores, all other orders receive negative acceptability constraints. Only orders that violate no constraint are found in the corpus. There is some evidence that the position of the auxiliary should be modeled by a gradable constraint.

Summary Variation as seen in German complex verb clusters presents a complex relationship between frequency and acceptability. Variation in acceptability despite zero frequency. Small variations in acceptability go hand in hand with large variations in frequency. Recent developments in weight-based OT allow us to capture the frequency-relationship observed for verb-clusters.

References Bader, M. & Häussler, J. (2010). Toward a model of grammaticality judgments. Journal of Linguistics 46, 273 330. Bader, M. & Schmid, T. (2009). Verb clusters in Colloquial German. The Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 12, 175 228. Baroni, M., Bernardini, S., Ferraresi, A. & Zanchetta, E. (2009). The WaCky Wide Web: A collection of very large linguistically processed web-crawled corpora. Language Resources and Evaluation Journal 23, 209 226. Boersma, P. & Hayes, B. (2001). Empirical tests of the gradual learning algorithm. Linguistic Inquiry 32, 45 86. Boersma, P. & Pater, J. (2008). Convergence properties of a gradual learning algorithm for Harmonic Grammar. Unpublished manuscript, University of Massachusetts at Amherst (available at http://roa.rutgers.edu/). Goldwater, S. & Johnson, M. (2003). Learning OT constraint ranking using a maximum entropy model. In J. Spenader, A. Eriksson & s. Dahl (Eds.), Proceedings of the Stockholm Workshop on Variation within Optimality Theory, (pp. 111 120). University of Stockholm. Jäger, G. (2003). Maximum entropy models and Stochastic Optimality Theory. Unpublished Manuscript, University of Potsdam (Rutgers Optimality Archive (ROA) 625-1003). Jäger, G. & Rosenbach, A. (2006). The winner takes it all almost: cumulativity in grammatical variation. Linguistics 44, 937 971. Keller, F. (2000). Gradience in grammar: Experimental and computational aspects of degrees of grammaticality. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edingburgh. Keller, F. (2006). Linear optimality theory as a model of gradience in grammar. In G. Fanselow, C. Féry, R. Vogel & M. Schlesewsky (Eds.), Gradience in grammar: Generative perspectives, (pp. 270 287). New York: Oxford University Press. Pater, J. (2009). Weigthed constraints in generative linguistics. Cognitive Science 33, 999 1035.