PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE"

From this document you will learn the answers to the following questions:

  • What was the subscription number for this article?

  • What is the number of the office in which this article was published?

  • What university is the source of this article?

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by:[bases] [BASES] On: 19 June 2007 Access Details: [subscription number ] Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: Mortimer House, Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Sports Sciences Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: Influences of volitional and forced intentions on physical activity and effort within the theory of planned behaviour To cite this Article: Chatzisarantis, Nikos L. D., Frederick, Christina, Biddle, Stuart J. H., Hagger, Martin S. and Smith, Brett, 'Influences of volitional and forced intentions on physical activity and effort within the theory of planned behaviour', Journal of Sports Sciences, 25:6, To link to this article: DOI: / URL: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. Taylor and Francis 2007

2 Journal of Sports Sciences, April 2007; 25(6): Influences of volitional and forced intentions on physical activity and effort within the theory of planned behaviour NIKOS L. D. CHATZISARANTIS 1, CHRISTINA FREDERICK 2, STUART J. H. BIDDLE 3, MARTIN S. HAGGER 4, & BRETT SMITH 1 1 University of Exeter, Exeter, UK, 2 Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University, Daytona Beach, Florida, USA, 3 Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK and 4 University of Essex, Essex, UK (Accepted 28 April 2006) Abstract In the present study, we examined the utility of volitional and forced intentions in predicting participation in physical activities and effort within the theory of planned behaviour. Four hundred and forty-four participants (184 males, 260 females) aged years (mean + s) completed self-report measures of intentions, attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioural control, volitional intentions, forced intentions, and past behaviour in a physical activity context. Six weeks later, they completed self-report measures of physical activity behaviour and effort. Results indicated that volitional intentions and forced intentions contributed to the prediction of effort over and above intentions, attitudes, subjective norms, perceptions of control, past behaviour, and the product terms of attitudes6intentions and subjective norms6intentions. Volitional intentions and forced intentions did not predict participation in physical activities over and above effort. We concluded that volitional intentions and forced intentions assist in the explanation of effort in the context of physical activity behaviour. Keywords: Physical activity, long-range prediction, meta-cognition Introduction The theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1985) proposes a model of intentional behaviour that has led to the successful prediction of social behaviour in several contexts (Ajzen, 1998). The model assumes that the behaviour in question, such as physical activity, is a function of volitional processes (van Ryn, Lytle, & Kirscht, 1996), and hence under the control of a person s overtly stated intention (Bagozzi, Baumgartner, & Yi, 1992; Bagozzi & Warshaw, 1992). However, while the theory of planned behaviour has received empirical support, research findings cast doubt on the long-term predictive efficacy of the model (Ajzen, 1985; Liska, 1984; Sheeran & Orbell, 1999). One possible reason for this is that intentions do not reflect volition accurately (Meinland, 1970). In the present study, therefore, we examined whether the constructs of volitional and forced intentions assist intentions in indicating volition, and thus improve the long-term predictive efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour model. Theory of planned behaviour and prediction of social behaviour The theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen & Madden, 1986) describes how people process information before engaging in deliberative, volitional behaviour. When applied to the domain of physical exercise (or activity), the theory predicts that people process three types of beliefs during deliberation. First, there are beliefs (behavioural beliefs) about outcomes associated with physical activity, which guide physical activity intentions through attitudes. Thus, attitudes reflect an overall evaluation towards physical activity captured in attribute dimensions such as good bad, harmful beneficial, or pleasant unpleasant (Ajzen, 2001, p. 28). Furthermore, direct attitudes are formed spontaneously as individuals process their beliefs about physical activity outcomes (Ajzen, 2001). Second, there are beliefs about expectations of others regarding physical activity (normative beliefs). These guide intentions through subjective norms. The theory of planned behaviour further enunciates that subjective Correspondence: N. L. D. Chatzisarantis, School of Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, St. Luke s Campus, Exeter EX1 2LU, UK. n.chatzisarantis@exeter.ac.uk ISSN print/issn X online Ó 2007 Taylor & Francis DOI: /

3 700 N. L. D. Chatzisarantis et al. norms reflect the influences that significant others (i.e. parents, teachers, peers, spouse) may exert on decisions to engage in physical activities. Third, according to theory, there are beliefs (control beliefs) about factors that may facilitate or impede participation in physical activities. As such, these control beliefs guide individuals intentions to engage in a particular behaviour through perceived behavioural control. That is, an individual s beliefs relating to control over participation in physical activities shape and frame the intention to undertake this behaviour (Ajzen, 2001). Furthermore, the theory of planned behaviour postulates that attitudes in combination with subjective norms and perceptions of control lead to the formation of physical activity intentions. These intentions, as Ajzen (1991) suggested, indicate how hard people are willing to try and how much effort people are planning to exert towards participation in physical activities. Moreover, tenets of this theory postulate that when physical activity is perceived not to be under complete volitional control, intentions predict social behaviour indirectly via effort (Ajzen, 1985). The construct of effort does not only indicate how much physical effort people exert when engaging in physical activities but, after Bagozzi and Kimmel (1995), how much psychological effort people devote towards engaging in physical activities. For example, in the context of physical exercise, the construct of effort may indicate how much effort people devote towards planning physical activity participation (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995). Accordingly, the direct effects of physical activity intentions on effort reflect the influences that intentions exert on attempts to participate in physical activities. In addition, the theory predicts that when perceived control is realistic, perceived behavioural control will predict physical activity behaviour directly (Ajzen & Madden, 1986; Hausenblas, Carron, & Mack, 1997; Sheeran & Orbell, 1998; Sheppard, Hartwick, & Warshaw, 1988). To date, the theory of planned behaviour has been corroborated by numerous meta-analytic reviews across a range of behaviours (Armitage and Conner, 2001; Conner & Armitage, 1998; Kim & Hunter, 1993) and specific health behaviours such as condom use (Albarracín, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001; Sheeran & Orbell, 1998; Sheeran and Taylor, 1999) and exercise (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002; Hausenblas et al., 1997). There is also evidence to suggest that intentions predict physical activity indirectly via effort (Chatizsarantis, Hagger, Biddle, & Karageorghis, 2002). Notwithstanding this corroborating evidence, the theory has its critics. According to Hagger et al. (2002), it does not explain all variation in physical activity behaviour. Furthermore, a meta-analysis by Sheeran and Orbell (1998) highlighted that intentions become less predictive of it when behaviour is observed over long intervals of time (i.e. more than 4 weeks). Given the small predictive validity of intentions, it would appear that further research is required that attempts to elucidate some of the complexities associated with intentions, prediction, and behaviour. One way to achieve this is to explore a number of the assumptions underlying the theory of planned behaviour and the measurement issues that relate to such assumptions. Some assumptions: Intentions, volition, and the prediction of social behaviour and effort According to Ajzen (1991, p. 181), intentions are assumed to capture the motivational factors that influence behavior; they are indicators of how hard people are willing to try, of how much effort they are planning to exert in order to perform the behavior. Building on the work of Meinland (1970), it could be argued that verbal expressions of intentions and experiences of volition are not the same. That is, people may report that they intend to engage in a certain social behaviour because they believe, for example, that it is their duty to do so. To illustrate the point, consider the case of compulsory education. Would a verbally expressed intention to participate in physical education classes in the upcoming month reflect willingness to participate in physical education classes? If physical education is a compulsory subject, pupils may intend to participate in physical education because they feel that it is their duty to do so. Hence, in some cases, people might well intend to engage in social behaviour without wanting to do so. The inadequacy of measures of intentions to reflect experiences of volition could be related to the relatively lower predictive validity of intentions observed in previous studies that investigated behaviour over long intervals of time (Sheeran & Orbell, 1998). It could be argued, for example, that the assumption that intentions can achieve long-range prediction is untenable considering that previous research has differentiated between volition and intentions, and demonstrated deferential effects of volitional and forced decisions on behaviour (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). Nearly 47 years of experimental work on cognitive dissonance theory has shown that decisions can be volitional or forced and that volitional decisions exert greater effects on attitudes and overt behaviour than forced decisions (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). Using a different conceptual framework, 36 years of experimental investigations of self-determination theory have shown that volitional decisions are associated with effort and prolonged participation in pro-social behaviours, whereas this is not always the case for

4 Volition and intentions 701 forced decisions (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Williams, Freedman, Ryan, & Deci, 1996). As Ryan, Koestner and Deci (1992) have argued, forced decisions can motivate effort for as along as people are forced to engage in the behaviour. When the external or internal (i.e. feelings of guilt) pressuring forces stop operating, forced decisions do not have an effect on effort or performance of behaviour (Deci et al., 1999). Therefore, a theoretically intriguing proposal is the hypothesis that measures of volitional and forced intentions will increase the utility of intentions and of the theory of planned behaviour in predicting effort. It is important to note here that in this study we do not use the term volition to describe the phase that mediates between intentions and performance of behaviour (Gollwiltzer, 1990). Rather, we use the term volition to describe the way that people experience intentions and decisions (Meinland, 1970). The term volition therefore is intended to describe the phenomenology of intentions. In addition, although we use the terms intention and decision interchangeably, we do recognize that the relationship between decisions and intentions is complex. In the present study, however, we do not consider that there is an additional step of deciding that occurs between deliberation and formation of intentions which is distinct from the formation of intentions and which brings about formation of intentions (Ajzen, 1991; Meinland, 1970). Having noted these caveats and definitions, in the next subsection we discuss issues relating to the measurement of volitional and forced intentions. Measurement of volitional intentions and intentions of duty One way to measure the volitional quotient of intentions is to ask people to report how they experience their intentions. Perceptions related to how people experience intentions indicate metajudgemental properties of intentions (Bassili, 1996) and, according to Meinland (1970), such a measurement can be obtained by asking people to report why individuals intend to participate in social behaviour. People reporting that they intend to engage in physical activities because they have to can be considered as holding intentions that are less volitional in nature because verbal expressions of have to reflect obligation (Meinland, 1970). In contrast, people reporting that they intend to participate in physical activities because they want to can be considered as holding volitional intentions given that the verbal expression of want to is an expression of volition (Meinland, 1970). It should also be stressed that verbal expressions of want to may not accurately reflect volition, given that a person may want to do his or her duty (Meinland, 1970). Therefore, it is important to include other indicators of volition that may indicate experiences of volition more accurately in the measurement of volitional intentions. Good indicators of volitional intentions, as Meinland (1970) suggested, include experiences of pleasure and energy that people accrue from their engagement in physical activities. For example, if people state that they want to exercise and they express pleasure at the prospect of exercising, this may serve as a good indicator that people s physical activity intentions are volitional. Similarly, it can be suggested that the extent to which people try to please significant others requests can serve as good indicators of less volitional intentions (Meinland, 1970). For instance, if people indicate that they intend to engage in physical activities to please significant others requests, then these may serve as good indicators of forced intentions. Set against this backdrop, in the present study we tested the proposition that intentions do not indicate willingness to participate in social behaviour and that measures of volitional and forced intentions will increase utility of intentions in indicating experiences of volition (Meinland, 1970). To test this proposition, measures of intentions that represented volitional and forced decisions (i.e. volitional intentions and forced intentions) were obtained and then tested for discriminant and predictive validity. Based on the conceptually informed arguments presented above, we tested three hypotheses. First, it was hypothesized that volitional and forced intentions would display discriminant validity with intentions (H 1 ). This hypothesis was important to test because if volitional and forced intentions did not display discriminant validity with intentions, it could be argued that intentions represented volition satisfactorily, in which case there was no need to incorporate volitional and forced intentions within the theory of planned behaviour. Second, it was hypothesized that volitional and forced intentions would make an independent contribution to the prediction of measures of effort that reflected willingness to participate in physical activities over and above intentions, perceptions of control, attitudes, and subjective norms (H 2 ). Here we set effort and not actual participation in physical activities as a criterion in evaluating predictive validity of volitional and forced intentions because our central hypothesis posits that volitional and forced intentions assist intentions in indicating willingness to participate in social behaviours and not necessarily actual execution of social behaviour. Clearly, willingness to execute a social behaviour and actual execution are not the same. People may be characterized by a strong willingness to exercise but fail to do so because environmental factors that

5 702 N. L. D. Chatzisarantis et al. people cannot control directly (i.e. transportation) inhibit their attempts to exercise (Ajzen, 1985). However, failure of measures of volitional or forced intentions to predict effort over and above the theory of planned behaviour raise questions about whether measures of volitional and forced intentions improve the utility of intentions in indicating willingness to participate in physical activities. Finally, we used measures of past behaviour to control statistically for the effects of unmeasured variables on effort and on physical activity behaviour (Bagozzi, 1981). Specifically, previously research has shown that past behaviour is a strong predictor of intentions and of social behaviour (Ajzen, 2002; Hagger et al., 2002). It has also been suggested that the main effects from past behaviour to future behaviour reflected effects from psychological variables that have not been taken into consideration by the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 2002; Bagozzi, 1981). Therefore, through measures of past behaviour, we iexamined whether the effects from volitional intentions and forced intentions on effort and social behaviour were independent from determinants of social behaviour that the theory of planned behaviour omits. It was hypothesized that if the effects from volitional and forced intentions to effort and behaviour were independent from antecedents of social behaviour that the theory of planned behaviour omits, volitional or forced intentions would predict effort over and above past behaviour (H 3 ). Overall, in this study we tested effects of volitional intentions and forced intentions on effort and physical activity participation in a manner presented in Figure 1. According to this diagram, volitional intentions and forced intentions exert direct effects on effort but not on physical activity participation. In addition, components of the theory of planned behaviour and past behaviour exert effects on effort and physical activity participation. One ramification of the present study is that intentions do not reflect volition accurately and that the constructs of volitional and forced intentions assist intentions in indicating experiences of volition. Methods Research participants and procedure A total of 444 individuals (184 males, 260 females) aged years (mean + s) were recruited from comprehensive schools and universities in the Midlands, UK. Participants were volunteers and informed consent was obtained from each individual and from the head teachers of the schools. A letter explaining the procedures and risks associated with our study was also delivered to the parents via their children, and parents were provided with the option to decline participation by their child by returning the letter. None of the parents declined participation. The participants took 20 minutes to complete the questionnaires in groups of less than 20 in quite classroom settings. Four researchers were responsible for implementing, distributing, and collecting the questionnaires. Teachers were also present during data collection. The participants were separated to prevent them from copying one another. They were not asked to report their name in the questionnaire. Response attrition was over 10.2%. A prospective design was employed with psychological variables being assessed at two instants in time. In the first wave of data collection, participants were provided with a definition of vigorous physical activity performed during leisure time, developed by Godin and Shephard (1985). This definition described vigorous physical activity as activities performed at vigorous intensity for at least 30 minutes, 3 days per week for the next 6 weeks, during leisure time. To ensure that the definition of leisure time physical activity was understood, the participants were asked to report examples of vigorous physical activities and of leisure time. Next, the questionnaire assessed variables specified by the theory of planned behaviour, past behaviour, and volitional and forced intentions. The participants were also instructed to use this definition when answering questions related to the theory of planned behaviour, past behaviour, volitional intentions, and forced intentions. After 6 weeks, actual participation in physical activities during leisure time and effort were assessed (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995; Godin & Shephard, 1985). Prospective responses were matched with baseline responses using dates of birth and sex as matching indices. Figure 1. A model presenting hypothesized relationships between components of the theory of planned behaviour, past behaviour, forced intentions, and volitional intentions. Measures The theory of planned behaviour and past behaviour. The development of the theory of planned behaviour questionnaire followed the procedures

6 Volition and intentions 703 recommended by Ajzen and Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen and Madden (1986). Three items drawn from Ajzen and Fishbein (1980) and Ajzen and Madden (1986) were used to measure behavioural intentions. An example was: I intend to do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities, for at least 30 minutes, three days per week, during my leisure time, over the next six weeks. All items were rated on 7-point Likert scales ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). Subjective norms were measured through two items, on a 7-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). An example was: Others who are important to me think that I should do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities for at least 30 minutes, three days per week, during my leisure time, over the next six weeks. Attitudes were assessed through three bipolar adjectives. One adjective was intended to reflect moral evaluations (bad good). An additional adjective echoed instrumental evaluations (harmful beneficial), while the third reflected affective evaluations (interesting boring). All adjectives were measured on 7-point semantic differential scales (Ajzen & Driver, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). An example was: For me, doing active sports and/or vigorous physical activities for at least 30 minutes, three days per week, during my leisure time, over the next six weeks.... Perceived behavioural control was assessed on 7- point scales (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). Examples included: I feel in complete control over whether I do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities for at least 30 minutes, 3 days per week, during my leisure time, over the next six weeks ; How much control do you have over doing active sports and/or vigorous physical activities for at least 30 minutes, 3 days per week, during your leisure time, over the next six weeks. Average scores ranging from 1 to 7 were used to represent attitudes, intentions, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control. Volitional and forced intentions. The questionnaire developed to measure volitional and forced intentions asked why the respondents intended to participate in active sports and/or vigorous physical activities for at least 30 minutes, 3 days per week, over the next six weeks, during their leisure time, and then provided six reasons that have been pre-selected by us to represent volitional and forced intentions. Indicators of volitional intentions reflected an expression of volition (e.g. because I really want to exercise ), feelings of enjoyment (e.g. because I enjoy physical activity ), and feelings of energy (e.g. because physical activity makes me feel energetic ). Indicators of forced intentions reflected feelings of obligation (e.g. because I have to exercise ), a tendency to satisfy important others requests (e.g. in order to please important others requests ), and a tendency not to disappoint important others (e.g. because I would disappoint others if I do not exercise ). All items were measured on 7-point Likert scales ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). As with components of the theory of planned behaviour, mean scores ranging from (1) to (7) were used to represent volitional intentions and forced intentions. Physical activity and effort. At the first (past behaviour) and second wave (future behaviour) of data collection, vigorous physical activity was assessed using Godin and Shephard s (1985) Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire. The instrument contains three open-ended questions capturing the frequency of mild, moderate, and vigorous physical activity. The present study targeted vigorous physical activity. Independent evaluations of the Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire found it to be valid, reliable, easy to administer, and to display concurrent validity with objective activity and fitness indices (Jacobs, Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993). The measure of physical activity that was administered during the first wave of data collection represents past behaviour, whereas the measure of physical activity that was administered at the second wave of data collection represents future behaviour. Measures of physical activity behaviour could range from 1 to 7. Perceived effort was measured during the second wave of data collection. The participants reported the effort they had exerted in a sense of maintaining willpower, planning, energy, trying, and discipline in the past five weeks (e.g. How hard did you try in a sense of maintaining your willpower to exercise the last 6 weeks, during your leisure time? ). This measure of effort is considered to reflect strength of will or desire to participate in physical activities more accurately than measures of physical activity given that indicators of effort indicate willpower whereas measures of physical activity participation indicate actual participation in physical activities. All types of effort were measured on 7-point scales ranging from tried not at all (1) to tried very hard (7) (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995). Mean scores ranging from 1 to 7 were used to represent effort. Data analysis A series of confirmatory factor analyses was used to examine discriminant validity between volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions (H 1 ) (Mulaik & Millsap, 2000). Discriminant validity was examined by evaluating the utility of a three-factor model (Model 1) in explaining the covariance between indicators of intentions, forced intentions, and volitional intentions (Bagozzi, 1981; Bentler,

7 704 N. L. D. Chatzisarantis et al. 1989). Model 1 was compared with two competing models that hypothesized lack of discriminant validity either between intentions and volitional intentions (Model 2) or between intentions and forced intentions (Model 3). Model 2 tested lack of discriminant validity between intentions and volitional intentions because it examined the utility of a single factor in explaining the covariance between measures of intentions and volitional intentions. Model 3 tested lack of discriminant validity between intentions and forced intentions because it examined the utility of a single factor in explaining the covariance between measures of intentions and forced intentions. Parameters of all confirmatory models were estimated with the maximum likelihood estimation method using the EQS statistical software (Bentler, 1989). In addition, in all models, variances of factors were fixed at 1.0 (Bentler, 1989), while cross-loadings and residual correlations were fixed at zero. Results from the confirmatory factor analysis were considered to support discriminant validity if (a) Model 1 explained the observed covariance matrix more satisfactorily than Models 2 and 3 did, and (b) the confidence interval of the correlation between constructs that assumed to possess discriminant validity (i.e. volitional intentions and intentions) did not include unity (Bagozzi, 1981). A hierarchical regression analysis was used to examine the utility of volitional intentions and forced intentions to predict effort and physical activity participation over and above the components of the theory of planned behaviour (H 2 ) and past behaviour (H 3 ). In the regression analyses predicting physical activity, effort was entered in the first step, intentions and perceptions of control were entered in the second step, attitudes and subjective norms were entered in the third step, past behaviour was entered in the fourth step, and volitional intentions and forced intentions were entered in the fifth step. It is important to note that effort was entered in the first step of the analysis because Ajzen s (1985) original model of the theory of planned behaviour assumed effort, and not intentions, to constitute the most proximal determinant of behaviour. The regression analysis predicting effort specified intentions and perceptions of control in the first step, attitudes and subjective norms in the second step, past behaviour in the third step, and volitional intentionsandforcedintentionsinthefourthstep. The third hypothesis was supported if volitional intentions and forced intentions contributed to the prediction of effort over and above the theory of planned behaviour and past behaviour. Assessment of model fit Adequacy of confirmatory models in explaining observations was assessed through fit indices. The most common statistic in assessing model fit is the chi-square goodness of fit test (w 2 ), which estimates discrepancies between model-implied and observed covariance matrices. However, the chisquare test has been criticized as being insufficient to assess model fit because its power to detect miniscule differences is high as sample size increases (Cohen, 1988). We therefore used the comparative fit index and standardized root mean square residual as a means of evaluating model fit because previous research has shown that these fit indices displayed restricted random variation under various conditions of model misspecification, sample size, and estimation methods (Fan, Thomson, & Wang, 1999; Hu & Bentler, 1999). Furthermore, a cut-off value close to 0.95 for the confirmatory fit index and a cut-off value close to 0.08 for the standardized root mean square residual were used to evaluate the adequacy of models because the Type I and II error rates associated with these criteria are low (Hu & Bentler, 1999). We also used Akaike s information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987) as a way to compare models (e.g. Models 1, 2, and 3). The AIC indicates the extent to which a model would cross-validate in an independent sample, without requiring a second sample. When used for model comparisons, the model that obtains the lowest AIC is considered to demonstrate the best fit (Rigdon, 1989). Results Confirmatory factor analyses The fit indices presented in Table I show that Model 1 exceeded recent criteria of good fit, whereas this was not necessarily the case for Models 2 and 3 (Fan et al., 1999; Hu & Bentler, 1999). In addition, the fit indices indicated that Model 1 explained observations more satisfactorily than the competing Models 2 and 3 given that the AIC of Model 1 was much lower than those of Models 2 and 3. Examination of the parameters of Model 1 showed that loadings of indicators of volitional intentions and forced intentions were significant and positive. The average magnitude of the standardized loadings for volitional and forced intentions was 0.72, which is well above the accepted minimum of 0.40 (Bagozzi, 1981; Ford, MacCallum, & Tait, 1986). Moreover, the confidence intervals of the correlations corrected for attenuation between intentions and volitional intentions (r ¼ 0.64, standard error ¼ 0.03), between intentions and forced intentions (r ¼.09, standard error ¼ 0.05), and between volitional intentions and forced intentions (r ¼.03, standard error ¼ 0.03) did not include unity. These results therefore corroborate the first hypothesis (H 1 ), that volitional

8 Volition and intentions 705 intentions, forced intentions, and intentions would display discriminant validity (Ford et al., 1986; Mulaik & Millsap, 2000). Descriptive statistics Table II presents descriptive statistics for the psychological measures and, as indicated, most of the measures attained satisfactory internal consistency given that the alpha coefficients exceeded An exception was the measure of subjective norms (a ¼ 0.64). However, the low reliability of subjective norms can be seen as a limitation of the theory of planned behaviour, since previous research applications of the theory indicated low reliability for subjective norms (Courneya & McAuley, 1995). Correlations showed that behavioural intentions and perceived behavioural control were positively associated with physical activity and effort (Ajzen, 1985). Attitudes, perceptions of control, and subjective norms were also positively associated with intentions (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). Volitional intentions were positively associated with effort and physical activity, whereas forced intentions were positively associated with effort only (see Table II). Prediction of physical activity participation and effort In accordance with the assumptions underlying the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1985), the first step of the hierarchical regression analysis (see Table III) showed that effort predicted participation in physical activities (F ¼ 41.27, P ). Consistent with Ajzen and Madden s (1986) predictions, the second step of the analysis showed that intentions predicted participation in physical activities (DF ¼ 25.91, P ). Perceived control did not contribute to the prediction of physical activity over and above effort given that, in the second step of the regression analysis, the semi-partial correlation of perceived control with physical activity was not statistically significant (b ¼ 0.00, P ). The third step in the analysis showed that attitudes and subjective norms did not improve the validity of intentions in predicting physical activity participation (DF ¼ 0.77, P ). Consistent with previous research (Hagger et al., 2002), the fourth step of the analysis showed that past behaviour contributed to the prediction of physical activity (DF ¼ 13.35, P ). The fifth step of the regression analysis demonstrated that volitional intentions and forced intentions did not contribute to the prediction of physical activity (DF ¼ 1.20, P ). Finally, we conducted an additional hierarchical regression analysis that predicted physical activity participation from components of the theory of planned behaviour, volitional intentions, forced intentions, and from past behaviour but not from effort. This hierarchical regression analysis revealed a significant effect of volitional intentions on physical activity (DF ¼ 7.00, P ). Forced intentions did not predict physical activity participation (DF ¼ 0.64, P ). Table I. Results of confirmatory factor analyses testing discriminant validity between volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions. w 2 CFI SRMSR AIC Model 1: Volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions (24) Model 2: Volitional intentions and intentions load to the same factor (26) Model 3: Forced intentions and intentions load to the same factor (26) Note: CFI ¼ comparative fit index; STMSR ¼ standardized root mean square residual; AIC ¼ Akaike s information criterion. Table II. Descriptive statistics for components of the theory of planned behaviour, volitional intentions, forced intentions, and past behaviour. mean s a Physical activity Effort * Intentions * 0.398* Perceived control * 0.202* 0.424* Attitudes * 0.354* 0.539* 0.298* Subjective norms * 0.120* Volitional intentions * 0.362* 0.569* 0.270* 0.618* Forced intentions * 0.141* * 0.105* Past behaviour * 0.324* 0.676* 0.337* 0.384* * 0.104* 1.0 Note: Correlations with an asterisk are significant at P

9 706 N. L. D. Chatzisarantis et al. Table III. Prediction of physical activity and effort. Physical activity Effort R 2 b t R 2 b t Steps Steps * * Effort * Intentions * * Perceived control Effort * * Intentions * Intentions * Perceived control Perceived control Attitudes * Effort * Subjective norms Intentions * * Perceived control Intentions * Attitudes Perceived control Subjective norms Attitudes * * Subjective norms Effort * Past behaviour Intentions * * Perceived control Intentions * Attitudes Perceived control Subjective norms Attitudes Past behaviour * Subjective norms Past behaviour Effort * Forced intentions * Intentions Volitional intentions * Perceived control Attitudes Subjective norms Past behaviour * Volitional intentions Forced intentions Note: Parameters with an asterisk are significant at P The hierarchical regression analysis that predicted effort supported a statistically significant effect from intentions to effort (F ¼ 27.88, P ). Perceived control did not contribute to the prediction of effort over and above intentions given that, in the first step of the regression analysis, the semi-partial correlation of perceived control with effort was not statistically significant (b ¼ 0.054, P ). The second step of the analysis indicated that attitudes improved the validity of intentions in predicting effort (DF ¼ 4.89, P ). However, the third step of the analysis showed that past behaviour did not contribute to the prediction of effort over and above the theory of planned behaviour. In accordance with the second hypothesis of the present study (H 2 ), the fourth step of the regression analysis demonstrated that volitional intentions and forced intentions contributed to the prediction of effort over and above past behaviour and components of the theory of planned behaviour (DF ¼ 8.12, P ). Interestingly, in accordance with hypothesis 3, in the fourth step of the analysis, the semi-partial correlations revealed that volitional intentions and forced intentions exerted a statistically significant effect on effort after controlling for past behaviour effects. Discussion In the present study, we examined the proposition that volitional intentions and forced intentions increased utility of intentions in indicating willingness to participate in physical activities (Meinland, 1970). Consistent with the initial hypothesis, our results supported discriminant validity between intentions, volitional intentions, and forced intentions, and a significant contribution of volitional intentions and forced intentions to the prediction of effort. Because measures of effort reflected willingness to participate in physical activity, the effects of volitional intentions and forced intentions on effort corroborate insufficiency of measures of intention in representing volitional decisions. As such, in the context of leisure time physical activity, our findings support past research and extend significantly the applications of the theory of planned behaviour model in several ways. The relationship between volition and intentions One clear finding that emerged from this study is that volitional intentions, forced intentions, and

10 Volition and intentions 707 intentions possessed discriminant validity. Specifically, parameters of the confirmatory factor Model 1 pointed out that volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions measured distinct psychological events. This is because the intentional model that assumed discriminant validity (Model 1) explained observations better than the models that assumed lack of discriminant validity (Models 2 and 3). These results for discriminant validity substantiate the proposition that intentions, volitional intentions, and forced intentions are empirically distinct: intentions measure the strength and/or direction of intentions (Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Meinland, 1970), whereas volitional intentions and forced intentions measure meta-judgemental properties of intentions (Bassili, 1996) that indicate the extent to which intentions are volitional in nature (Meinland, 1970). It is also important to emphasize that the confirmatory factor analysis supports the notion that volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions do not share common method factors given that Model 2 assumed zero correlations at the residual space of the factors (Bagozzi, 1981; Bentler, 1989). It is also worth noting that measures of intentions, volitional intentions, and forced intentions do not share common method variance despite the fact that the wording of the questions measuring intentions, volitional intentions, and forced intentions made reference to intentions. It therefore appears that participants identified that measures of volitional intentions and forced intentions assessed meta-judgemental properties of intentions (i.e. how participants experience their intentions) and not intentions per se. It is also interesting to note that the pattern of correlations between psychological variables (see Table II) indicated that intentions were positively correlated with volitional intentions and not with forced intentions. This finding is important because it suggests that, in the context of leisure time physical activity, intentions are experienced on average as volitional. This may be due to beliefs that leisure activities are enjoyable (Sansone, Sachau, & Weir, 1989; Tang & Baumeister, 1984). For many people, the narrative of leisure time represents freedom from work and duties, allowing them to be at ease, to turn to pleasure, to lay aside obligations and rules (Grayling, 2001). Alternatively, the correlation between volitional intentions and intentions could reflect a relationship between strength of intentions and experiences of volition. On both theoretical (Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Meinland, 1970) and empirical (Kline, 1993) grounds, it can be suggested that strong intentions are experienced as being more volitional than weak intentions. Therefore, volitional intentions and intentions may be strongly correlated because of a positive relationship between intention strength and volition. The prediction of effort and of physical activity participation Another research finding from the regression analysis is the contribution that volitional intentions and forced intentions made to the prediction of effort. Specifically, the regression analysis showed that volitional intentions and forced intentions predicted effort over and above intentions (see Table III). Because our measures of effort indicated willingness to participate in physical activities, the current results support Meinland s (1970) proposition that volitional intentions improve utility of intentions in representing willingness to participate in physical activities. The contribution of volitional intentions to the prediction of effort is additionally in agreement with a number of theories that have differentiated between volitional and forced decisions (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; Williams et al., 1996). In addition, the results of the present study can be explained with reference to tenets of selfdiscrimination theory (Kuhl & Kazen, 1994). According to this theory, there is a strong relationship between volitional decisions, forced decisions, and effort (see also Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan et al., 1992). However, it is important to stress that in addition to confirming tenets of psychological theories, like self-determination theory and self discrimination theory, the present study extends previous research in various ways. The first is related to the prediction of social behaviour. The regression analysis showed that volitional intentions and forced intentions predicted effort and not physical activity behaviour. In part, this is because effort exertion is within a person s volitional control to a greater extent than performance of behaviour (Ajzen, 1985; Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995). Furthermore, performance of physical activity is more likely to be impeded by factors that the person cannot control (e.g. transportation, resources) than behavioural attempt is (Ajzen, 1985; Hagger et al., 2002; Kuhl & Kazen, 1994). Therefore, it can be expected that volitional and forced decisions influence effort and not participation in physical activities (Chatzisarantis et al., 2002; Deci et al., 1999; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Because effort is associated with participation in physical activities, the results of the present study suggest that volitional intentions contribute to the prediction of physical activity indirectly via effort. Not only do the present findings suggest how the predictive efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour model could be improved, they also extend research in that they demonstrate the inadequacy of measures of intentions in representing volition (Meinland, 1970). In particular, researchers have not previously examined the assumption that intentions reflect

11 708 N. L. D. Chatzisarantis et al. willingness to participate in physical activities (Armitage & Conner, 2001; Hagger et al., 2002). In part, this is because researchers took for granted that intentions represent decisions that are volitional in nature (Ajzen, 1991). By comparing predictive validity of volitional intentions with intentions, the present study represents the first attempt to demonstrate empirically the insufficiency of measures of intentions in representing volition. In addition, models of health behaviour, such as the health belief model (Janz & Becker, 1984), protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1984), and the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen & Madden, 1986), consider various antecedents of health behaviour to result in the formulation of a uni-dimensional intention. As such, the effects of forced decisions on behaviour have been ignored by these models despite evidence supporting effects of forced decisions on behaviour (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959; Ryan et al., 1992). Hence, by measuring forced intentions, in the present study we demonstrate empirically effects from forced intentions to effort. Most importantly, we found evidence for independent effects from volitional intentions and forced intentions over and above a host of other variables such as attitudes, subjective norms, perceptions of control, and past behaviour (H 3 ). Therefore, the present findings rule out rival hypotheses, and document that an extended theory of planned behaviour that includes past behaviour cannot represent volitional or forced decisions satisfactorily. Limitations and conclusions Although the present study had a number of strengths and extended the theory of planned behaviour in a variety of ways, it was not without limitations and caveats. On the basis of the present results, causal inferences between volitional intentions, forced intentions, and effort cannot be drawn because volitional and forced intentions were not manipulated experimentally. In addition, we are acutely aware that our participants did not make actual decisions and our measures need further examination. Thus future research should examine whether manipulations of volitional intentions, forced intentions, and intentions exert independent effects on physical activity. Furthermore, it is important to improve the content validity of volitional intentions and forced intentions measures by adding new items and/or improving the comprehensibility of the existing ones. This is particularly germane for certain items measuring forced intentions. For example, the comprehensibility of the items in order to please important others requests and because I would disappoint others if I do not exercise might have been problematic. The reliability analysis supports this contention given that compared with the reliability of volitional intentions, the reliability of forced intentions was low. In conclusion, in the present study we demonstrated that measures of volitional and forced intentions improved utility of intentions in predicting willingness to participate in physical activities. In addition, the findings suggest that effort predicts physical activity participation over and above intentions. Of most important, we observed effects of volitional intentions and of forced intentions on effort over and above attitudes, subjective norms, perceptions of control, and past behaviour. Because effort is associated with actual participation in physical activities, the current findings suggest that volitional intentions contribute to the prediction of physical activity indirectly via effort. The implications of these findings are that intentions do not represent volitional or forced decisions accurately and that measures of volitional and forced intentions improve the predictive efficacy of intentions. References Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behaviour. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behaviour (pp ). Heidelberg: Springer. Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 50, Ajzen, I. (1998). Models of human social behaviour and their application to health. Psychology and Health, 13, Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past on later behaviour: Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. E. (1991). Prediction of leisure participation from behavioural, normative, and control beliefs: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Leisure Sciences, 13, Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behaviour. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal directed behaviour: Attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioural control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & Muellerleile, P. A. (2001). Theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127, Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, Bagozzi, R. (1981). An examination of the validity of two models of attitude. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 16, Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., & Yi, Y. (1992). Appraisal processes in the enactment of intentions to use coupons. Psychology and Marketing, 9, Bagozzi, R. P., & Kimmel S. K. (1995). A comparison of leading theories for the prediction of goal directed behaviours. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34,

12 Volition and intentions 709 Bagozzi, R. P., & Warshaw, P. R. (1992). An examination of the etiology of the attitude behaviour relation for goal-directed behaviours. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 27, Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative properties of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Bentler, P. M. (1989). EQS structural equations program manual. Los Angeles, CA: BMDP Statistical Software. Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., & Karageorghis, C. (2002). The cognitive processes by which perceived locus of causality predicts participation in physical activity. Journal of Health Psychology, 7, Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Hillsadale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, Courneya, K. S., & McAuley, E. (1995). Cognitive mediators of the social influence exercise adherence relationship: A test of the theory of planned behaviour. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 18, Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and selfdetermination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Fan, X., Thompson, B., & Wang, L. (1999). Effects of sample size, estimation methods and model specification on structural equation modelling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1, Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, Ford, J., MacCallum, R., & Tait, M. (1986). The application of factor analysis in psychology: A critical review and analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39, Godin, G., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise behaviour in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10, Gollwiltzer, P. (1990). Action phases and mind sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Grayling, A. C. (2001). The meaning of things. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour in physical activity: Predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, Hausenblas, H. A., Carron, A. V., & Mack, D. E. (1997). Application of the Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behaviour to exercise behaviour: A meta analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, Jacobs, D. R., Ainsworth, B. E., Hartman, T. J., & Leon, A. S. (1993). A simultaneous evaluation of ten commonly used physical activity questionnaires. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 25, Janz, N., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief model: A decade later. Health Education Quarterly, 11, Kim, M. S., & Hunter, J. E. (1993). Relationships among attitudes, behavioural intentions, and behaviour: A metaanalysis of past research, part 2. Communication Research, 20, Kline, P. (1993). The handbook of psychological testing. London: Routledge. Kuhl, J., & Kazen, M. (1994). Self discrimination and memory: State orientation and false self-ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, Liska, A. E. (1984). A critical examination of the causal structure of the Fishbein/Ajzen attitude behaviour model. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47, Meinland, J. W. (1970). The nature of intentions. London: Methuen. Mulaik, A., & Millsap, R. E. (2000). Doing the four-step right. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 7, Rigdon, E. E. (1999). Using the Friedman method of ranks for model comparison in structural equation modelling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 3, Rogers, R. W. (1984). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology (pp ). New York: Guilford. Ryan, R., Koestner, R., & Deci, E. (1992). Ego-involved persistence: When free-choice behaviour is not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion, 15, Sansone, C., Sachau, D. A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruction on intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1998). Do intentions predict condom use? Meta-analysis and examination of six moderator variables. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Augmenting the Theory of Planned Behaviour: Roles for anticipated regret and descriptive norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, Sheeran, P., & Taylor, S. (1999). Predicting intentions to use condoms: A meta-analysis and comparison of the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendation and future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, Tang, T. L., & Baumeister, R. A. (1984). Effects of personal values, perceived surveillance, and task labels on task preference: The ideology of turning play into work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, van Ryn, M., Lytle, L. A., & Kirscht, J. P. (1996). A test of the Theory of Planned Behaviour for two health-related practices. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, Williams, G., Freedman, Z., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss and maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,

Rens van de Schoot a b, Peter Lugtig a & Joop Hox a a Department of Methods and Statistics, Utrecht

Rens van de Schoot a b, Peter Lugtig a & Joop Hox a a Department of Methods and Statistics, Utrecht This article was downloaded by: [University Library Utrecht] On: 15 May 2012, At: 01:20 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article was downloaded by: On: 6 January 2010 Access details: Access Details: Free Access Publisher Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

Online publication date: 11 October 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Online publication date: 11 October 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [George Mason University] On: 10 February 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917277981] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and

More information

The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study

The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study This article was downloaded by: [65.186.78.206] On: 10 April 2014, At: 17:16 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Online publication date: 19 May 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Online publication date: 19 May 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Patterson, David A.] On: 19 May 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 922426156] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

INVESTIGATING BUSINESS SCHOOLS INTENTIONS TO OFFER E-COMMERCE DEGREE-PROGRAMS

INVESTIGATING BUSINESS SCHOOLS INTENTIONS TO OFFER E-COMMERCE DEGREE-PROGRAMS INVESTIGATING BUSINESS SCHOOLS INTENTIONS TO OFFER E-COMMERCE DEGREE-PROGRAMS Jean Baptiste K. Dodor College of Business Jackson State University HTUjeandodor@yahoo.comUTH 601-354-1964 Darham S. Rana College

More information

Indices of Model Fit STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING 2013

Indices of Model Fit STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING 2013 Indices of Model Fit STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING 2013 Indices of Model Fit A recommended minimal set of fit indices that should be reported and interpreted when reporting the results of SEM analyses:

More information

Published online: 17 Jun 2010.

Published online: 17 Jun 2010. This article was downloaded by: [Sam Houston State University] On: 07 August 2014, At: 15:09 Publisher: Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

Examining the Savings Habits of Individuals with Present-Fatalistic Time Perspectives using the Theory of Planned Behavior

Examining the Savings Habits of Individuals with Present-Fatalistic Time Perspectives using the Theory of Planned Behavior Examining the Savings Habits of Individuals with Present-Fatalistic Time Perspectives using the Theory of Planned Behavior Robert H. Rodermund April 3, 2012 Lindenwood University 209 S. Kingshighway, Harmon

More information

How To Understand The History Of Part Time Business Studies

How To Understand The History Of Part Time Business Studies This article was downloaded by: [148.251.235.206] On: 27 August 2015, At: 06:33 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place,

More information

EFFECT OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERN & SOCIAL NORMS ON ENVIRONMENTAL FRIENDLY BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS

EFFECT OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERN & SOCIAL NORMS ON ENVIRONMENTAL FRIENDLY BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS 169 EFFECT OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERN & SOCIAL NORMS ON ENVIRONMENTAL FRIENDLY BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS Joshi Pradeep Assistant Professor, Quantum School of Business, Roorkee, Uttarakhand, India joshipradeep_2004@yahoo.com

More information

Using Learning from Work for Progression to Higher Education: a degree of experience

Using Learning from Work for Progression to Higher Education: a degree of experience This article was downloaded by: [148.251.235.206] On: 27 August 2015, At: 21:16 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place,

More information

The Relationships between Perceived Quality, Perceived Value, and Purchase Intentions A Study in Internet Marketing

The Relationships between Perceived Quality, Perceived Value, and Purchase Intentions A Study in Internet Marketing The Relationships between Quality, Value, and Purchase Intentions A Study in Internet Marketing Man-Shin Cheng, National Formosa University, Taiwan Helen Cripps, Edith Cowan University, Australia Cheng-Hsui

More information

ANALYSIS OF USER ACCEPTANCE OF A NETWORK MONITORING SYSTEM WITH A FOCUS ON ICT TEACHERS

ANALYSIS OF USER ACCEPTANCE OF A NETWORK MONITORING SYSTEM WITH A FOCUS ON ICT TEACHERS ANALYSIS OF USER ACCEPTANCE OF A NETWORK MONITORING SYSTEM WITH A FOCUS ON ICT TEACHERS Siti Rahayu Abdul Aziz 1, Mohamad Ibrahim 2, and Suhaimi Sauti 3 1 Universiti Teknologi MARA, Malaysia, rahayu@fskm.uitm.edu.my

More information

The Technology Acceptance Model with Online Learning for the Principals in Elementary Schools and Junior High Schools

The Technology Acceptance Model with Online Learning for the Principals in Elementary Schools and Junior High Schools The Technology Acceptance Model with Online Learning for the Principals in Elementary Schools and Junior High Schools RONG-JYUE FANG 1, HUA- LIN TSAI 2, CHI -JEN LEE 3, CHUN-WEI LU 4 1,2 Department of

More information

APPLYING THE TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL AND FLOW THEORY TO ONLINE E-LEARNING USERS ACCEPTANCE BEHAVIOR

APPLYING THE TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL AND FLOW THEORY TO ONLINE E-LEARNING USERS ACCEPTANCE BEHAVIOR APPLYING THE TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL AND FLOW THEORY TO ONLINE E-LEARNING USERS ACCEPTANCE BEHAVIOR Su-Houn Liu, Chung Yuan Christian University, vandy@mis.cycu.edu.tw Hsiu-Li Liao, Chung Yuan Christian

More information

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations September, 2002 (Revised January, 2006) Icek Ajzen Brief Description of the Theory of Planned Behavior According to the theory

More information

Structural Equation Modelling (SEM)

Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) (SEM) Aims and Objectives By the end of this seminar you should: Have a working knowledge of the principles behind causality. Understand the basic steps to building a Model of the phenomenon of interest.

More information

Online publication date: 15 March 2010

Online publication date: 15 March 2010 This article was downloaded by: [Swets Content Distribution] On: 17 September 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 925215345] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

UNDERSTANDING EXPLORATORY USE

UNDERSTANDING EXPLORATORY USE UNDERSTANDING EXPLORATORY USE OF ERP SYSTEMS 1 Rui D. Sousa Terry College of Business University of Georgia rsousa@uga.edu Dale L. Goodhue Terry College of Business University of Georgia dgoodhue@terry.uga.edu

More information

Factors affecting the Satisfaction of China s Mobile Services Industry Customer. Su-Chao Chang a, Chi-Min Chou a, *

Factors affecting the Satisfaction of China s Mobile Services Industry Customer. Su-Chao Chang a, Chi-Min Chou a, * Factors affecting the Satisfaction of China s Mobile Services Industry Customer Su-Chao Chang a, Chi-Min Chou a, * a Dept. of Business Administration, National Cheng Kung University, 1 University Rd.,

More information

WHAT IS A JOURNAL CLUB?

WHAT IS A JOURNAL CLUB? WHAT IS A JOURNAL CLUB? With its September 2002 issue, the American Journal of Critical Care debuts a new feature, the AJCC Journal Club. Each issue of the journal will now feature an AJCC Journal Club

More information

Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level

Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level psyct.psychopen.eu 2193-7281 Research Articles Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level Shalini Sisodia* a, Ira Das a [a] Department of Psychology,

More information

California Published online: 09 Jun 2014.

California Published online: 09 Jun 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Mr Neil Ribner] On: 10 June 2014, At: 20:58 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Battery Bias

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Battery Bias Glossary of Terms Ability A defined domain of cognitive, perceptual, psychomotor, or physical functioning. Accommodation A change in the content, format, and/or administration of a selection procedure

More information

www.engineerspress.com

www.engineerspress.com www.engineerspress.com Volume: 1 Issue: 6 Pages: 101-112 Analyzing the Effects of Electronic Human Resource Management on the Talent Management Strategies (Medical University of Isfahan as a Case Study)

More information

PHYSICIANS ACCEPTANCE OF WEB-BASED MEDICAL ASSESSMENT SYSTEMS: FINDINGS FROM A NATIONAL SURVEY

PHYSICIANS ACCEPTANCE OF WEB-BASED MEDICAL ASSESSMENT SYSTEMS: FINDINGS FROM A NATIONAL SURVEY PHYSICIANS ACCEPTANCE OF WEB-BASED MEDICAL ASSESSMENT SYSTEMS: FINDINGS FROM A NATIONAL SURVEY Bengisu Tulu School of Information Science Claremont Graduate University bengisu.tulu@cgu.edu Brian Hilton

More information

A COMPARISON ANALYSIS ON THE INTENTION TO CONTINUED USE OF A LIFELONG LEARNING WEBSITE

A COMPARISON ANALYSIS ON THE INTENTION TO CONTINUED USE OF A LIFELONG LEARNING WEBSITE International Journal of Electronic Business Management, Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 213-223 (2012) 213 A COMPARISON ANALYSIS ON THE INTENTION TO CONTINUED USE OF A LIFELONG LEARNING WEBSITE Hsiu-Li Liao * and

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[canadian Research Knowledge Network] On: 6 May 2008 Access Details: [subscription number 783016864] Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Health Behavior: Psychosocial Theories

Health Behavior: Psychosocial Theories Health Behavior: Psychosocial Theories S. Sutton a a University of Cambridge UK Available online 2002. Abstract A number of psychosocial theories has been developed to predict, explain, and change health

More information

Evgenia Theodotou Metropolitan College, Athens, Greece. Abstract. Introduction. Motivation to learn: the theoretical framework

Evgenia Theodotou Metropolitan College, Athens, Greece. Abstract. Introduction. Motivation to learn: the theoretical framework Vol.4, No.1. April 2014 pp. 17 21. Early years education: are young students intrinsically or extrinsically motivated towards school activities? A discussion about the effects of rewards on young children

More information

Attitude, Behavioral Intention and Usage: An Empirical Study of Taiwan Railway s Internet Ticketing System

Attitude, Behavioral Intention and Usage: An Empirical Study of Taiwan Railway s Internet Ticketing System Attitude, Behavioral Intention and Usage: An Empirical Study of Taiwan Railway s Internet Ticketing System Wen-Hung Wang Department of Shipping and Transportation Management National Taiwan Ocean University,

More information

The Relationship between Social Intelligence and Job Satisfaction among MA and BA Teachers

The Relationship between Social Intelligence and Job Satisfaction among MA and BA Teachers Kamla-Raj 2012 Int J Edu Sci, 4(3): 209-213 (2012) The Relationship between Social Intelligence and Job Satisfaction among MA and BA Teachers Soleiman Yahyazadeh-Jeloudar 1 and Fatemeh Lotfi-Goodarzi 2

More information

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan ABSTRACT The focus of this research is to determine the impact of

More information

[This document contains corrections to a few typos that were found on the version available through the journal s web page]

[This document contains corrections to a few typos that were found on the version available through the journal s web page] Online supplement to Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67,

More information

Simple Linear Regression Inference

Simple Linear Regression Inference Simple Linear Regression Inference 1 Inference requirements The Normality assumption of the stochastic term e is needed for inference even if it is not a OLS requirement. Therefore we have: Interpretation

More information

CHAPTER 5: CONSUMERS ATTITUDE TOWARDS ONLINE MARKETING OF INDIAN RAILWAYS

CHAPTER 5: CONSUMERS ATTITUDE TOWARDS ONLINE MARKETING OF INDIAN RAILWAYS CHAPTER 5: CONSUMERS ATTITUDE TOWARDS ONLINE MARKETING OF INDIAN RAILWAYS 5.1 Introduction This chapter presents the findings of research objectives dealing, with consumers attitude towards online marketing

More information

ASSESSMENT: Coaching Efficacy As Indicators Of Coach Education Program Needs

ASSESSMENT: Coaching Efficacy As Indicators Of Coach Education Program Needs March, 2003 Volume 5, Issue 1 ASSESSMENT: Coaching Efficacy As Indicators Of Coach Education Program Needs Lena Fung, Ph.D. Department of Physical Education Hong Kong Baptist University Hong Kong, SAR

More information

Factorial Invariance in Student Ratings of Instruction

Factorial Invariance in Student Ratings of Instruction Factorial Invariance in Student Ratings of Instruction Isaac I. Bejar Educational Testing Service Kenneth O. Doyle University of Minnesota The factorial invariance of student ratings of instruction across

More information

Validation of the MUSIC Model of Academic Motivation Inventory: A Measure of Students Motivation in College Courses

Validation of the MUSIC Model of Academic Motivation Inventory: A Measure of Students Motivation in College Courses Validation of the MUSIC Model of Academic Motivation Inventory: A Measure of Students Motivation in College Courses Brett D. Jones & Gary Skaggs Virginia Tech Research presented at the International Conference

More information

MAGNT Research Report (ISSN. 1444-8939) Vol.2 (Special Issue) PP: 213-220

MAGNT Research Report (ISSN. 1444-8939) Vol.2 (Special Issue) PP: 213-220 Studying the Factors Influencing the Relational Behaviors of Sales Department Staff (Case Study: The Companies Distributing Medicine, Food and Hygienic and Cosmetic Products in Arak City) Aram Haghdin

More information

Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance

Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance This chapter deals with the analysis of customers perception regarding health insurance and involves its examination at series of stages i.e.

More information

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data John Reece Discipline of Psychology School of Health Sciences Impetus for This Research Oh, can you study psychology

More information

DEVELOPING AN EXTENDED TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL: DOCTORS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS IN JORDAN

DEVELOPING AN EXTENDED TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL: DOCTORS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS IN JORDAN DEVELOPING AN EXTENDED TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE MODEL: DOCTORS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS IN JORDAN INTRODUCTION Fauziah Baharom 1, Ola T. Khorma 2, Haslina Mohd 3, and Mahmood G. Bashayreh

More information

Learning Attitude and Its Effect on Applying Cloud Computing Service to IT Education

Learning Attitude and Its Effect on Applying Cloud Computing Service to IT Education Learning Attitude and Its Effect on Applying Cloud Computing Service to IT Education Chen-Feng Wu Department of Information Management, Yu Da University No 168, Hsueh-fu Rd, Tanwen Village, Chaochiao Township,

More information

The Online Journal of New Horizons in Education Volume 3, Issue 3

The Online Journal of New Horizons in Education Volume 3, Issue 3 Undergraduates Who Have a Lower Perception of Controlling Time Fail To Adjust Time Estimation Even When Given Feedback Yoshihiro S. OKAZAKI [1], Tomoya IMURA [2], Masahiro TAKAMURA [3], Satoko TOKUNAGA

More information

Online publication date: 20 November 2009

Online publication date: 20 November 2009 This article was downloaded by: [Michigan State University] On: 17 December 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 908199210] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and

More information

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING *Zahra Gholami Ghareh Shiran 1, Ghodsi Ahghar 2, Afshin Ahramiyan 3, Afsaneh Boostan

More information

Time Management Does Not Matter For Academic Achievement Unless You Can Cope

Time Management Does Not Matter For Academic Achievement Unless You Can Cope DOI: 10.7763/IPEDR. 2014. V 78. 5 Time Management Does Not Matter For Academic Achievement Unless You Can Cope Azura Hamdan 1, Rohany Nasir 1, Rozainee Khairudin 1 and Wan Sharazad Wan Sulaiman 1 1 National

More information

Pragmatic Perspectives on the Measurement of Information Systems Service Quality

Pragmatic Perspectives on the Measurement of Information Systems Service Quality Pragmatic Perspectives on the Measurement of Information Systems Service Quality Analysis with LISREL: An Appendix to Pragmatic Perspectives on the Measurement of Information Systems Service Quality William

More information

An Investigation of the Factors Influencing the Adoption of E-Health Services:

An Investigation of the Factors Influencing the Adoption of E-Health Services: An Investigation of the Factors Influencing the Adoption of E-Health Services: The Case for Selected Hospitals in Lagos State, Nigeria 1 Senanu Okuboyejo & 2 Valerie Ochiotu Department of Computer and

More information

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES.

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. 277 CHAPTER VI COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. This chapter contains a full discussion of customer loyalty comparisons between private and public insurance companies

More information

Theories of Behavior Change

Theories of Behavior Change Theories of Behavior Change Defining Theories of Behavior Change Behavior change is often a goal for staff working directly with constituents, organizations, governments, or communities. Individuals charged

More information

Cloud Computing: A Comparison Between Educational Technology Experts' and Information Professionals' Perspectives

Cloud Computing: A Comparison Between Educational Technology Experts' and Information Professionals' Perspectives Noa Aharony 1 Cloud Computing: A Comparison Between Educational Technology Experts' and Information Professionals' Perspectives Noa Aharony Department of Information Science, Bar-Ilan University Noa.aharony@biu.ac.il

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[ebscohost EJS Content Distribution] On: 30 October 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 768320842] Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Overview of Factor Analysis

Overview of Factor Analysis Overview of Factor Analysis Jamie DeCoster Department of Psychology University of Alabama 348 Gordon Palmer Hall Box 870348 Tuscaloosa, AL 35487-0348 Phone: (205) 348-4431 Fax: (205) 348-8648 August 1,

More information

Effectiveness of positive psychology training in the increase of hardiness of female headed households

Effectiveness of positive psychology training in the increase of hardiness of female headed households Effectiveness of positive psychology training in the increase of hardiness of female headed households 1,2, Ghodsi Ahghar* 3 1.Department of counseling, Khozestan Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad

More information

Business Security Architecture: Weaving Information Security into Your Organization's Enterprise Architecture through SABSA

Business Security Architecture: Weaving Information Security into Your Organization's Enterprise Architecture through SABSA This article was downloaded by: [188.204.15.66] On: 20 February 2012, At: 01:40 Publisher: Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

A Study on Customer Orientation as Mediator between Emotional Intelligence and Service Performance in Banks

A Study on Customer Orientation as Mediator between Emotional Intelligence and Service Performance in Banks International Journal of Business and Management Invention ISSN (Online): 2319 8028, ISSN (Print): 2319 801X Volume 2 Issue 5 ǁ May. 2013ǁ PP.60-66 A Study on Customer Orientation as Mediator between Emotional

More information

E-learning: Students perceptions of online learning in hospitality programs. Robert Bosselman Hospitality Management Iowa State University ABSTRACT

E-learning: Students perceptions of online learning in hospitality programs. Robert Bosselman Hospitality Management Iowa State University ABSTRACT 1 E-learning: Students perceptions of online learning in hospitality programs Sungmi Song Hospitality Management Iowa State University Robert Bosselman Hospitality Management Iowa State University ABSTRACT

More information

Coaching Efficacy and Coaching Effectiveness: Examining Their Predictors and Comparing Coaches and Athletes Reports

Coaching Efficacy and Coaching Effectiveness: Examining Their Predictors and Comparing Coaches and Athletes Reports applied research The Sport Psychologist, 008,, 383-404 008 Human Kinetics, Inc. Coaching Efficacy and Coaching Effectiveness: Examining Their Predictors and Comparing Coaches and Athletes Reports Maria

More information

Running head: SCHOOL COMPUTER USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE. Using the U.S. PISA results to investigate the relationship between

Running head: SCHOOL COMPUTER USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE. Using the U.S. PISA results to investigate the relationship between Computer Use and Academic Performance- PISA 1 Running head: SCHOOL COMPUTER USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE Using the U.S. PISA results to investigate the relationship between school computer use and student

More information

Charitable Giving: The Effectiveness of a Revised Theory of Planned Behaviour Model in. Predicting Donating Intentions and Behaviour. Joanne R.

Charitable Giving: The Effectiveness of a Revised Theory of Planned Behaviour Model in. Predicting Donating Intentions and Behaviour. Joanne R. 1 Running Head: CHARITABLE GIVING AND THE TPB Charitable Giving: The Effectiveness of a Revised Theory of Planned Behaviour Model in Predicting Donating Intentions and Behaviour Joanne R. Smith Andreè

More information

in nigerian companies.

in nigerian companies. Information Management 167 in nigerian companies. Idris, Adekunle. A. Abstract: Keywords: Relationship Marketing, Customer loyalty, Customer Service, Relationship Marketing Strategy and Nigeria. Introduction

More information

Factors affecting professor facilitator and course evaluations in an online graduate program

Factors affecting professor facilitator and course evaluations in an online graduate program Factors affecting professor facilitator and course evaluations in an online graduate program Amy Wong and Jason Fitzsimmons U21Global, Singapore Along with the rapid growth in Internet-based instruction

More information

Applications of Structural Equation Modeling in Social Sciences Research

Applications of Structural Equation Modeling in Social Sciences Research American International Journal of Contemporary Research Vol. 4 No. 1; January 2014 Applications of Structural Equation Modeling in Social Sciences Research Jackson de Carvalho, PhD Assistant Professor

More information

The Effect of Online Social Networking on Facilitating Sense of Belonging among University Students Living Off Campus

The Effect of Online Social Networking on Facilitating Sense of Belonging among University Students Living Off Campus The Effect of Online Social Networking on Facilitating Sense of Belonging among University Students Living Off Campus Kine Dorum Craig Bartle Martin Pennington University of Leicester, UK kd41@leicester.ac.uk

More information

The Theory of Planned Behavior

The Theory of Planned Behavior ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR AND HUMAN DECISION PROCESSES 50, 179-211 (1991) The Theory of Planned Behavior ICEK AJZEN University of Massachusetts at Amherst Research dealing with various aspects of* the theory

More information

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior Icek Ajzen Brief Description of the Theory of Planned Behavior According to the theory, human behavior is guided by three kinds of considerations:

More information

http://jist.irandoc.ac.ir *adelsulaimany@gmail.com 1. zarafshani2000@yahoo.com 2. Theory Planned Behavior (TPB) TPB

http://jist.irandoc.ac.ir *adelsulaimany@gmail.com 1. zarafshani2000@yahoo.com 2. Theory Planned Behavior (TPB) TPB * 1 1389/06/ 29 : 1389/05/05 : 2 251-8 223 ( ) 2 251-8 231 ( ) ISC SCOPUS L ISA http://jist.irandoc.ac.ir 343-325 2 27 1390 : *adelsulaimany@gmail.com 1. zarafshani2000@yahoo.com 2. Theory Planned Behavior

More information

DOES ONLINE TRADING AFFECT INVESTORS TRADING INTENTION? Ya-Hui Wang, National Chin-Yi University of Technology

DOES ONLINE TRADING AFFECT INVESTORS TRADING INTENTION? Ya-Hui Wang, National Chin-Yi University of Technology The International Journal of Business and Finance Research VOLUME 8 NUMBER 5 2014 DOES ONLINE TRADING AFFECT INVESTORS TRADING INTENTION? Ya-Hui Wang, National Chin-Yi University of Technology ABSTRACT

More information

Predicting Undergraduate Nursing Students Intention to Use the Electronic Health Records Software Application

Predicting Undergraduate Nursing Students Intention to Use the Electronic Health Records Software Application Predicting Undergraduate Nursing Students Intention to Use the Electronic Health Records Software Application Yanika Kowitlawakul a and Sally Chan a Alice Lee Centre for Nursing Studies/Yong Loo Lin School

More information

Comparing Two Theories of Health Behavior: A Prospective Study of Noncompletion of Treatment Following Cervical Cancer Screening

Comparing Two Theories of Health Behavior: A Prospective Study of Noncompletion of Treatment Following Cervical Cancer Screening Health Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 25, No. 5, 604 615 0278-6133/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.604 Comparing Two Theories of Health Behavior: A

More information

The Relationship between Performance Management and Job Motivation of the Staff Working in Petrochemical Company

The Relationship between Performance Management and Job Motivation of the Staff Working in Petrochemical Company International Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Research. Vol., 3(6), 620-625, 2014 Available online at http:// www. ijpbrjournal.com ISSN 2322-4002 2014 Victor Quest Publications The Relationship between

More information

A revalidation of the SET37 questionnaire for student evaluations of teaching

A revalidation of the SET37 questionnaire for student evaluations of teaching Educational Studies Vol. 35, No. 5, December 2009, 547 552 A revalidation of the SET37 questionnaire for student evaluations of teaching Dimitri Mortelmans and Pieter Spooren* Faculty of Political and

More information

Perception of drug addiction among Turkish university students: Causes, cures, and attitudes

Perception of drug addiction among Turkish university students: Causes, cures, and attitudes Addictive Behaviors 30 (2005) 1 8 Perception of drug addiction among Turkish university students: Causes, cures, and attitudes Okan Cem Çırakoğlu*, Güler Içnsın Faculty of Economic and Administrative Sciences,

More information

The Influence of Stressful Life Events of College Students on Subjective Well-Being: The Mediation Effect of the Operational Effectiveness

The Influence of Stressful Life Events of College Students on Subjective Well-Being: The Mediation Effect of the Operational Effectiveness Open Journal of Social Sciences, 2016, 4, 70-76 Published Online June 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/jss http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jss.2016.46008 The Influence of Stressful Life Events of

More information

SEM Analysis of the Impact of Knowledge Management, Total Quality Management and Innovation on Organizational Performance

SEM Analysis of the Impact of Knowledge Management, Total Quality Management and Innovation on Organizational Performance 2015, TextRoad Publication ISSN: 2090-4274 Journal of Applied Environmental and Biological Sciences www.textroad.com SEM Analysis of the Impact of Knowledge Management, Total Quality Management and Innovation

More information

A STUDY ON CONSUMER BEHAVIOUR TOWARDS ONLINE SHOPPING IN COIMBATORE DISTRICT

A STUDY ON CONSUMER BEHAVIOUR TOWARDS ONLINE SHOPPING IN COIMBATORE DISTRICT IMPACT: International Journal of Research in Business Management (IMPACT: IJRBM) ISSN(E): 2321-886X; ISSN(P): 2347-4572 Vol. 3, Issue 7, Jul 2015, 51-62 Impact Journals A STUDY ON CONSUMER BEHAVIOUR TOWARDS

More information

EXAMINING HEALTHCARE PROFESSIONALS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS USING UTAUT

EXAMINING HEALTHCARE PROFESSIONALS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS USING UTAUT EXAMINING HEALTHCARE PROFESSIONALS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS USING UTAUT Matthew J. Wills, Dakota State University Omar F. El-Gayar, Dakota State University Dorine Bennett, Dakota State

More information

PERSONALITY TRAITS AS FACTORS AFFECTING E-BOOK ADOPTION AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS

PERSONALITY TRAITS AS FACTORS AFFECTING E-BOOK ADOPTION AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS PERSONALITY TRAITS AS FACTORS AFFECTING E-BOOK ADOPTION AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS Nurkaliza Khalid Fakulti Sains dan Teknologi Maklumat Kolej Universiti Islam Antarabangsa Selangor nurkaliza@kuis.edu.my ABSTRACT

More information

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive Statistics Descriptive Statistics Primer Descriptive statistics Central tendency Variation Relative position Relationships Calculating descriptive statistics Descriptive Statistics Purpose to describe or summarize

More information

Dual. Utilitarian. Productivity. Fun

Dual. Utilitarian. Productivity. Fun An Interdisciplinary Journal on Humans in ICT Environments ISSN: 1795-6889 www.humantechnology.jyu.fi Volume 2 (2), October 2006, 225 235 AN ACCEPTANCE MODEL FOR USEFUL AND FUN INFORMATION SYSTEMS Thomas

More information

Issues in Information Systems Volume 16, Issue I, pp. 163-169, 2015

Issues in Information Systems Volume 16, Issue I, pp. 163-169, 2015 A Task Technology Fit Model on e-learning Linwu Gu, Indiana University of Pennsylvania, lgu@iup.edu Jianfeng Wang, Indiana University of Pennsylvania, jwang@iup.edu ABSTRACT In this research, we propose

More information

Effects of Teaching through Online Teacher versus Real Teacher on Student Learning in the Classroom

Effects of Teaching through Online Teacher versus Real Teacher on Student Learning in the Classroom Effects of Teaching through Online Teacher versus Real Teacher on Student Learning in the Classroom Sirous Hadadnia Islamic Azad University-Mamasani Branch, Iran Norouz Hadadnia Zargan Office of Education,

More information

INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION

INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION INTERNAL MARKETING ESTABLISHES A CULTURE OF LEARNING ORGANIZATION Yafang Tsai, Department of Health Policy and Management, Chung-Shan Medical University, Taiwan, (886)-4-24730022 ext.12127, avon611@gmail.com

More information

Technological Acceptance and Consumer's Behavior on Buying Online Insurance

Technological Acceptance and Consumer's Behavior on Buying Online Insurance International Conference on ebusiness, ecommerce, emanagement, elearning and egovernance [IC5E] 112 International Conference on ebusiness, ecommerce, emanagement, elearning and egovernance 2015 [IC5E 2015]

More information

Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology

Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology Conceptualising and Modelling Virtual Experience for the Online Retailer: The 3D Technology INTRODUCTION Previous studies (e.g., Jiang & Benbasat, 2005; Algharabat & Dennis, 2010 a, b, c) regarding online

More information

Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands b Universitat Jaume I, Castellon, Spain. Online publication date: 13 February 2011

Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands b Universitat Jaume I, Castellon, Spain. Online publication date: 13 February 2011 This article was downloaded by: [University of Utrecht] On: 15 February 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 907217953] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

RECRUITERS PRIORITIES IN PLACING MBA FRESHER: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

RECRUITERS PRIORITIES IN PLACING MBA FRESHER: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS RECRUITERS PRIORITIES IN PLACING MBA FRESHER: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS Miss Sangeeta Mohanty Assistant Professor, Academy of Business Administration, Angaragadia, Balasore, Orissa, India ABSTRACT Recruitment

More information

Perceived Autonomy Support in Physical Education and Leisure-Time Physical Activity: A Cross-Cultural Evaluation of the Trans-Contextual Model

Perceived Autonomy Support in Physical Education and Leisure-Time Physical Activity: A Cross-Cultural Evaluation of the Trans-Contextual Model Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 97, No. 3, 376 390 0022-0663/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.376 Perceived Autonomy Support in

More information

Constructing a Technology Readiness Scale for Sports Center RFID Door Security System Users

Constructing a Technology Readiness Scale for Sports Center RFID Door Security System Users Constructing a Technology Readiness Scale for Sports Center RFID Door Security System Users Mu-Cheng Wu, National Chin-Yi University of Technology, Taiwan Chao-Chien Chen, Asia University, Taiwan Chih-Hung

More information

NASPE Sets the Standard

NASPE Sets the Standard This article was downloaded by: [Bowling Green SU] On: 25 March 2015, At: 09:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Organizational Commitment among Public and Private School Teachers

Organizational Commitment among Public and Private School Teachers The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 2, Issue 3, Paper ID: B00381V2I32015 http://www.ijip.in April to June 2015 ABSTRACT: Organizational Commitment

More information

Are School Level Supports for Teachers and Teacher Collegiality Related to Other School Climate Characteristics and Student Academic Performance?

Are School Level Supports for Teachers and Teacher Collegiality Related to Other School Climate Characteristics and Student Academic Performance? S3 Factsheet Are School Level Supports for Teachers and Teacher Collegiality Related to Other School Climate Characteristics and Student Academic Performance? Effective learning conditions for students

More information

Influence of information search on risky investment preferences: Testing a moderating role of income

Influence of information search on risky investment preferences: Testing a moderating role of income 2011 3rd International Conference on Information and Financial Engineering IPEDR vol.12 (2011) (2011) IACSIT Press, Singapore Influence of information search on risky investment preferences: Testing a

More information

A STUDY ON MODELING INVESTORS BEHAVIOR TOWARDS ONLINE SHARE TRADING, COIMBATORE

A STUDY ON MODELING INVESTORS BEHAVIOR TOWARDS ONLINE SHARE TRADING, COIMBATORE INTERNATIONAL International Journal of Marketing JOURNAL and Human OF Resource MARKETING Management (IJMHRM), AND HUMAN ISSN 0976 RESOURCE 6421 (Print), MANAGEMENT (IJMHRM) ISSN 0976 6421 (Print) ISSN

More information

Daring Greatly: How the Courage to Be Vulnerable Transforms the Way We Live, Love, Parent, and Lead. Click for updates

Daring Greatly: How the Courage to Be Vulnerable Transforms the Way We Live, Love, Parent, and Lead. Click for updates This article was downloaded by: [184.100.72.114] On: 19 January 2015, At: 17:22 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Relationship Quality as Predictor of B2B Customer Loyalty. Shaimaa S. B. Ahmed Doma

Relationship Quality as Predictor of B2B Customer Loyalty. Shaimaa S. B. Ahmed Doma Relationship Quality as Predictor of B2B Customer Loyalty Shaimaa S. B. Ahmed Doma Faculty of Commerce, Business Administration Department, Alexandria University Email: Shaimaa_ahmed24@yahoo.com Abstract

More information

A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics

A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics A Casual Structure Analysis of Smart phone Addiction: Use Motives of Smart phone Users and Psychological Characteristics Dept. of Social Welfare, BaekSeok Culture University, ijoo65@hanmail.net Abstract

More information