An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks



Similar documents
PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Overview of Methodology. Human Electrophysiology. Computing and Displaying Difference Waves. Plotting The Averaged ERP

On the control of visual spatial attention: evidence from human electrophysiology

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Ten Simple Rules for Designing and Interpreting ERP Experiments Steven J. Luck University of Iowa

The Effects of Reading Speed on Visual Search Task

The electrophysiology of introspection q

Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition

Decoding Information Processing When Attention Fails: An Electrophysiological Approach

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics

The Capacity of Visual Short- Term Memory Is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects G.A. Alvarez and P.

ERPLAB TOOLBOX TUTORIAL

Visual Singleton Detection ( Pop-out ) is Mediated by Dimensionbased

Università di Padova, Padova, Italy b Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Localization of temporal preparation effects via trisected reaction time

Measuring and modeling attentional functions

Curriculum Vitae. Adriane E. Seiffert. January 5, 2010

PERSPECTIVE. How Top-Down is Visual Perception?

Subjects. Subjects were undergraduates at the University of California, Santa Barbara, with

Adaptive control of event integration: Evidence from event-related potentials

ERPs in Cognitive Neuroscience

Pop-Out Without Awareness: Unseen Feature Singletons Capture Attention Only When Top-Down Attention Is Available

Visual Attention and Emotional Perception

Electrophysiological responses to visuospatial regularity

The Binding Problem Solutions to the spatial binding problem

Data Analysis Methods: Net Station 4.1 By Peter Molfese

Video-Based Eye Tracking

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

ARTICLE IN PRESS Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx xxx

1 An Introduction to Event-Related Potentials and Their Neural Origins

Perceptual Processes in Matching and Recognition of Complex Pictures

Andrea Greve, a, Mark C.W. van Rossum, a,1 and David I. Donaldson b,2. Introduction

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention

The Psychonomic Society, Inc. Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee

The influence of scene organization on attention: Psychophysics and electrophysiology

Memory for perceived and imagined pictures an event-related potential study

3. Methods. 2. The study Participants

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. A NEURAL BASIS FOR EXPERT OBJECT RECOGNITION James W. Tanaka 1 and Tim Curran 2. Participants.

On the representation of words and nonwords in visual short-term memory: Evidence from human electrophysiology

Brain and Cognition 71 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain and Cognition. journal homepage:

Electrophysiological correlates of categorization: P300 amplitude as index of target similarity

Powered by TCPDF (

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind

Factorial Design. A factorial design Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Effect of Attraction x Emotion

HOW AUTOMATICALLY IS MEANING ACCESSED: A REVIEW OF THE EFFECTS OF ATTENTION ON SEMANTIC PROCESSING

False discovery rate and permutation test: An evaluation in ERP data analysis

PhD student at the Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Department of Psychology,

Behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging studies

Making Two Responses to a Single Object: Implications for the Central Attentional Bottleneck

SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT. A Thesis

Documentation Wadsworth BCI Dataset (P300 Evoked Potentials) Data Acquired Using BCI2000's P3 Speller Paradigm (

Time Window from Visual Images to Visual Short-Term Memory: Consolidation or Integration?


Online Chapter 11 A Closer Look at Averaging: Convolution, Latency Variability, and Overlap

Andreas Wutz a, Alfonso Caramazza a b & David Melcher a a Centre for Mind and Brain Sciences, University of

Agent Simulation of Hull s Drive Theory

Brain dynamics associated with recollective experiences of emotional events Mathias Weymar a, Andreas Löw a, Lars Schwabe b and Alfons O.

Artifact Removal Procedure Distorts Multifocal Electroretinogram

Programs for diagnosis and therapy of visual field deficits in vision rehabilitation

An ERP study of sustained spatial attention to stimulus eccentricity

History of eye-tracking in psychological research

Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: An ERP study. Birkbeck University of London, UK

Music matters: Preattentive musicality of the human brain

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data.

NeuroImage 62 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Single Factor design. Single design experiment. Experimental design. Textbook Chapters

Toward Construction of an Inexpensive Brain Computer Interface for Goal Oriented Applications

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology Doctoral School

Electrophysiological Activity Generated During the Implicit Association Test: A Study Using

Semantic Anomalies in Second Language Processing: An Electrophysiological Study*

Subjects: Fourteen Princeton undergraduate and graduate students were recruited to

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion. Macaque visual areas

Linguistic Mediation of Visual Search: Effects of Speech Timing and Display

The Time Course of Consolidation in Visual Working Memory

Prospectus for Recording EEG and ERP in Infants

Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: an ERP study

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

CELL PHONE INDUCED PERCEPTUAL IMPAIRMENTS DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

Visual spatial search task (VISSTA): a computerized assessment and training program

Biological Psychology 72 (2006) Methodology. Thomas P. Urbach a, *, Marta Kutas a,b

Jonathan Robert Folstein, Ph.D Macalester College, St. Paul, Minnesota B.A., Philosophy

ALTERING THE NEAR-MISS EFFECT IN SLOT MACHINE GAMBLERS MARK R. DIXON, BECKY L. NASTALLY, JAMES E. JACKSON, AND REZA HABIB

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain).

Working Memory Capacity and Attention Network Test Performance

Electrophysiological evidence for a natural/artifactual dissociation

Bernice E. Rogowitz and Holly E. Rushmeier IBM TJ Watson Research Center, P.O. Box 704, Yorktown Heights, NY USA

It's Great But Not Necessarily About Attention

International Journal of Psychophysiology

PERCEPTUAL RECOGNITION AS A FUNCTION OF MEANINGFULNESS OF STIMULUS MATERIAL '

The Spatial Representation of Natural Scenes Helene Intraub Department of Psychology University of Delaware Newark, DE 19716

The Effects of Cell Phone Distraction on Cognitive Tasks. Yu Lee, Chanda Atkinson, Danielle Hritsko, Kobe Acquaah

The question of whether faces are processed by qualitatively

Using Neuroscience to Understand the Role of Direct Mail

Detection performance in pop-out tasks: Nonmonotonic changes with display size and eccentricity

Adaptive information source selection during hypothesis testing

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex

Effects of Orientation Disparity Between Haptic and Graphic Displays of Objects in Virtual Environments

Principles of Data Visualization

Transcription:

Psychophysiology, 46 (2009), 771 775. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2009 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00814.x BRIEF REPORT An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks VERONICA MAZZA, a,b MASSIMO TURATTO, a,b and ALFONSO CARAMAZZA a,c a Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, Trento, Italy b Department of Cognitive Sciences and Education, University of Trento, Trento, Italy c Cognitive Neuropsychology Laboratory, Harvard University, Cambridge, USA Abstract We investigated whether the N2pc is unequivocally linked to distractor-suppression mechanisms, as is commonly assumed. According to the distractor-suppression account of the N2pc, no suppression, and thus no N2pc, should occur when homogeneous distractors help in selecting the target, such as when the target feature is unpredictable. Participants performed a simple detection or a finer discrimination on a singleton target, which had either a variable or a constant color. Contrary to the distractor-suppression account, an N2pc was present for both the variable and the constant conditions, and for both tasks. Additionally, target feature consistency correlated with earlier N2pc onsets relative to variable blocks. Both results indicate that the N2pc is not unequivocally linked to distractor-suppression mechanisms, but may index mechanisms involved in identifying and localizing relevant stimuli through enhancement of their features. Descriptors: Distractor suppression, Attentional orienting, Visual search, N2pc, Feature repetition, Visual working memory, Event-related brain potentials Many theories propose that attention operates as a spatial filter of the incoming inputs, suppressing the information relative to distractor objects and thus maximizing the processing of the relevant objects (Desimone & Duncan, 1995). In the ERP field, Luck and colleagues proposed the Ambiguity Resolution Theory (ART; Luck et al., 1997), which posits that attention acts by suppressing the inputs from distractor objects. The N2pc component (a more pronounced negativity occurring at 180 300 ms post-stimulus at posterior sites contralateral to the target location relative to ipsilateral sites) has been considered as the ERP counterpart of such suppressive mechanisms (see Luck, 2005). More specifically, the N2pc would occur whenever interfering distractors are presented with the target, and its amplitude would increase with increasing interference and increasing need for suppression. Crucially, the ART predicts that distractors are not suppressed when they are useful to determine the presence of a target (Luck, 2005). For example, when the target feature is not known in advance, such as when it varies unpredictably from trial to trial, the visual system would not suppress distractors as they would be helpful in selecting the target, and no N2pc should be observed (Luck & Hillyard, 1994a). Not all previous evidence is consistent with this prediction. Whereas Luck and Hillyard This research has been supported by a grant from the Fondazione Cassa di Risparmio di Trento e Rovereto, Italy, and from the Ministero dell Istruzione, dell Università e della Ricerca, Italy. Address reprint requests to: Veronica Mazza, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy. E-mail: veronica.mazza@unitn.it 771 (1994a, Experiment 2) found no N2pc when the target feature varied unpredictably throughout a block of trials, other researchers failed to find such a modulation (e.g., Schubo et al., 2007). In a recent study, Mazza, Turatto, and Caramazza (2009) manipulated whether the target feature (color) was kept constant or varied unpredictably. Contrary to the predictions of the ART, no modulation of the N2pc as a function of target-color variability emerged. However, whereas in Luck and Hillyard s experiment (1994a) participants had to simply detect the target s presence, in our experiment they performed the additional operation of discriminating the target s shape from similarly shaped distractors. This additional discrimination component might require suppression of distractors also in the variable target condition and, therefore, N2pc amplitude modulation (see Leblanc, Prime, & Jolicoeur, 2008). For this reason, in the present study we compared the variable and constant conditions both in a detection and in a discrimination task. In the detection task, participants reported the presence/absence of a uniquely colored diamond; in the discrimination task, the uniquely colored diamond was presented on each trial, and participants judged its shape. Within each task, the target (when present) could have the same color for an entire block of trials (constant condition) or could vary its color unpredictably (variable condition). According to the ART, we should find a modulation of the N2pc amplitude as a function of target variability, with a clear N2pc for the constant condition, where distractors supposedly represent a real source of interference and should thus be suppressed; in addition, we should not

772 V. Mazza et al. find an N2pc for the variable condition (at least for the detection task) because distractors should help in determining the target s presence and thus should not be suppressed (Luck, 2005). Methods Participants Sixteen volunteers, aged 19 35 years and with normal or corrected vision, participated in the experiment, after providing informed consent. The study was approved by the University of Trento Ethics Committee. Stimuli and Procedure Stimuli consisted of equiluminant red and green diamonds (17 cd/m 2 ) presented on a black background (1 cd/m 2 ). Each diamond (0.61 0.81) had a 0.41 corner trimmed on the left or right side (see Figure 1a). On each trial, the display contained 20 diamonds, equally distributed to the left and right side of fixation. The diamonds were located within a 10 (columns, 11.41) 8 (rows, 8.61) matrix. On half of the trials of the detection task (target-present trials), one of the diamonds (the target) had a unique color (either red or green) and appeared with equal probability and in random order to the left or right of fixation, with the restriction that it always occurred either in the third or fourth column of the matrix relative to fixation. On the other half of the trials (target-absent trials), 20 homogeneously colored diamonds (either red or green) were presented. In the discrimination task, the target was presented on each trial. In both tasks, there were variable and constant blocks. In the variable blocks, the target varied unpredictably in color, sometimes red and sometimes green. In the constant blocks, it always had the same color, either red or green (counterbalanced across participants). Each visual display was presented for 150 ms. In the detection task, participants reported whether the target was present or absent by pressing one of two keys on a computer keyboard with their left or right hand. In the discrimination task, they indicated the side of the cut (left/right) of the target by pressing one of the two keys used in the detection task. In both tasks, speed and accuracy were emphasized equally. Maximum time for responding was 1,500 ms. The inter-trial interval was 1,500 ms. In the detection task, participants performed ten blocks (five variable and five constant) of 112 trials per block; in the discrimination task, ten blocks (five variable and five constant) with 56 trials per block were delivered. The detection task was always performed first, to ensure that an N2pc for this task is not simply a carryover effect from the discrimination task where we expect to find an N2pc (Mazza et al., 2009). The order of the target-color variability (either variable or constant) was counterbalanced across participants. Two training blocks of 20 trials, one for each task, were delivered prior to the start of each part of the experimental session. EEG Recording and Data Analysis EEG was recorded from 25 electrodes (including the following posterior sites: P7, PO7, P3, O1, O2, P4, PO8, and P8). These electrodes and the left earlobe electrode were recorded with a right-earlobe reference, and then re-referenced offline to the average of the left and right earlobe sites. Horizontal electrooculogram (HEOG) was recorded with two electrodes positioned on the outer canthi of both eyes. Impedance was kept below 6 KO for all electrodes. Amplifier bandpass was 0.01 40 Hz, and digitization rate was 500 Hz. Trials with horizontal eye movements (HEOG exceeding 30 mv), eye blinks, and other artifacts (any electrode exceeding 80 mv) were excluded. The average of trials retained was 86%. Averages for correct responses were computed relative to the 100-ms interval preceding the display onset, separately for each task and target-color variability. Statistical analyses were conducted on mean difference amplitudes obtained by subtracting event-related potential (ERP) waveforms at ipsilateral posterior electrodes (PO7, P7, P3, O1 for left target locations and PO8, P8, P4, O2 for right target locations) from those recorded at contralateral sites (i.e., PO8, P8, P4, O2 for left targets and PO7, P7, P3, O1 for right targets) for the following post-stimulus intervals: N2pc (180 300 ms), late sustained contralateral negativity (350 600 ms). The factors considered were electrode (PO7/PO8 versus P7/P8 versus P3/P4 versus O1/O2), task (detection versus discrimination), and target-color variability (variable versus constant). Greenhouse-Geisser corrections for sphericity violations were applied, and corrected p-values are reported. Results Figure 1. (A) Example of trials in the variable (first and third row) and constant (second and fourth row) conditions, separately for detection and discrimination. (B) Mean RTs (with standard errors) for the variable (gray) and constant conditions (black) in both tasks. Behavioral Performance An ANOVA on correct response times (RTs) between 200 1,500 ms for target-present trials with task (detection versus discrimination) and target variability (variable versus constant) as

N2pc and distractor suppression 773 Figure 2. (A) Grand-average ERP waveforms obtained in the 600 ms post-stimulus interval at two representative posterior electrodes (left: PO7/PO8; right: P7/P8) contralateral (solid lines) and ipsilateral (dashed lines) to the target location, as a function of target-color variability and task. (B) Topographical ERP scalp distribution maps of the N2pc (200 300 ms post-stimulus) in the four experimental conditions, obtained by computing the contralateral minus ipsilateral difference activity for each electrode pair, and mirrored across the midline. (C) The grand-average difference waveforms (obtained by subtracting ipsilateral activations from contralateral activations recorded at posterior sites PO7 and PO8, collapsed across tasks) show the earlier N2pc onset in the constant relative to the variable condition (as indicated by the rectangle). factors revealed a significant main effect of task, F(1,15) 5 10.49, p 5.006: participants were faster in the detection (M 5 536 ms, SD 5 82.7 ms) than in the discrimination condition (M 5 577 ms, SD 5 68 ms; see Figure 1b). In both tasks, RTs were also faster on constant (M 5 550 ms, SD 5 75.1 ms) than on variable trials (M 5 563 ms, SD 5 69.7 ms), F(1,15) 5 5.67, p 5.03. The interaction was not significant, Fo1. Participants were overall highly accurate in both tasks (96% of correct responses). The ANOVA on percentage of correct responses showed a marginally significant effect of target variability, F(1,15) 5 4, p 5.06: participants were slightly more accurate on constant (M 5 96%) than on variable trials (M 5 95%). No other effect approached significance, all ps4.65. ERP Results As can be seen from Figure 2A, the N2pc amplitude was affected by the task, being larger for discrimination than for detection; no difference, however, emerged between the variable and constant conditions (also see Figure 2B). A subsequent sustained contralateral negativity (350 600 ms) was observable for the discrimination task only. Confirming these observations, the ANOVA in the N2pc time window (180 300 ms) showed a significant effect of task, F(1,15) 5 8.2, p 5.01, with a more pronounced N2pc for discrimination than detection. Post-hoc comparisons (t-tests) against the null mean confirmed the presence of the N2pc for both detection and discrimination, both pso.001. The effect of electrode was significant, F(3,45) 5 6.3, p 5.009, with an overall more pronounced N2pc at the most lateral electrode sites (PO7/PO8 and P7/P8) than at the most medial sites (P3/P4 and O1/O2; see Figure 2B). No other effects reached significance, all ps4.13. To assess for differential effects of target-color variability on the N2pc onset, mean difference values at posterior electrodes (collapsed across sites) were tested (via t-test) against the null mean for each successive 5-ms time window in the 170 210 ms time range. The a level for the number of comparisons was adjusted following the False Discovery Rate procedure (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995), which controls for the ratio between the number of null hypotheses erroneously rejected and the overall rejected hypotheses. Whereas for the constant condition an N2pc was present from 175 ms onwards (i.e., mean difference values significantly differed from zero for all time windows after that point, all pso.013), no reliable N2pc was found for the variable

774 V. Mazza et al. condition until 195 ms (all ps4.06 for the 170 190 ms range; see Figure 2C). 1 In the subsequent sustained contralaterality (350 600 ms), the effect of task was still significant, F(1,15) 5 71.3, po.001, with a more pronounced activity for the discrimination task. However, comparisons against the null mean revealed that the contralateral activation was present for the discrimination task only, all t(15)43.9, all pso.001 (detection task: all tso1.6, all ps4.14). Discussion There are several new findings in this study. First, the results challenge one of the assumptions of the ART, according to which the N2pc amplitude should be modulated as a function of target variability, with an N2pc for the constant but not for the variable condition (Luck, 2005). Our data clearly show the presence of an N2pc for both the constant and variable conditions, regardless of whether distractors are strategically useful for target selection. 2 Schubo et al. (2007) also found a clear N2pc in a detection task with color or orientation singleton targets, which changed their features unpredictably. A potential explanation for the discrepancy between the present results and those by Luck & Hillyard (1994a) is suggested by Schubo et al. s (2007) results, who found that orientation singletons elicited much smaller N2pc amplitudes than color singletons. In our experiment, the defining dimension of the target was color, whereas it was orientation in Luck and Hillyard s (1994a) experiment. It is possible, therefore, that the absence of an N2pc in Luck and Hillyard s study could be due to an overall lack of power in the N2pc amplitude, perhaps due to the smaller sample of participants relative to the Schubo et al. study. The present finding has important consequences for the interpretation of the N2pc. One possibility, consistent with the distractor-suppression account of the N2pc, would be to assume that distractors are always suppressed, independently of whether or not they benefit target selection. Alternatively, we propose that the N2pc reflects the allocation of extra computational resources to one hemifield in order to analyze the target features in detail (see also Eimer, 1996; Mazza et al., 2009). Some theories of visual attention (e.g., Duncan & Humphreys, 1989) assume that when multiple elements are presented their representations are activated in parallel. The displays used in our experiment contained multiple similar distractors, resulting in high levels of activation of distractors relative to the singleton target. Hence, in 1 We additionally performed an analysis based on jackknifing by Miller, Patterson, and Ulrich (1998) to determine the difference between the constant and variable conditions in the N2pc onset latency. We found that, based on 50% of the peak amplitude values, the overall N2pc onset latency was shifted for both the variable and constant conditions, relative to the analysis reported in the main text. Nonetheless, the onset of the N2pc was significantly earlier for the constant (191 ms) relative to the variable condition (206 ms; overall difference: 15 ms, t(15) 5 2.3, po.05), consistent with the analysis in the text. 2 Is the effect found in the variable condition for the detection task truly an N2pc, or does it reflect a modulation of a bilateral N2 between target-present and target-absent trials (see Luck & Hillyard, 1994b, Experiment 1)? In a separate analysis (not reported here), we observed that target-absent trials elicited a more pronounced posterior N2 in both ipsilateral and contralateral sites, but, importantly, this was true for both the variable and the constant conditions. In other words, while it is possible that the N2pc interacts with the bilateral N2, it does so equally in both conditions; hence, there is no reason to suppose that the N2pc in the variable condition of the present experiment is not a genuine one. order to process the target appropriately, the visual system would have needed to direct more attentional resources to the target stimulus. This may be especially true for the discrimination task, and consistent with this idea, we found greater N2pcs for the discrimination than for the detection task. Second, our results showed for the first time that target consistency affected the latency of the N2pc, with earlier N2pc onsets for the constant relative to the variable blocks. This finding is not predicted by the suppression account of the N2pc, according to which no suppression and thus no N2pc should be observed in the variable condition. However, it is in line with our interpretation of the N2pc, which would reflect the ability of the visual system to use target consistency to more efficiently isolate the location where the relevant item is presented. Future work will have to determine whether the N2pc is mainly driven by featurebased or location-based properties of the visual display; namely, whether it reflects the allocation of attention to a specific location or to an object that contains a critical target feature. A third interesting finding is that the N2pc was not abolished when participants had to simply detect the presence of a unique feature (i.e., in the detection task). It has been suggested that this type of feature searches may be accomplished either preattentively (e.g., Treisman & Gelade, 1980) or with distributed attentional resources (e.g., Bravo & Nakayama, 1992). However, if one assumes that the N2pc is related to focused attention, then the present results challenge the idea of a net separation between pre-attentive (or distributed) and focalized attentional processes. Finally, a second contralateral posterior negativity was observed in the 350 600 ms interval for the discrimination task only. This sustained activation has been reported in experiments investigating working memory for lateralized stimuli (e.g., McCollough, Machizawa, & Vogel, 2007; Vogel & Machizawa, 2004). Recently, we observed this contralateral negativity in a visual search task with no explicit memory requirements (Mazza et al., 2007), and proposed that it reflects the active maintenance of briefly displayed visual information when deeper analyses are required. Consistent with this view, the sustained negativity was present for finer judgments of the target (i.e., in the discrimination task), but not when no further processing other than detection of a singleton feature was required. This dissociation suggests that the two ERP activations may reflect at least partially independent processing stages, one (the N2pc) related to the allocation of attentional resources to the relevant element presented at a specific location in the visual field, while the late sustained activity would reflect either the maintenance of information in working memory until a finer analysis is performed, or the in-depth process itself (also see Drew & Vogel, 2008; McCollough et al., 2007). REFERENCES Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B, 57, 289 300. Bravo, M. J., & Nakayama, K. (1992). The role of attention in different visual-search tasks. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 465 472. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193 222. Drew, T., & Vogel, E. K. (2008). Neural measures of individual differences in selecting and tracking multiple moving objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4183 4191.

N2pc and distractor suppression 775 Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433 458. Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225 234. Leblanc, E., Prime, D. J., & Jolicoeur, P. (2008). Tracking the location of visuospatial attention in a contingent capture paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 657 671. Luck, S. J. (2005). The operation of attentionfmillisecond by millisecondfover the first half second. In H. Ogmen & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.), The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., McDermott, M. T., & Ford, M. A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 64 87. Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994a). Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1000 1014. Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994b). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291 308. Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35, 99 115. Mazza, V., Turatto, M., Umiltà, C., & Eimer, M. (2007). Attentional selection and identification of visual objects are reflected by distinct electrophysiological responses. Experimental Brain Research, 181, 531 536. Mazza, V., Turatto, M., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc. Cortex, in press. McCollough, A. W., Machizawa, M. G., & Vogel, E. K. (2007). Electrophysiological measures of maintaining representations in visual working memory. Cortex, 43, 77 90. Schubo, A., Schroger, E., Meinecke, C., & Muller, H. J. (2007). Attentional resources and pop-out detection in search displays. NeuroReport, 18, 1589 1593. Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97 136. Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428, 748 751. (Received June 3, 2008; Accepted September 19, 2008)