Research design and data analysis in realism research Rana Sobh Department of Marketing, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand, and



Similar documents
Grounded Theory. 1 Introduction Applications of grounded theory Outline of the design... 2

Methodology in Social Psychology. Logics of inquiry

Research Methods: Qualitative Approach

Implementation of Realism in Case Study Research Methodology

Using qualitative research to explore women s responses

Using Case Studies in Research

The Relevance of Glaserian and Straussian Grounded Theory Approaches in Researching Human Resource Development

Interview studies. 1 Introduction Applications of interview study designs Outline of the design... 3

What is Grounded Theory? Dr Lynn Calman Research Fellow School of Nursing, Midwifery and Social Work

Scientist-practitioner?

QUALITATIVE RESEARCH. [Adapted from a presentation by Jan Anderson, University of Teesside, UK]

Revision. AS Sociology. Sociological Methods. The relationship between Positivism, Interpretivism and sociological research methods.

Exploring the directions and methods of business development. A comparative multiple-case study on Ikea and Vodafone

CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY. The purpose of this study was to describe which aspects of course design

A Framework for a Destination Reputation Management Process: A case study of three destinations

Analysing Qualitative Data

CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY Introduction. emerging markets can successfully organize activities related to event marketing.

How to Develop a Research Protocol

Philosophical underpinnings and ongoing debates of marketing and consumer research (doctoral course, 7,5 ECTS)

Overcoming the false dichotomy of quantitative and qualitative research: The case of criminal psychology

Teaching Beliefs of Graduate Students in Adult Education: A Longitudinal Perspective

Modernization of Library and Information Science Education through the Enhancement of Intercultural Communication

Case study research design

Service quality: beyond cognitive assessment Bo Edvardsson Service Research Center, Karlstad University, Karlstad, Sweden

9699 SOCIOLOGY 9699/22 Paper 22 (Data Response), maximum raw mark 50

Introduction to quantitative research

USING NVIVO FOR DATA ANALYSIS IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCH AlYahmady Hamed Hilal Saleh Said Alabri Ministry of Education, Sultanate of Oman

Successfully completing case study research: combining rigour, relevance and pragmatism

What Is a Case Study? series of related events) which the analyst believes exhibits (or exhibit) the operation of

Critical Inquiry in Educational Research and Professional Practice

SOME IMPORTANT NOTES ON QUALITATIVE RESEARCH. Prof. Dr. Andreas Budihardjo August, 2013

IHTE-1800 Research methods: Case and action research. Sari Kujala, spring 2007

Implementing Technology Education in a High School: A Case Study

Characteristics of Effective and Sustainable Teaching Development Programs for Quality Teaching in Higher Education

A Cross-Case Analysis of How Faculty Connect Learning in Hybrid Courses

Market validation in the context of new high-tech ventures

Process of Strategic Marketing Decision Making in Small Business. Graham Jocumsen and Michael Gardiner, University of Southern Queensland.

The Importance of Trust in Relationship Marketing and the Impact of Self Service Technologies

CASE STUDIES: Research Methods

THE LINK BETWEEN ORGANIZATIONAL CULTURE AND PERFORMANCE MANAGEMENT PRACTICES: A CASE OF IT COMPANIES FROM ROMANIA

Research Paradigms, the Philosophical Trinity, and Methodology

Applied Interpretation: A Review of Interpretive Description by Sally Thorne

Appraising qualitative research articles in medicine and medical education

FINANCE AND ACCOUNTING OUTSOURCING AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF SERVICE PROVIDERS AND THEIR CLIENTS IN AUSTRALIA AND NEW ZEALAND.


Qualitative Research Methods CRIM 7316 Advanced Topics in Criminal Justice Spring 2016

The University of Adelaide Business School

Interviews and Focus Groups in Advertising, Public relations and Media

How to write your research proposal

*Heinemann, London, 1979

Subject area: Ethics. Injustice causes revolt. Discuss.

Towards a new paradigm of science

Using Qualitative Information For Impact Assessment: Introducing The Qualitative Impact Assessment Protocol (QUIP)

Chapter 1 Introduction to the Study

Portfolio Assessment as an Alternate Appraisal Method: A Faculty Perspective

Harvard Business Review says our elite research is irrelevant: why soft science is the key to regaining leadership in marketing knowledge

Rudy Hirschheim. E.J. Ourso College of Business Louisiana State University.

Mixed-Methods Research Methodologies

This Module goes from 9/30 (10/2) 10/13 (10/15) MODULE 3 QUALITATIVE RESEARCH TOPIC 1. **Essential Questions**

Research Methods Carrie Williams, ( Grand Canyon University

Syllabus EDRS 812: Qualitative Methods in Educational Research George Mason University Spring Course Goals

MANAGING CUSTOMER CONTACT SERVICE THROUGH DATABASE MARKETING: DEVELOPING A THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK. Sarah Spencer-Matthews Sunshine Coast University

Running Head: HUMAN RESOURCE PRACTICES AND ENTERPRISE PERFORMANCE. Pakistan. Muzaffar Asad. Syed Hussain Haider. Muhammad Bilal Akhtar

Research design and methods Part II. Dr Brian van Wyk POST-GRADUATE ENROLMENT AND THROUGHPUT

Methods in Case Study Analysis

Criminal Justice Evaluation Framework (CJEF): Conducting effective outcome evaluations

Measurement and measures. Professor Brian Oldenburg

Qualitative Analysis of Content

DYNAMIC SUPPLY CHAIN MANAGEMENT: APPLYING THE QUALITATIVE DATA ANALYSIS SOFTWARE

Resolving problems with the Foundations of PhD Education: Theoretical Perspectives in Long Thesis PhD Submissions

School Psychology Doctoral Program Dissertation Outline 1 Final Version 6/2/2006


Unifying Epistemologies by Combining World, Description and Observer

Determinants of Customer Loyalty: An Exploratory Investigation on Relational Benefits in the Context of Customer Club

INTERNATIONAL CONSULTING FIRMS IN RUSSIA

Job Design from an Alternative Perspective

General Philosophy. Dr Peter Millican, Hertford College. Lecture 3: Induction

On the attributes of a critical literature review. Saunders, Mark N. K. 1 & Rojon, Céline 2. United Kingdom.

Entrepreneurial Marketing in Small Software Technology Firms: Use of Networks and Relationships

QUALITATIVE METHODS IN STATISTICS EDUCATION RESEARCH: METHODOLOGICAL PROBLEMS AND POSSIBLE SOLUTIONS

CALIDAD TÉCNICA Y PEDAGÓGICA EN EL SERVICIO DE E-LEARNING. Chaminda Chiran Jayasundara (1) University of Colombo, Sri Lanka.

EXPLORING COLLABORATION AMONG HISTORICALLY BLACK UNIVERSITIES & DOCTORAL/RESEARCH UNIVERSITIES IN THE USA

Inequality, Mobility and Income Distribution Comparisons

Understanding challenges of information security culture: a methodological issue

ASSOCIATED STUDY MATERIALS

Assessing Online Asynchronous Discussion in Online Courses: An Empirical Study

Theory and Research. Theory functions three ways in research: ways that can suggest other possibilities.

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEGREE. Educational Leadership Doctor of Philosophy Degree Major Course Requirements. EDU721 (3.

Research Courses in the CPED Education Doctorate: What s the Difference? Purpose

MBA Dissertation Guidelines

Doctor of Education - Higher Education

Case study as a research strategy: Investigating extreme weather resilience of construction SMEs in the UK

Inductive theory generation: A grounded approach to business inquiry

Becoming a Librarian

The Library

Chapter 1: Health & Safety Management Systems (SMS) Leadership and Organisational Safety Culture

A paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Australian Association for Research in Education, Newcastle, N.S.W.

Customer Relationship Management based on Increasing Customer Satisfaction

Hasan Basri. Syiah Kuala University, Banda Aceh, Indonesia

Transcription:

The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0309-0566.htm EJM 40,11/12 1194 Received February 2005 Revised August 2005 and September 2005 Research design and data analysis in realism research Rana Sobh Department of Marketing, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand, and Chad Perry Department of Marketing and Management, Southern Cross University, Gold Coast, Australia European Journal of Marketing Vol. 40 No. 11/12, 2006 pp. 1194-1209 q Emerald Group Publishing Limited 0309-0566 DOI 10.1108/03090560610702777 Abstract Purpose The aim of this paper is to apply principles of the realism paradigm within qualitative research projects. Design/methodology/approach The paper starts by establishing the usefulness of realism research for investigating marketing management phenomena, and then considers the implications of the realism paradigm for research design. Findings Issues such as the level of prior theory required, the use of replication logic and triangulation are discussed. In addition, guidelines for realism data analysis and reporting are developed. Originality/value This paper provides an explicit set of principles for realism research design and data analysis that is different from those in other types of research. Keywords Research, Design, Data analysis Paper type Conceptual paper Introduction Many researchers are concerned with the choice between a quantitative and a qualitative methodology. Essentially, quantitative researchers use numbers and large samples to test theories, and qualitative researchers use words and meanings in smaller samples to build theories (for example, Easterby-Smith et al., 1991). Some researchers use only one type of methodology while others suggest that both types may sometimes be appropriate (Borch and Arthur, 1995; Hyde, 2000), even though the two methods may produce contradictory findings about the same phenomena, or at least unrelated ones it is likely that quantitative methods and qualitative methods will eventually answer questions that do not easily come together to provide a single, well-integrated picture of the situation (Patton, 1990, pp. 464-5). However, a core issue for researchers is not related to choice of methodologies but related to acknowledgement of the research paradigms. A methodology is only one of the three elements of a paradigm that researchers either explicitly or implicitly work within a paradigm includes the other elements of ontology and epistemology (Guba and Lincoln, 1994). Essentially, ontology is reality, epistemology is the relationship between that reality and the researcher, and methodology is the techniques used by the researcher to discover that reality. In brief, a paradigm is an overall conceptual framework within which a researcher may work, that is, a paradigm can be regarded as the basic belief system or worldview that guides the investigator (Guba and Lincoln, 1994, p. 105). Philosophical assumptions that support four different paradigms

of science positivism, realism, constructivism and critical theory are summarised in Table I. Underlying these four paradigms is the question of knowledge creation: how can the findings of one research project be generalised to other situations? In the first of the four paradigms, positivism, knowledge is statistically generalised to a population by statistical analysis of observations about an easily accessible reality. In the second paradigm of realism, the findings of one study are extended by analytical generalisation that shows how the empirical findings of a research project nestle within theories (ontology cells, Table I). In other words, the aim of realism paradigm is to generalise to theoretical propositions and not to populations (Yin, 1989, p. 21). In the other two paradigms of constructivism and critical theory, reality is perception and so generalisation of one research finding about someone s perceptions to another person s theory about reality, cannot be done. For example, in constructivism, findings are related to individual views of the world and create a world of multiple constructed realities. Such views cannot be usefully compared with those of other Research design and data analysis 1195 Paradigm Element Positivism Constructivism Critical theory Realism Ontology Reality is real and apprehensible Epistemology Findings true researcher is objective by viewing reality through a one-way mirror Common methodologies Mostly concerns with a testing of theory. Thus mainly quantitative methods such as: survey, experiments, and verification of hypotheses Multiple local and specific constructed realities Created findings researcher is a passionate participant within the world being investigated In-depth unstructured interviews, participant observation, action research, and grounded theory research Virtual reality shaped by social, economic, ethnic, political, cultural, and gender values, crystallised over time Value mediated findings researcher is a transformative intellectual who changes the social world within which participants live Action research and participant observation Reality is real but only imperfectly and probabilistically apprehensible and so triangulation from many sources is required to try to know it Findings probably true researcher is value-aware and needs to triangulate any perceptions he or she is collecting Mainly qualitative methods such as case studies and convergent interviews Note: Essentially, ontology is reality, epistemology is the relationship between that reality and the researcher and methodology is the technique used by the researcher to discover that reality Source: Based on Perry et al. (1999), which itself was based on Guba and Lincoln (1994) from which the quotations come Table I. Four scientific paradigms

EJM 40,11/12 1196 individuals (as in post-modern research) (Bazeley, 2004). Similarly, in the critical theory paradigm, perceptions are judged by their appropriateness to subjective conventions such as beauty and justice (as in feminist research) (Perry et al., 1999). Of the four paradigms, this paper will focus on marketing knowledge creation within the realism paradigm. In contrast to this paradigm, the positivism paradigm position on knowledge creation about an easily apprehensible reality through value-free procedures is well known, although it may often be inappropriate for reasons developed in the next section. In addition, constructivism and critical theory research will not be covered in detail here because they are explicit or implicit in much qualitative research literature. Their assumption that reality is a subjective construction is sometimes inappropriate for marketing management for reasons discussed in the next section. Thus the aim of this paper is to explain how realism research can be designed to build knowledge about the external reality of marketing. Essentially, we argue that working within the realism paradigm requires procedures that are different from procedures in positivism, constructivism and critical theory paradigm research. In particular, procedures relating research design and about data analysis and reporting require explicit acknowledgment, although none appear to exist. Our contribution is therefore explicit sets of principles for realism research design and data analysis that are different from other types of research. In addressing this aim, this article concentrates on research design and data analysis. Other issues in realism research, for example, arguments about philosophical details (Hunt, 1994; Zinkhan and Hirscheim, 1992) or how to judge the quality of realism research (Healy and Perry, 2000; Thompson and Perry, 2004) have been dealt with elsewhere. The article has four sections. We begin with a brief introduction to realism s core ideas and compare them to those of the other three paradigms. Then research design building on prior theory and using replication logic in realism research is described. Next, appropriate analysis and reportage techniques in realism research are discussed. Positivism paradigm Before examining realism research design, its particular appropriateness for much marketing research needs to be established vis-à-vis other research done within other paradigms. In essence, a paradigm reflects a researcher s understanding of the nature of existence that is beyond logical debate because each paradigm is rational within its own constructed logic (Lincoln and Guba, 1985). That is, there is no objective ground for choosing a paradigm. All that one can do is work within a paradigm that is consistent with a researcher s own presumptions, presumptions that cannot be tested on any empirical or logical grounds. Thus assumptions behind some paradigms need to be discussed first, to try to determine how closely or not they fit the perceived values and needs of the stakeholders of research projects. The first, positivism paradigm is the most widely used paradigm for business school research (Orlikowski and Baroudi, 1991) and assumes implicitly or explicitly that reality can be measured by viewing it through a one way, value-free mirror (this discussion of paradigms is based on Perry (2002). Engineers, for example, use these assumptions to research a physical science phenomenon like a bridge. This received

view (Guba and Lincoln, 1994, p. 106) of science has dominated the formal discourse in the physical and social science for some 400 years. However, the positivism paradigm has been criticised for its exclusion of the discovery dimensions in inquiry and the under-determination of theory (Deshpande, 1983; Guba and Lincoln, 1994). For example, the assumptions of positivism noted above are appropriate in a physical science such as zoology, but may be inappropriate when approaching a complex social science phenomenon, which involves reflective humans, such as marketing. Indeed, some research has shown that these almost default assumptions of business school research are sometimes inappropriate for research about social science phenomena like marketing and management. Consider some of this research in turn. First, replication of positivism research does not usually produce the same results as the prior research, as one would expect from the value-free methods of positivism research about an easily apprehensible reality. Hubbard and Armstrong (1994) found that only 15 percent of replication studies in the social science of marketing fully confirmed the prior findings and only 25 percent partially confirmed them. Indeed, fully 60 percent of replication studies provided results that conflicted with their predecessor. Of those few studies that support prior research, over half were done by the same researcher as did the prior research (Hubbard and Vetter, 1996), confirming that the research did not appear to be done in the value-free way that positivism supposes. Second, meta-analyses of several positivism studies often confirm this picture of the inadequacy of positivism for investigations of social phenomena. A meta-analysis compares several quantitative studies on a common scale. For example, the correlations over many studies between customer satisfaction and these constructs cover a wide range of values that even spread across positive and negative values (Szymanski and Henard, 2001):. expectations 20.13 to þ 0.66; Research design and data analysis 1197. disconfirmation 20.24 to þ 0.87; and. performance 20.37 to þ 0.81. Other research has confirmed this picture of positivism s somewhat incomplete handling of social science phenomena, despite its predominance in business school research. For example, a study of thorough investigation of senior managers on business school advisory boards, new MBA recruits into firms, their superiors and consultants found that many of these business people considered much academic research coming out of business schools was irrelevant. In the researchers own words, they found that (Porter and McKibbin, 1988, p. 180, emphases added): As far as we could tell, many key managers and executives pay little or no attention to such research and findings [of business academics]...the direct impact appears nil...not a single [manager] who was interviewed cited the research of business schools as either their most important strength or their major weakness... The business world is... ignoring the research coming out of business schools. That is, there seems to be a gulf between academics who usually work from the positivism paradigm and practitioners. Confirming this gulf was a thorough textual analysis of all theory and practice articles on organisational culture in industry (read

EJM 40,11/12 1198 by practitioners) and academic journals from 1975 to 1985 that found practitioners were not influenced by academics (Barley et al., 1988). In a more recent investigation of this gulf between practitioners and marketing academics in particular, McKenzie et al. (2002) found very low readership of academic marketing journals by practitioners. Finally, after reviewing recent research about academic research in the UK, France and Germany, Tapp (2004, p. 493) concludes that academic research agendas are seen as significantly removed from practitioners and their problems. In brief, we argue that using positivism as the default paradigm in social sciences like marketing has not been fruitful. This paper will investigate another paradigm that may make academic marketing research more so. Constructivism and critical theory paradigms The major alternative to positivism in the social sciences is constructivism and critical theory. Essentially, these paradigms argue that the world is constructed by people and that these constructions should be the driving forces investigated in social science research. For example, some people perceive Levi jeans are the best jeans and worth buying, even though tests done within a positivism framework by consumer magazines show their cloth, stitching and buttoning are not as well constructed as other brands jeans. Similarly, a person can commit suicide because of the black world they perceive themselves to be in, whether that eternal world is really as unwelcoming as they perceive it or not. That is, a core element of these two paradigms is that each person s constructed reality is so powerful an influence on their behaviour that any external reality is relatively unimportant and, moreover, there is no way of comparing the multiple constructed realities of different people. However, this incommensurability of perceptions, that is, the relativism at the heart of these paradigms, makes the two paradigms a cul de sac for many academic researchers. For example, Hunt (1994) asks how these paradigms can help in trying to research whether the Holocaust occurred to Jews in the 1940s some people perceive that it occurred and some that it did not, and these differences cannot be compared within the two paradigms of constructivism and critical theory: It is indeed true that one of the multiple realities that some people hold... is that the Holocaust never occurred...an alternative multiple reality is that the Holocaust did in fact occur...which multiple realities is correct? Sincere...advocates of reality relativism must stand mute when confronted with this question. One way offered out of this cul de sac is negotiations between people with different constructed worlds, to arrive at some shared understanding. But will these negotiations be possible when they have unequal power? For example, will burglars who perceive that taking possessions of wealthier people is equitable, and wealthy householders who do not want their homes burgled, be able to negotiate some shared understanding? Pawson and Tilley (1997, pp. 16-21) answer that question with a clear No : It is hard not to snigger when [constructivists like] Guba and Lincoln talk about getting stakeholders to agree to and formally sign conditions for a productive hermeneutic dialectic... [Their] hermeneutic dialectic circles (not surprisingly) go round in circles, rather than constituting a linear advance on truth.

In particular, constructivism and critical theory are not especially relevant in research about an organisation having to survive within a market, because marketing managers have to deal with a world that is external, that is out there and that does not particularly care about the perceptions of an individual manager: In marketing, the company s external environment is always more important than the internal. The real decisions are made in the world outside among consumers, middlemen, competitors, politicians, legislators and trade organisations. The external environment is neither particularly knowledgeable nor interested in the company and its development (Gummesson, 2000, p. 105, emphasis added). In brief, subjective meaning within an individual s constructed realities may indeed determine some outcomes like individual customers choice of jeans but not issues concerning the marketing management of those perceptions. Overall, constructivism/critical theory may be useful for research about important social science phenomena like suicide, falling in love, family life, office power politics in big organisations with slow or blurry feedback loops to the outside, political ideologies, racism and nationalism. In this constructivism and critical theory research, findings are related to individual views of the world and create a world of multiple constructed realities. Such views of individuals cannot be usefully compared with those of other individuals (Bazeley, 2004), as noted above. However, much marketing management is about managing many transactions in an external market place and necessarily involves comparing and managing many constructed realities. In this sort of world, research has to be like a court room trial where evidence is sought about the external reality of guilty or not guilty that exists independently of what did or did not drive a person to commit the crime, even though that external reality can only be known imperfectly. We need a picture (even though it will be an imperfect one) of how managers can manage the perceptions of many customers within an external market, at a profit. In short, constructivism and critical theory are cul de sacs for much marketing management research. Research design and data analysis 1199 Realism paradigm principles In turn, the fourth paradigm is realism. Realism is an increasingly useful worldview for some social scientists. Indeed, it is a growing movement transforming the intellectual scene in management research. Its philosophical position is that reality exists independently of the researcher s mind, that is, there is an external reality (Bhaskar, 1978; Harre and Madden, 1975). This external reality consists of abstract things that are born of people s minds but exist independently of any one person, it is largely autonomous, though created by us (Magee, 1985, p. 61). A person s perceptions are a window on to that blurry, external reality. Realism refers to this external reality as consisting of structures that are themselves sets of interrelated objects, and of mechanisms through which those objects interact. For example, a realism researcher considers a buyer and a seller are objects that interact, and their interaction is not merely the creation of someone s imagination, as noted in the quotation from Gummesson (2002, p. 105) above that the company s external environment is always more important then the internal. The real decisions are made in the world outside.... That is, marketers cannot do whatever they feel like doing in a post-modern fashion (Brown, 1998) because they must aim to meet the needs of an external market place at profit. Thus realism research is searching towards an understanding of the common

EJM 40,11/12 1200 reality of an economic system in which many people operate inter-dependently. That is, realists believe that there is a real world out there to discover. However, that real external world is only imperfectly and probabilistically apprehensible (Guba and Lincoln, 1994; Tsoukas, 1989; Merriam, 1988). Realists acknowledge differences between the real world and their particular view of it and try to construct various views of this reality in terms of which ones are relative in time and place (Riege, 2003). As such, the underlying structures investigated in a social science are only contingently linked to the experiences that a researcher has in the field. In other words, the combined effects of underlying structures and mechanisms result in patterns in experiences, but those patterns will not always occur. Thus social phenomena by their nature are fragile, so that causal impacts are not fixed but are contingent on their environment. Accordingly, the contexts of observed phenomena are very important. Consequently, the desire of realism research is to develop a family of answers that covers several contingent contexts and different reflective participants (Pawson and Tilley, 1997, p. 152). That is, the results sought are not based on an experiment that shows that A directly causes B, like a match directly causes gunpowder to explode in a science like physics. In social science phenomena, there are few direct A to B causality links because any links are strongly influenced by the context. For example, whether the installation of extra lights in a car park influences the crime rate in the car park depends on the four Ps of context place (where), period (time), people and process (after Pawson and Tilley, 1997; Carson et al., 2001). Consider more closely the four Ps of context in our lights and car park example. Whether lights affect the crime rate depends on the place of the car park where it is. If it is in an affluent suburb, what little crime does occur may be reduced; but in low income, high-crime area with inadequate policing, the crime rate might be unaffected. Similarly, if most car parking occurs during the period of daytime, lights may not affect the crime rate. Furthermore, if the people who might steal cars are hardened criminals who have successfully stolen cars before, the existence of lights will not deter them; but if the people are inexperienced, the lights might deter them. In brief, treating people as independent, standardised objects is inappropriate because doing so ignores their ability to reflect on problem situations, and act upon this (Robson, 1993, p. 60). That is, the issues are longitudinal over time, as well as being investigated at a particular point in time. Finally, consider the core process of lights shining in cars. If the cars in the car park are expensive products like Mercedes cars, lights may not deter would-be criminals; however, if the cars are cheap near-bombs, the lights may make the small payoff from a theft not worth the risk of being caught. In brief, this car park and lights example illustrates the complex cause-and-effect links within the context of a complex, science situation that makes the realism paradigm appropriate for this type of research problem. It is these complex cause-and-effect links grounded in the context of a situation that are referred to as causal tendencies or powers (Bhaskar, 1978, p. 20) that are influenced in different ways by hard-to-control contexts and other factors. This imperfection of the realism view of an external reality implies that a search for just one negative result to disprove a theory may not be as appropriate in realism research in the social sciences as it is in the physical sciences (or as straightforward as Yin (1994, p. 31) suggests). This kind of search for a negative result was advocated by Popper (1959, cited in Easterby-Smith et al., 1991) who said a theory about the colour of swans should not be stated in the positive form that all swans are white but in the

negative form that no swan is black. Then if a search for black swans results in one sighting, the negative form of the theory will have been falsified, and the theory will have to be recast to become possibly all swans are black or white. However, in realism research, one negative result is not proof that the underlying mechanisms have been misunderstood, because the negative result may be a result of the context s intervention. Instead of looking for single instances, realism research should be consistently asking why a result has been found, because the observed findings are merely outcroppings of a deeper, unobserved and unobservable reality (Neuman, 1994, p. 423) or the tip of an iceberg (Gummesson, 2000). Moreover, quantitative results from a positivist method like a survey are unlikely to provide understanding of this deeper reality and therefore should not be a major part of any realism research project. Easton (1999, pp. 79, 81) sums up the overall implications of the realism position for careful probing of underlying explanations in this way:... the researcher [has] to identify the contingent causal powers that are operating in the particular situations under research and the ways they combine and interact in order to create the particular events observed...to seek for the underlying reality through the dark veil that hides it. Research design and data analysis 1201 Research design in realism research In the previous section we examined the realism paradigm core ideas and argued for its appropriateness to investigate marketing phenomena. However, in order to build knowledge about the external reality of marketing within this paradigm, explicit principles for analysing and reporting data within this paradigm realism research need to be developed. The aim of this section is to do so. In addition, issues such as the level of prior theory required, the use of replication logic and triangulation are discussed. Prior theory Consider how principles of the realism approach have implications for the design of a realism research project that are different from those of constructivism and critical theory research. First, realism researchers enter the field with prior theories. In contrast, grounded theory researchers, for example, gradually construct a theory from interacting with their own accumulating data, without any inputs from other people s theories in the literature. For example, a researcher investigating beer drinking was not allowed to read any other research about beer consumption until after all her data had been collected (Carson et al., 2001). For realism researchers, there is an external reality and other people have usually researched or experienced aspects of that reality before, and so their perceptions are some of the many windows on to that reality deserving some consideration before realism data collection starts (Perry et al., 1999, p. 18). In other words, realism researchers can support Miles and Huberman s (1994, p. 17) advice that a preliminary conceptual framework about the underlying structures and mechanisms should be developed from the literature and/or from people with experience of the phenomenon, before entering the field to collect data at the outset...[develop a] rudimentary conceptual framework... However, Miles and Huberman base their position on the practicalities of efficient research and aim their advice at beginning researchers, while realism researchers base their position on their ontology.

EJM 40,11/12 1202 A practical effect of this realism approach to research design is that realism research will often consist of two stages. The first stage will be relatively exploratory, while the literature is step-by-step enfolded around sequential interview data, as in the analytic induction method or convergent interview methodology, for example (Carson et al., 2001; Hyde, 2000). In this realism research, the researcher should start reading the literature about the phenomenon before the first interview and continue reading it while the interviews are proceeding, enfolding the literature around the findings as they emerge from the interviews. Having prior knowledge also helps in the selection of interviewees, and allows the opening and probe questions in the first and later interviews to be more effective and efficient. It also helps the researcher to make more believable, small encouraging noises during the conversation of the interview, as well as helping the researcher to recognise when something important has been said (although the interviewee should not be biased towards comments that please the interviewer but do not reflect their own perceptions of reality). In these ways, rapport and interview dynamics are assisted through a sharing of concerns. In brief, prior theory is precious and is gradually built on in stage one. How that prior theory and its initial framework are incorporated in to stage two is dealt with below. Consider an example of convergent interviewing in stage one of a realism research project. Woodward (1997) was investigating how brand equity was measured in services. Although the literature about measuring brand equity of goods was extensive, little was known about measuring the brand equity of services. In the first stage of her research project, she had to find out what was done in the real world. So she arranged to interview three consultants in her region and two by telephone in the country s capital city, to find the major issues involved in the measurement of the brand equity of services. Her first interview s questions were based on her limited, initial reading and asked questions like Is there a difference between the marketing of goods and services?, Can brand knowledge of a service be made up of brand awareness and image?, and What would be an interesting service brand for me to study? As the interviews proceeded, she added questions based on the previous interviews and on her increasing knowledge of the branding literature. After five interviews, she understood the critical issues involved in her study and had gathered examples of service brands she was confident she was on track for the rest of her research project. In brief, prior theory in realism research can be viewed as additional evidence, that is, perceptions, which can be used to clarify the imperfectly apprehensible external reality by triangulating on that reality (Riege, 2003; Perry, 1998). Triangulation Triangulation (Denzin, 1978) has been used as a synonym for mixed methods (Bazeley, 2004). However, the idea of triangulation assumes a single reality and thus is only appropriate within the realism paradigm and is not appropriate within constructivism or critical theory research those two paradigms assume that there is not a single objective reality but rather multiple subjectively constructed realities that are incommensurable. Although Denzin (1978) initially argued for triangulation in all qualitative research, he backed away from that position for constructivism and critical theory research (Denzin, 1989) (except to overcome personal biases of the researcher).

In other words, why use different methods to try to validate a constructivism or critical theory reality that cannot be related to the many other such realities? However, in realism research, triangulation provides a family of answers (Pawson and Tilley, 1997) that covers its reality s several contingent contexts, to capture a single, external, and complex reality. For example, different interviewees are asked the same question to test whether they answer with the same perceptions. Sometimes, these different triangulation sources will provide different perceptions, but those different perceptions should not be considered to be confusing glimpses of the same reality, rather they should be considered to foster understanding of the reasons for the complexities of that reality. This is another reason why mere numbers should not be used in realism data analysis and why the data analysis should concentrate on reasons why. Research design and data analysis 1203 Replication Another implication of the realism paradigm for research design concerns selection of cases, usually called replication. (Here, the term case refers to the unit of analysis, that is, an example of the realism structure and mechanism that is being investigated, and so is not limited to cases study research.) Replication in positivism research refers to repeating a study in various ways, expecting the results to be the same (Hubbard and Armstrong, 1994). In contrast, in realism research, replication refers to choice of cases where the results are expected to be the same or different. In other words, careful choice of cases should be made such that they either produce (Yin, 1994):. similar results for predictable reasons, that is, literal replication; or. contrary results for predictable reasons, that is, theoretical replication. For example, a researcher might predict that brokers in a distribution chain would say roughly the same thing in interviews (literal replication), and that end-consumers would say different things from the brokers (theoretical replication). Two issues about theoretical and literal replication in realism research require further discussion. First, as noted, contingencies in the context of phenomena are critical in realism research. Thus the two types of replication should be based on a priori contextual issues, such as level in the distribution chain above. Studying the effect of the contexts will help to provide better understanding of the underlying structure of objects and mechanisms, which are the core concerns of a realism researcher. This issue of contextual replication implies that realism research usually involves multiple cases. These several cases should be regarded as multiple experiments and not multiple respondents in a survey, and so replication logic and not sampling logic should be used for multiple-case studies (Yin, 1994, pp. 45-50). Thus relevance rather than representativeness is the criterion for case selection (Stake, 1994). Thus there will be some within-case analysis of each case, as the first step in data analysis (as recommended by Patton, 1990, p. 388). However, these single case analyses are not as useful or important for understanding underlying structures and mechanisms, as the later cross-case analyses are (Yin, 1989, p. 106). Of course a single case should provide more in-depth data than multiple cases can, within the time and resource constraints of a research project. However, relying on just one case to provide the contextual conditions investigated in a realism research project requires an

EJM 40,11/12 1204 extraordinarily rich case. Perhaps a longitudinal study of just one firm over several years of business cycles and organisational structures might provide equivalent data to multiple cases over one or two years. That is, a single case realism research project should be considered only when one or more of the three special, single-case conditions in Yin (1994) are present. Moreover, a single case project should include two or more of the conceptual frameworks noted above, to be triggers for eliciting understanding about underlying structures and mechanisms. A second issue about replication in realism research is that different contexts may or may not produce different findings. One reason for this situation is that research findings may be affected by the way that interviews were conducted in different cases, rather than by the contexts around the phenomena. For example, different levels of rapport with different interviewees may provide findings that similar levels of rapport would not have. To guard against this possibility, the second stage of a realism project can aim at confirming or verifying the conceptual framework developed in the first stage by using the same interview protocol across all cases. That is, the first stage has a different aim from the second stage. Stage one emphasises explanation as it builds a conceptual framework; stage two emphasises prediction of reasons for phenomena while it confirms or disconfirms that conceptual framework. In brief, replication is a tool used in realism research design to begin the process of knowledge creation, but that process of knowledge creation depends on going deeper than contextual issues in the analysis of data about outcroppings of observations, further down to the underlying structures and mechanisms. Thus we now consider data analysis in realism research. Data analysis in realism research Because realism research is about underlying structures and mechanisms, its data are almost always qualitative data about meanings. How should transcripts of interviews or reports of observations be analysed? Guidelines for data reduction and display can be developed that fit the principles of realism. Data reduction The first guideline for realism research refers to how codes are generated for use in the first stage of reducing the data to a manageable form. To begin, realism research is not interested in virtually every detail of all the perceptions of respondents, like constructivism and critical theory research is. There are usually three coding passes in the data reduction phase of constructivist or critical theory data analysis. The first pass is usually an open pass where codes are generated from the perception data themselves (Neuman, 1994). All the perceptions in the transcripts are the reality being investigated in constructivism research. In contrast, in realism research, only those perceptions relevant to the external reality are worth investigating. Thus the codes used to reduce data in realism research are usually generated from the conceptual framework rather than from the data. In other words, realism researchers can leapfrog the first pass used in constructivism research and move directly to the second, axial coding pass where codes from the conceptual framework are allocated to chunks of text. Then they proceed to the third, selective coding pass of comparisons and interpretations.

There are two problems with this stage of data reduction in realism research. First, this realism procedure may be that some patterns, which are relevant to the underlying structures and mechanisms were not included in the conceptual framework, and so may be missed for the second axial coding pass. However, stage one of the research project should have picked up most relevant structures and mechanisms and included them in the conceptual framework. Moreover, the interviews of stage two will have begun with open questions that should uncover any structures and mechanisms missing from the initial conceptual framework. That is, in the interviews, issues in the conceptual framework will only be discussed in answers to closed, probe questions if they were not raised in the answer to the open questions. Finally, the last question in any interview should ask if there is anything relevant that has not been asked in the interview. If any new issues are raised in the interviews, they can be added to the data analysis codes based on conceptual framework. The risk of missing some issues about the underlying structures and mechanisms during coding is thus addressed, as far as possible. A second problem with this stage of data reduction is that the researcher s own biases may corrupt the process. This corruption will be minimised through his or her interactions with other researchers literature and with stage 1 interviewees while developing the initial conceptual framework. Nevertheless, this concern does raise the issue of how much a researcher s own values can influence the picture of an external reality that is being developed in realism research. Perhaps the best that one can aim for value-awareness rather than value-removal. Thus the researcher s own background and values should be stated explicitly in terms such as in the following example (Thompson, 2003): I am a 42-year-old male European Australian with a wife and two children. I have lived in countries outside Australia for about 2 years. I have travelled extensively in Australia and overseas including having visited 20 countries, six of which were developing countries. I am trained in western management at master s degree level and have worked as a manager of Australian enterprises for around five years. I have also studied cross-cultural management, and worked in cross-cultural situations including in emergency aid and development aid situations. I have studied participatory theory at master s degree level. My understanding of technical issues is limited to that of an average Australian who has no formal training in technical areas...i have values that reflect my background above, but aimed to be aware of those values within my thesis research, and aimed to minimise the impact of those values on findings (Carson et al., 2001). Research design and data analysis 1205 Data display After data reduction, the next step is data display. The conceptual framework and the data determine how the displays are constructed. However, four realism guidelines can be suggested. First, the displays can show numerical frequencies of empirical experiences, but the interpretation of the displays in the text should eschew numbers and concentrate on interpretations (although the numbers in the displays should be referred to for the reader to be able to follow the line of reasoning). For example, text could look like this: Most respondents thought shipping was not important because schedules were reliable (row 2 of table 4.3). Thank goodness the unions are tame (A2). No worries we have good port agents and shipping lines (B1).

EJM 40,11/12 1206 After all, the cases were not a random sample and precise frequencies formed from them are not the end point of the analysis. Second, every observation should have an explanation of why the observation occurred, as the example above showed. In realism research, these explanations should focus on contingencies, structures and mechanisms. Next, representative quotations in support of explanations should occur frequently in the text with links to the respondent who said it, to provide the in-depth understanding that realism researcher seek. Finally, data analysis computer software like NUD * IST that are often used in constructivism and critical theory research can track and match every phrase of every perception in a transcript, but that approach is not essential in realism research. Constructivism and critical theory research need to thoroughly map a subjectively constructed world, but realism research is primarily concerned with only those perceptions about an external, objective reality. This software may help in indexing those limited number of perceptions that are relevant to the purpose of a realism research project but our experience is that the costs in time and effort of setting up and running the software outweigh the indexing benefits. Realism research emphasises relationships, connections, and creativity, and computer software may lead to a decrease in sensitivity about these (Carson and Coviello, 1996). That is, computer programs are suitable for subjective research that is not aware of anything other than the text properties of the data and cannot interpret, make deductions or generalisations from the data (Dembrowski and Hammer-Lloyd, 1995, p. 53), but realism research is different. Conclusions about realism research and implications for further research In summary, the realism paradigm appears to be appropriate for researching some marketing phenomena. Indeed, it appears to be more appropriate than the default positivism paradigm and more suited for research about the external reality of a market place than the very subjective constructivism and critical theory paradigms. The realism paradigm considers the external reality of the market place but acknowledges that the complexities of a market place need to be investigated with in-depth qualitative research methods instead of somewhat inappropriate positivism methods. Use of the realism paradigm favours a two-stage approach to a research project, one stage that builds one or two conceptual frameworks, and one or more stages that confirm or disconfirm the framework(s). Replication logic should be used to explore the effects of context upon the underlying structures and mechanisms that are the core of the frameworks. Above all, realism data analysis should concentrate on interpretations of the data to focus on underlying structures and mechanisms. Finally, what are the implications of the discussion above for the research that follows realism research? Realists would correctly agree that other contexts of the studied structures and mechanisms need to be investigated, and other structures and mechanisms may be worth investigating. Nevertheless, some realists would argue that quantitative theory testing is not required after a realism research project, for two reasons. First, the positivism paradigm underlying most quantitative theory testing will investigate different and more shallow phenomena than the deep structures and mechanisms investigated in realism research. That is, quantitative methods provide answers to different questions than those of qualitative methods. Next, quantitative theory testing cannot deliver the statistical generalisation to a population of marketing

phenomena that it purports to deliver, as noted above. In brief, a realism project does not necessarily lead to a later, theory-testing project. Nevertheless, realism research could be used to generate empirically based conceptual frameworks that could be operationalized for later testing. However, the later research s overall question will have to be changed from a how or why question into a what question (Yin, 1994). References Barley, J.N., Meyer, G.W. and Gash, D.C. (1988), Cultures of culture: academics practitioners and the pragmatics of normative control, Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 33 No. 1, pp. 24-60. Bazeley, P. (2004), Issues in mixing qualitative and quantitative approaches to research, in Buber, R. and Gadner, J. (Eds), Applying Qualitative Methods to Marketing Management Research, Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke, pp. 141-56. Bhaskar, R. (1978), Realist Theory of Science, Wheatsheaf, Harvester, Coleshill. Borch, O.J. and Arthur, M.B. (1995), Strategic networks among small firms: implications for strategy research methodology, Journal of Management Studies, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 419-41. Brown, S. (1998), Post-modern Marketing Two, Thompson, London. Carson, D. and Coviello, N. (1996), Qualitative research issues at the marketing or entrepreneurship interface, Marketing Intelligence & Planning, Vol. 14 No. 6, pp. 51-8. Carson, D., Gilmore, A., Gronhaug, K. and Perry, C. (2001), Qualitative Research in Marketing, Sage, London. Dembrowski, S. and Hammer-Lloyd, S. (1995), Computer applications a new road to qualitative data analysis, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 9 No. 11, pp. 50-62. Denzin, N.K. (1978), The Research Act, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY. Denzin, N.K. (1989), The Act Research, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Deshpande, R. (1983), Paradigms lost : on theory and method in research in marketing, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 47 No. 4, pp. 101-10. Easterby-Smith, M., Thorpe, R. and Lowe, A. (1991), Management Research: An Introduction, Sage, London. Easton, G. (1999), Case research as a methodology for industrial networks: a realist apologia, in Naude, P. and Turnbull, P. (Eds), Network Dynamics in International Marketing, Pergamon, Oxford. Guba, E.G. and Lincoln, Y.S. (1994), Competing paradigms in qualitative research, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (Eds), Handbook of Qualitative Research, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 105-17. Gummesson, E. (2000), Qualitative Methods in Management Research, Sage, London. Gummesson, E. (2002), Total Relationship in Marketing Strategy: Moving Strategy from the 4Ps Product, Price, Promotion, Place of Traditional Marketing Management to the 30Rs The Thirty Relationships of a New Marketing Paradigm, Butterworth-Heinemann, Oxford. Harre, R. and Madden, E.H. (1975), Causal Powers, Basil Blackwell, Oxford. Healy, M. and Perry, C. (2000), Comprehensive criteria to judge validity and reliability of qualitative research within the realism paradigm, Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 118-26. Hubbard, R. and Armstrong, J.S. (1994), Replications and extensions in marketing: rarely published but quite contrary, International Journal of Research in Marketing, Vol. 11, June, pp. 233-48. Research design and data analysis 1207

EJM 40,11/12 1208 Hubbard, R. and Vetter, D.E. (1996), An empirical comparison of published replications research in economics, finance, management and marketing, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 35 No. 2, pp. 153-64. Hunt, S.D. (1994), On rethinking marketing: our discipline, our practice, our methods, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 13-25. Hyde, K.F. (2000), Recognising deductive processes in qualitative research, Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 82-9. Lincoln, Y.S. and Guba, E.G. (1985), Naturalistic Inquiry, Sage Publications, London and New Delhi. McKenzie, J., Wright, S., Ball, D.F. and Baron, P.J. (2002), The publications of marketing faculty who are we really talking to?, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 36 Nos 11/12, pp. 1196-208. Magee, B. (1985), Popper, 3rd ed., Fontana, London. Merriam, S.B. (1988), Case Study Research in Education: A Qualitative Approach, Jossey-Bass Publications, San Francisco, CA. Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994), Qualitative Data Analysis A Sourcebook of New Methods, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA. Neuman, W.L. (1994), Social Research Methods: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches, Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA. Orlikowski, W.J. and Baroudi, J.J. (1991), Studying information technology in organization: research approaches and assumptions, Information Systems Research, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 1-14. Patton, M. (1990), Qualitative Research Methods, Sage, London. Pawson, R. and Tilley, N. (1997), Realistic Evaluation, Sage, London. Perry, C. (1998), Processes of a case study methodology for postgraduate research in marketing, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 32 Nos 9/10, pp. 785-802. Perry, C. (2002), Case research in marketing, The Marketing Review, No. 1. Perry, C., Riege, A. and Brown, L. (1999), Realism s role among scientific paradigms in marketing research, Irish Marketing Review, Vol. 12 No. 2, pp. 16-23. Porter, W.M. and McKibbin, L.E. (1988), Management Education and Development: Drift or Thrust into the 21st Century?, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY. Riege, A.M. (2003), Validity and reliability tests in case study research: a literature review with hands-on applications for each research phase, Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, Vol. 6 No. 2, pp. 75-86. Robson, C. (1993), Real World Research: A Resource for Social Scientists and Practitioners-Researchers, Blackwell Publishers, Oxford. Stake, R.E. (1994), Case studies, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (Eds), Handbook of Qualitative Research, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Szymanski, D.M. and Henard, D.H. (2001), Customer satisfaction: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence, Journal of the Academy of Marketing, Vol. 29, Winter, p. 16. Tapp, A. (2004), The changing face of marketing academia, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 38 Nos 5/6, pp. 492-9. Thompson, F. and Perry, C. (2004), Generalizing results of an action research in one work place to other situations and practice, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 38 Nos 3/4, pp. 401-18.

Thompson, P. (2003), An action research analysis of factors involved in Australian small- to medium-sized enterprises planning and tendering for overseas development aid projects, PhD thesis, Southern Cross University, Lismore. Tsoukas, H. (1989), The validity of idiographic research explanations, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 551-61. Woodward, T. (1997), Identifying and measuring customer-based brand equity and its elements for a service industry, PhD thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane. Yin, R.K. (1989), Case Study Research Design and Methods, 1st ed., Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Yin, R.K. (1994), Case Study Research Design and Methods, 2nd ed., Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Zinkhan, G.M. and Hirscheim, R. (1992), Truth in marketing theory and research: an alternative perspective, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 56 No. 2, pp. 80-8. Research design and data analysis 1209 Further reading Burke, J.B. (1985), The Day the Universe Changed, British Broadcasting Corporation, London. De Ruyter, K. and Scholl, N. (1998), Positioning qualitative research: reflections from theory and practice, Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 7-14. Eisenhardt, K.M. (1989), Building theory from case study research, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 14 No. 4, pp. 532-50. Godfrey, P.C. and Hill, C. (1995), The problem of unobservables in strategic management research, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 16 No. 7, pp. 519-33. About the authors Rana Sobh is a Research Fellow in the Department of Marketing, University of Auckland. Her research interests are in consumer psychology and goal-directed behaviour in particular. She is also interested in applying the realism paradigm in consumer research and mixing qualitative and quantitative research methods. Rana Sobh is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: r.sobh@auckland.ac.nz Chad Perry is a retired professor of marketing and management at Southern Cross University in Australia. His research interests are qualitative research methods and relationship marketing. To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints