Title: Phonological Neighborhood Density Effects on Treatment of Naming in Aphasia

Similar documents
Table 1. Results for all participants (pts) for tests given pre- and post-treatment

Psychology G4470. Psychology and Neuropsychology of Language. Spring 2013.

African American English-Speaking Children's Comprehension of Past Tense: Evidence from a Grammaticality Judgment Task Abstract

Comprehensive Reading Assessment Grades K-1

Clinician Portal: Enabling a Continuity of Concussion Care

SPECIFIC LEARNING DISABILITIES (SLD)

Patterns of Strengths and Weaknesses in L.D. Identification

Technical Report. Overview. Revisions in this Edition. Four-Level Assessment Process

Improvement of Visual Attention and Working Memory through a Web-based Cognitive Training Program

Treatment for naming deficits in participants with aphasia

Areas of Processing Deficit and Their Link to Areas of Academic Achievement

The effect of instrumentality and verb-noun name relation on verb retrieval in bilingual Greek-English anomic aphasic individuals

Critical Review: The efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation approaches for recovery of memory impairments following stroke

Semantic naming errors 1. Mimicking aphasic semantic errors in normal speech production: Evidence from a novel. experimental paradigm

452 RUMMER AND ENGELKAMP

62 Hearing Impaired MI-SG-FLD062-02

PROP OSAL FOR A GRA DUA TE CE RTIFIC ATE P ROG RAM IN COGNITIVE SCIENCE AT THE UNIVERSITY OF MASSACH USETTS/AMHE RST

LIST OF PUBLICATIONS Kati Renvall 2014

Cognitive Rehabilitation of Blast Traumatic Brain Injury

Phonological naming therapy in jargon aphasia: Positive but paradoxical effects

Critical Review: Sarah Rentz M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

WMS III to WMS IV: Rationale for Change

Critical Review: What are the effects of adding music to the treatment of speech and language disorders in pre-school and school aged children?

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Language competence among cognitively non-disabled individuals with cerebral palsy

Effects of hearing words, imaging hearing words, and reading on auditory implicit and explicit memory tests

CURRICULUM VITAE. Toby Macrae, Ph.D., CCC-SLP

There are many reasons why reading can be hard. This handout describes

Reading Competencies

Prof Dr Dr Friedemann Pulvermüller Freie Universität Berlin WS 2013/14 Progress in Brain Language Research Wed, 4-6 pm ct, K 23/11

Sherry Peter M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Developing a standardized measure of short-term memory and syntactic complexity: results from subtests of the CRTT-R

PERSPECTIVE. How Top-Down is Visual Perception?

L OCUTOUR. Get Ready to Spell! MULTIMEDIA COGNITIVE REHABILITATION

Frequently Asked Questions about Making Specific Learning Disability (SLD) Eligibility Decisions

Evidence-Based Practice and This Course

Tamiko Azuma. Curriculum Vitae (Updated July 2004)

Categories of Exceptionality and Definitions

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

Activation of intentional mechanisms through utilization of nonsymbolic movements in aphasia rehabilitation

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION

Psychological and Neuropsychological Testing

RESTORATIVE TECHNIQUES IN COGNITIVE REHABILITATION: PROGRAM DESIGN AND CLINICAL BENEFITS

Report of Children with Disabilities (IDEA) Exiting Special Education by Basis of Exit, Disability Category and Age

The Effects of Speech Production and Vocabulary Training on Different Components of Spoken Language Performance

Context effects of pictures and words in naming objects, reading words, and generating simple phrases

Sophia van Hees a b, Anthony Angwin b, Katie McMahon c & David Copland a b d a Centre for Clinical Research, University of

INCREASE YOUR PRODUCTIVITY WITH CELF 4 SOFTWARE! SAMPLE REPORTS. To order, call , or visit our Web site at

Functional Auditory Performance Indicators (FAPI)

The Brain. Hemisphere-specific Deficits. Broca s Area 1/2/11. Cerebral Specialization

MICHIGAN TEST FOR TEACHER CERTIFICATION (MTTC) TEST OBJECTIVES FIELD 062: HEARING IMPAIRED

History: Memory & the brain

Joseph K. Torgesen, Department of Psychology, Florida State University

Evaluating Survey Questions

The Continuous Performance Test (CPT) as an Assessment of. Voluntary Attention in Preschool Children at High Risk for Learning.

Importance, Selection and Use of Outcome Measures. Carolyn Baum, PhD, OTR, FAOTA Allen Heinemann, PhD, ABPP (RP), FACRM

MA IN REHABILITATION IN LANGUAGE AND SPEECH PATHOLOGY (TAUGHT IN BULGARIAN)

Direct Evidence Delay with A Task Decreases Working Memory Content in Free Recall

Critical Review: Examining the Effectiveness of the Orton-Gillingham Reading Approach for Poor Readers in Elementary School

Aphasia Assessment. Introduction. Etiology

3030. Eligibility Criteria.

Treatment for Acquired Apraxia of Speech. Kristine Stanton Grace Cotton

How To Treat Aphasia

IV. Publications 381

Essentials of WAIS-IV Assessment

Overview. Neuropsychological Assessment in Stroke. Why a Neuropsychologist. How to make a referral. Referral Questions 11/6/2013

Potential Application of Lifelogging Technology (SenseCam) in Rehabilitation

SPPB. Research lines 18/02/2008. Neurociència cognitiva del llenguatge (i d altres funcions cognitives) Speech Perception Production and Bilingualism

Relevant Concreteness and its Effects on Learning and Transfer

A Cognitive Neuropsychological Approach to Assessment and Intervention in Aphasia

Lecture 2, Human cognition

Florida Center for Reading Research RAVE-O

Tracking the Time Course of Word-Frequency Effects in Auditory Word Recognition With Event-Related Potentials

Gf - FLUID INTELLIGENCE

CHARACTERIZING THE EFFECT OF VIDEOPHONE CONVERSATIONS ON INTERSECTION DRIVING PERFORMANCE. Urbana, Illinois, USA

WHAT IS COGNITIVE INTERVENTION/REHABILITATION?

10 th Annual Eleanor M. Saffran Conference on Cognitive Neuroscience and Rehabilitation of Communication Disorders

Visual Memory Improvement in Recognition

Reading and writing processes in a neurolinguistic perspective

Experimental methods. Elisabeth Ahlsén Linguistic Methods Course

PSYCHOLOGICAL AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING

Communication Strategies for Primary Progressive Aphasia & FTD s:

Encyclopedia of School Psychology Neuropsychological Assessment

Introduction. Methods

Effects of Automated Transcription Delay on Non-native Speakers Comprehension in Real-time Computermediated

Chapter 7: Memory. Memory

Critical Review: Does music therapy have a positive impact on language functioning in adults with dementia?

CURRICULUM VITAE AMBER THIESSEN, Ph.D., CCC-SLP

UCSB Koegel Autism Center 1

Ph.D in Speech-Language Pathology

The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, fourth edition (CELF-4;

Marcia E. Humpal, M.Ed., MT-BC Ronna S. Kaplan, M.A. MT-BC. Journal of Music Therapy

Critical Review: Does print referencing during shared storybook reading improve pre-literacy skills in preschoolers?

Running Head: DYSLEXIA: CAUSES, PERFORMANCE DIFFERENCES Dyslexia 1

Assessment and Treatment of Cognitive Impairment after Acquired Brain Injury

Steps for Implementation: Discrete Trial Training

Long-term Memory for 400 Pictures on a CommonTheme

Made available courtesy of Speech-Language-Hearing Association of Virginia:

WHAT IS CEREBRAL PALSY?

Data, Measurements, Features

The Interaction of Concreteness and Phonological Similarity in Verbal Working Memory

Transcription:

Title: Phonological Neighborhood Density Effects on Treatment of Naming in Aphasia Summary: Anomia, a difficulty in word retrieval, is one of the most prevalent disturbances in aphasic speech production. Heterogeneity in naming impairments poses a challenge to developing treatment programs for word retrieval difficulties in aphasia. Recent cognitive descriptions of word-finding impairments have been based on a two-step model of word production (Dell, Schwartz, Martin, Saffran, & Gagnon, 1997). Disruptions in accessing one level may result in predominately word substitution or semantic errors, while difficulty with a different level may result in phonological errors, for example responses with only partial phonology and/or incorrect phoneme substitutions. Therefore, considering the error types when planning treatment programs for anomia, may prove to be efficacious by allowing clinicians to target therapy to a specific underlying deficit. Treatments targeting stage-specific deficits in the word retrieval process have recently been explored to improve word production. For impairments affecting the phonological access stage, therapies have been based on the rationale that repeated productions may re-strengthen the connections between the lexical representation and the phonological form. While this rationale is theoretically sound, most phonological treatments have not been shown to be effective beyond the trained items in the therapy context. Clinicians may need to consider additional aspects of the word retrieval process that may interact with word production. One of these factors is the role of phonological neighborhoods or collections of words that are phonetically similar to a specific target (Luce & Pisoni, 1998). For example, the words mat and rat would be neighbors of the word cat. Target words with many neighbors are characterized as having high density neighborhoods and promote increased activation during phonological access in word retrieval, while target words from sparse neighborhoods have fewer neighbors and fewer phonological forms become activated (Luce & Pisoni, 1998; Gordon, 2002). Since neighborhood density, like other lexical variables, has been shown to interact with the word retrieval process, it likely influences susceptibility of target words to error in aphasic speech production (Gordon, 2002). The influence of phonological neighborhoods has been explored extensively in both normal language recognition (Goldinger, Luce & Pisoni, 1989; Goldinger, Luce, Pisoni & Marcario, 1992; Luce & Pisoni, 1998; Vitevich & Luce, 1998) and production (Vitevitch, 1997), but limited evidence exists for how this lexical factor may enhance or hinder word retrieval in aphasic speech production. Generally, the findings indicate that dense phonological neighborhoods may facilitate lexical retrieval in individuals with aphasia and reduce a target s susceptibility to error, which is consistent with the data from normal speech production (Gordon 2002). Therefore, the present study explores if manipulating the phonological neighborhood density of therapy stimulus words has an effect on naming ability, particularly in individuals with predominately phonological-level deficits. The study used a single-subject basic ABA design with baseline, treatment, and maintenance phases. The treatment protocol was based on a study by Fisher et al. (2009) and used a basic picture naming paradigm. An individual with a primarily phonologically-based impairment, as determined by an extensive test battery, was selected as the participant. Prior to the therapy program a picture naming test consisting of 186 words with high and low phonological neighborhood density values was administered to the participant with aphasia and a

control participant three times. The stimuli for the treatment program included target words that were incorrectly produced in two of the three naming tests and either two phonologically related words or two unrelated words, yielding a total of 30 triplet sets. The triplet sets comprise four phonologically related conditions and each had 6 triplet sets: a) Front-matched high density triplets, b) Front-matched low density triplets, c) End-matched high density triplets, d) Endmatched low density triplets, and one unrelated set condition. There were also 16 untrained items from high and low density neighborhoods used to probe generalization. The therapy phase consisted of 24 treatment sessions with 8 consecutive sessions that trained triplet sets in each condition: front-matched, end-matched, and unrelated. The therapy protocol involved multiple presentations of pictures representing each of the words in a triplet set under the five conditions. Instructions for each condition were given, e.g. all three words begin with the same sound, or all three words end with the same sound(s). Accuracy on training items was recorded for each treatment session. Additionally, all trained and untrained items were probed through a picture naming test for naming accuracy and response time three times during the treatment phase and at one month after the conclusion of the therapy phase to determine maintenance gains. For the treatment data, effect size values comparing pre- and post-treatment performance were calculated, using a method outlined by Beeson and Robey (2006), for each treatment condition. The front-matched high density condition had a medium treatment effect size, d= 9.3, and the front-matched low density condition had a small effect size, d = 4.6. The remaining conditions did not show treatment effects. To assess naming performance during the treatment sessions, an analysis of neighborhood density by treatment session was completed by collapsing all treatment conditions into high and low density conditions, independent of front- or endmatched. A two-way repeated ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the independent variables of treatment session (session 1-8) and neighborhood density (high vs. low). Results indicated a significant main effects of treatment session, F (7,82) = 7.487, p < 0.01, and neighborhood density, F (1, 88) = 9.022, p < 0.01. Interaction between treatment session and neighborhood density was not significant. A similar analysis was completed in which all treatment conditions were collapsed into front- versus end-matched conditions. There was a significant main effect of treatment session, F (7, 81) = 5.959, p < 0.01. However, the main effect of matched-condition and the interaction were not significant. The present study serves as a preliminary investigation into the effect of phonological neighborhood density on naming performance in the treatment of individual with a phonologically-based word-finding impairment. Results suggest that training words from dense phonological neighborhoods may show greater improvements in naming ability. Additionally, training high density words within front-matched triplets may lead to the greatest treatment effect following training. Clinicians should be aware that stimulus parameters, such as phonological neighborhood density, may interact in complex ways with the effectiveness of treatment protocols.

References Beeson, P.M. & Robey, R.R. (2006). Evaluating single-subject treatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia literature. Neuropsychological Review, 16, 161-169. Dell, G.S., Schwartz, M.F., Martin, N., Saffran, E.M., Gagnon, D.A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801-838. Fisher, C.A., Wilshire, C.E., & Ponsford, J.L. (2009). Word discrimination therapy: A new technique for the treatment of a phonologically based word-finding impairment. Aphasiology, 23, 676-693. Goldinger, S.D., Luce, P.A., & Pisoni, D.B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501-518. Goldinger, S.D., Luce, P.A., Pisoni, D.B., & Marcario, J.K. (1992). Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: the roles of competition and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1211-1238. Gordon, J.K. (2002). Phonological neighborhood effects in aphasic speech errors: spontaneous and structured contexts. Brain and Language, 82, 113-145. Luce, P.A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the Neighborhood Activation Model. Ear and Hearing, 1-36. Vitevitch, M.S. (1997). The neighborhood characteristics of malapropisms. Language and Speech, 40, 211-228. Vitevitch, M.S., & Luce, P.A. (1998). When words compete: levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9, 325-329.

Figures and Graphs Figure 1: Therapy Program Design Figure 2: Treatment Protocol for each Triplet P1, P2, P3- three pictures representing each word within the triplet set. The brackets indicated what was visually presented on a PowerPoint slide. The arrows show naming order. Figure 3: Graphs of percent correct naming accuracy during treatment sessions (S) and for baseline (B) and probe naming tests (P) for each condition. Baseline and probe sessions consisted of a randomized naming test of treatment items and the treatment protocol involved naming pictures in triplet set.