This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Similar documents
Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Report on the Ontario Principals Council Leadership Study

Humans show mate copying after observing real mate choices

Running head: GENDER EFFECT 1. Gender Effect of Parent-Child Relationships on Parental Health. Jazmine V. Powell

Two-Way ANOVA Lab: Interactions

SCHOOL OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SCIENCES DON T FORGET TO RECODE YOUR MISSING VALUES

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender,

Introduction to Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) Limitations of the t-test

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL 1. The Effects of Alcohol on the Acceptability of Casual Sex. College of Western Idaho

TEACHERS AS ROLE MODELS FOR STUDENTS LEARNING STYLES

forum Forecasting Enrollment to Achieve Institutional Goals by Janet Ward Campus Viewpoint

USE OF AN ONLINE RULER TO SELF MEASURE DIGIT RATIO

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal

IMPACT OF TRUST, PRIVACY AND SECURITY IN FACEBOOK INFORMATION SHARING

The Best Men Are (Not Always) Already Taken

Beyond Tops and Bottoms

A good honors thesis topic has four basic attributes. It is. Narrow Interesting Original Feasible.

Introducing Social Psychology

The Relationship between Gender and Attitudes towards Marriage

Analysis of the perceptions of accounting students and practitioners regarding the ethnicity of earnings management post Sarbanes-Oxley

Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley

Test Bias. As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but

UNDERSTANDING DOMESTIC VIOLENCE IN CAMBODIA

Advertisements gaining control. Advertisement is affecting our everyday lives through the media, and many individuals

Additional Probability Problems

HIV/AIDS: AWARENESS AND BEHAVIOUR

Examining Differences (Comparing Groups) using SPSS Inferential statistics (Part I) Dwayne Devonish

research/scientific includes the following: statistical hypotheses: you have a null and alternative you accept one and reject the other

Role of husbands and wives in Ephesians 5

Measurement and Measurement Scales

THE BODY LANGUAGE RELATIONSHIPS

Module 9. Building Communication Skills

International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy 98

Topic #6: Hypothesis. Usage

Factors Influencing Laptop Buying Behavior a Study on Students Pursuing Ug/Pg in Computer Science Department of Assam University

What is the Humanist Perspective? What are the key ideas in the Humanistic perspective of personality?

Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships

Validation of the Chally Assessment System with a Sample of Sales Representatives

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE

Expectancy Value Theory: Motivating Healthcare Workers

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores

Social Media Study in European Police Forces: First Results on Usage and Acceptance

Which Infidelity Type Makes You More Jealous? Decision Strategies in a Forced-choice Between Sexual and Emotional Infidelity

CRM Systems and Customer Survey Measurement A Panoramic View of Customers by Jamie Baker-Prewitt, Ph.D.,Vice President, Burke, Inc.

Experimental Design and Hypothesis Testing. Rick Balkin, Ph.D.

Joseph Fordham. Kuo-Ting Huang. Corrie Strayer. Rabindra Ratan. Michigan State University

Mind on Statistics. Chapter 4

Association Between Variables

CALCULATIONS & STATISTICS

Influence of Gender and Age in Aggressive Dream Content of Spanish Children and Adolescents

Sample Paper for Research Methods. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne

GIVING VOICE TO VALUES: BRIEF INTRODUCTION

LOCUS OF CONTROL AND DRINKING BEHAVIOR IN AMERICAN INDIAN ALCOHOLICS AND NON-ALCOHOLICS

Social Exchange Theory Applied to Romantic. Relationships

Preventing Unplanned Pregnancy and Completing College AN EVALUATION OF ONLINE LESSONS

Preskill S., & Brookfield, S.D. (2009). Learning as a way of leading: Lessons from the struggle for social justice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Q FACTOR ANALYSIS (Q-METHODOLOGY) AS DATA ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE

Sociology- Is it just Common Sense? True or False?

AN INTRODUCTION TO SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. Here in the Philippines, we believe in the saying of our national hero Dr.

LAB : THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

Chapter Five: RELATIONSHIP DYNAMICS

The Work Environment for Tenure-Track/Tenured Faculty at the University of Maryland. ADVANCE Research and Evaluation Report for CMNS

6.4 Normal Distribution

Same-Sex Marriage: Breeding Ground for Logical Fallacies

Pair Programming Improves Student Retention, Confidence, and Program Quality

Writing a degree project at Lund University student perspectives

A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF WORKFORCE DIVERSITY IN SERVICE AND MANUFACTURING SECTORS IN INDIA

Choosing Human Resources Development Interventions

Test Anxiety, Student Preferences and Performance on Different Exam Types in Introductory Psychology

An Examination of the Association Between Parental Abuse History and Subsequent Parent-Child Relationships

How To Understand Different Cultures

NEW JERSEY VOTERS DIVIDED OVER SAME-SEX MARRIAGE. A Rutgers-Eagleton Poll on same-sex marriage, conducted in June 2006, found the state s

Title. Euphausia superba. Author. Sophia Erb

Chapter 8 Hypothesis Testing Chapter 8 Hypothesis Testing 8-1 Overview 8-2 Basics of Hypothesis Testing

HYPOTHESIS TESTING: CONFIDENCE INTERVALS, T-TESTS, ANOVAS, AND REGRESSION

What issues can be resolved in mediation? What is mediation?

Sweden. Act on Equality between Women and Men. The Equal Opportunities Act (SFS 1991:433)

Infidelity and Kin Selection: Does Cheating Seem as Bad When it s All in the Family?

Behaving Intelligently: Leadership Traits & Characteristics Kristina G. Ricketts, Community and Leadership Development

Research Design and Research Methods

The Personal Learning Insights Profile Research Report

Father s height (inches)

Psychology 60 Fall 2013 Practice Exam Actual Exam: Next Monday. Good luck!

Changes in the Demographic Characteristics of Texas High School Graduates. Key Findings

The Mozart effect Methods of Scientific Research

Running head: BODY ART AND ACADEMIC SUCCESS 1

Attitudes About Cosmetic Surgery: Gender and Body Experience

GREENE VALUATION ADVISORS, LLC

Servant Leadership Practices among School Principals in Educational Directorates in Jordan

Chapter 2 Quantitative, Qualitative, and Mixed Research

Who takes the lead? Social network analysis as a pioneering tool to investigate shared leadership within sports teams. Fransen et al.

Why are there so few women in the tech industry in Oslo, Norway?

AP: LAB 8: THE CHI-SQUARE TEST. Probability, Random Chance, and Genetics

UNDERSTANDING THE TWO-WAY ANOVA

The Effect of Flexible Learning Schedule on Online Learners Learning, Application, and Instructional Perception

Mode and Patient-mix Adjustment of the CAHPS Hospital Survey (HCAHPS)

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

Attitudes about Charter Cable

Transcription:

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit: http://www.elsevier.com/copyright

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 1016 1019 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp FlashReports Who s chasing whom? The impact of gender and relationship status on mate poaching Jessica Parker *, Melissa Burkley Oklahoma State University, Department of Psychology, Stillwater, OK 74078, United States article info abstract Article history: Received 22 January 2009 Revised 30 March 2009 Available online 3 May 2009 Keywords: Mate poaching Cheating Gender Relationship status Are women more interested in men who are already in a relationship? Female and male participants who were single or in a relationship viewed information about an opposite-sex other and indicated their interest in pursuing this target. Half of the participants were told that the target was single and half read that the target was currently in a relationship. The results showed that only single women were more interested in pursuing an attached target rather than a single target. We discuss how these results add to what is already known about mate poaching. Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. A commonly heard complaint among women is that all the good men are taken, but is it possible that this perception is really based on the fact that taken men are perceived as good? This belief that women are attracted to men who are currently attached is commonly depicted in movies, television and the tabloids, but few have investigated it empirically. The purpose of this experiment was to investigate (1) if women are more likely than men to prefer an already taken individual and (2) if this gender difference depends on the relationship status of the participant. This tendency to pursue someone who is already in a romantic relationship is often referred to as mate poaching (Schmitt, 2004; Schmitt & Buss, 2001). Mate poaching appears to be a relatively common practice that occurs across a wide range of cultures. For example, Schmitt (2004) found that across ten world regions, 57% of men and 35% of women indicated they had engaged in an attempt at mate poaching, suggesting that this behavior is a universal mating practice. Researchers have not only identified the frequency of mate poaching but have also examined the characteristics that make someone more or less likely to poach. For example, people who mate poach are more likely to be low in agreeableness and conscientiousness than those who do not (Schmitt & Buss, 2001). However, the mate poaching characteristic that has received the most attention is gender. * Corresponding author. E-mail address: jessica.parker@okstate.edu (J. Parker). Gender differences in mate poaching Several studies have investigated whether men or women are more likely to engage in mate poaching; however, the results have been inconclusive. Some argue that men would be more likely to mate poach because men cheat more in general (e.g., Clark & Hatfield, 1989; Clark, Shaver, & Abrahams, 1999). The work by Schmitt and colleagues seems to support this assertion by showing that male respondents are more likely to report engaging in mate poaching tactics (Schmitt, 2004; Schmitt & Buss, 2001). Conversely, some argue that mate poaching is different from other forms of cheating and as such, women may be more likely to practice it. Research on animal mating (e.g., fish, birds) has shown that female animals are more likely to choose a male that has already been chosen by other females (e.g., Dugatkin, 1992; Galeg & White, 1998). The assumption is that chosen men signal they have desirable qualities that non-chosen men do not have and as a result, women perceive them as more viable mates. Research on human preferences does show that women rate men as more desirable when they are surrounded by other women, compared to being alone or surrounded by other men (Hill & Buss, 2008). Conversely, men rate women as less desirable when they are surrounded by other men, compared to being alone or surrounded by other women. Although this work did not examine mate poaching per se (i.e., the target was not described as being in a relationship with any of the surrounding individuals), it does suggest that women may be more likely to copy the mate preferences of their peers, a pattern that closely mirrors the animal research previously described. 0022-1031/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.022

J. Parker, M. Burkley / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 1016 1019 1017 In an attempt to more directly examine gender differences in mate poaching, Uller and Johansson (2002) had female participants interact with two men, one who wore a wedding ring. The women then rated both men on a variety of dimensions, including physical attractiveness and interest in dating and having sex with each man. The results showed that women rated both men equally across all the dimensions. A more recent study, however, did find evidence that women are more attracted to attached men, but this effect was influenced by the women s ovulation cycle. Bressan and Stranieri (2008) showed women several photos of men along with descriptions that the men were either single or attached. Results indicated that women who were currently in a relationship themselves were more attracted to attached men, but this was only the case when their fertility level was low. Conversely, when the women s fertility level was high, they were more attracted to single men. For single women, there was no difference in attractiveness ratings across the various conditions. This work therefore suggests that women are more attracted to taken men, but only when they are in a relationship themselves and are not fertile. Thus, research on gender differences in mate poaching seems inconclusive. Work by Schmitt and colleagues suggest that men are more likely to mate poach. However, because this work relies solely on self-report data, it is possible that women are just less likely to admit that they have mate poached. Research using experimental designs instead suggests that women are more likely to mate poach, but only when they are in a relationship themselves and are not fertile. In addition to the differences in research methods, these studies also differ on exactly what they are measuring. The work by Schmitt asked people to report on their own poaching proclivities; whereas, the experimental research focused exclusively on attractiveness ratings. Attraction, however, is just one aspect of mating decisions. As such, it is likely that studies focusing solely on attraction ratings do not capture all the aspects of mate poaching and this may also explain the inconsistency in findings. For instance, Dugatkin (2000) suggests that women s preference for attached mates will be much more pronounced when asked to indicate their interest in marrying the target rather than just rating the attractiveness of the target. Present study The purpose of the present study is to provide a more complete picture of the gender differences in mate poaching. Our first goal was to utilize dependent measures that capture a wider range of mate poaching experiences, rather than focusing exclusively on attraction ratings as other studies have done. This study therefore included a variety of questions that assessed participants interests in pursuing a relationship with the target. Our second goal was to incorporate a factor that we believed would interact with gender to influence mate poaching: The relationship status of the participant. As Schmitt and Buss (2001) pointed out, poaching attraction may vary depending on whether the mate poacher is single or already in a relationship (p. 913). Yet, to date, only one study has examined relationship status in mate poaching (Bressan & Stranieri, 2008), but since this study focused exclusively on women, it is unknown if relationship status effects men and women equally. The present study is the first to examine both men and women s evaluations of single and attached targets, and it is also the first to do so with both single and attached participants. It was predicted that women would be more interested in an attached target compared to a single target but that men would not show this preference. However, this female preference for an attached target was only expected to occur for single women. If attached men signal desirable resources and a willingness to commit to family life as some have suggested (Dugatkin, 2000; Uller & Johansson, 2002), then this signal should be more appealing to single women who are lacking such resources. Women who are in a relationship themselves have already found a mate who is willing to commit and so it is unlikely that they will be enticed by such prospects. Method Participants and design The sample consisted of 184 undergraduates (97 women) from Oklahoma State University, with 46% of the sample identified as single (35 women, 49 men) and 54% as attached (62 women, 38 men). Participation was for partial course credit. The study involved a 2 (gender: women versus men) 2 (relationship status: single versus attached) 2 (target: single versus attached) factorial design. Materials and procedure Participants were seated in an individual computer cubicle and were told the study was examining similarity effects on attraction. Next, participants completed a survey on their personal preferences, including qualities that would describe their ideal romantic partner (questions were similar to those used on match.com or eharmony.com). Participants were told the computer would use this information to match them with a fellow student in the database that responded similarly. Next, the computer supposedly generated a match and participants were shown a photograph and descriptive information regarding an opposite-sex target. All men viewed the same photo of a woman and all women viewed the same photo of a man. These photos were selected during pre-testing because they were both rated as being moderately attractive. Above the photograph, participants read that the target shared similar interests with the participant (based on previous survey responses). Therefore, all participants likely identified the target as physically attractive and perceived the target as similar in interests. In addition to this information, participants were randomly assigned to read that the target was either single or in a current romantic relationship. Next, participants indicated how interested they were in pursuing the target. These questions were created using modified items from other infidelity and mate poaching studies (Clark et al., 1999; Johnson & Rusbult, 1989). Specifically, participants responded to the following questions: How appealing is this person; How likely would you show interest (make eye contact, smile, etc.) in this person; How compatible do you think you are with this person; How likely would you initiate a conversation with this person; How likely would you initiate a relationship with this person; How direct would you be in initiating a romantic relationship with this person. Responses were made on a 3 (e.g., very unlikely) to +3 (e.g., very likely) scale. The items demonstrated good internal reliability (a =.81) and were combined into a single composite score, with higher scores indicating a greater interest in pursuing the target. Participants also rated how physically attractive the target was on a 3 (very unattractive) to +3 (very attractive) scale. 1 1 Although the ratings of attraction and interest were positively correlated (r=.57, p <.01), we expected them to result in different patterns (e.g., Dugatkin, 2000) and therefore treated them as separate theoretical constructs this separation is also consistent with other attraction research (e.g., study five of Elliot & Niesta, 2008; Uller & Johansson, 2002).

1018 J. Parker, M. Burkley / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 1016 1019 Results Interest in pursuing the target Participants interest in pursuing the target was analyzed using a 2 (gender: women versus men) 2 (relationship status: single versus attached) 2 (target: single versus attached) analysis of variance (ANOVA). The only significant main effect was gender, F(1, 176) = 21.08, p <.001, g 2 =.11, such that men (M =.98, SD =.71) were more interested than women (M =.47, SD =.85) in pursuing the target. As predicted, this main effect was qualified by a significant three-way interaction, F(1, 176) = 7.77, p =.01, g 2 =.04. To reveal the pattern of data underlying the three-way interaction, simple main effects were analyzed separately for single and attached participants. For single participants (see top of Fig. 1), there was a significant effect of gender, F(1, 80) = 8.21, p =.01, g 2 =.09, such that single men (M =.91, SD =.71) were more interested than single women (M =.47, SD =.82) in pursuing the target. Importantly, this effect was qualified by a significant gender target interaction, F(1, 80) = 6.23, p =.02, g 2 =.07. As predicted, single women were more interested in pursuing an attached target (M =.75, SD =.73) than a single target (M =.17, SD =.83), F(1, 80) = 5.46, p =.02. However, single men showed no difference in interest between the attached (M =.81, SD =.73) and single target (M = 1.05, SD =.69), F(1, 80) = 1.23, p =.27. As expected, single women were more interested in poaching an attached target rather than pursuing a single target, whereas single men were not. For participants in a relationship (see bottom of Fig. 1), there was also an effect of gender, F(1, 96) = 13.47, p <.001, g 2 =.12, such that attached men (M = 1.15, SD =.71) were more interested than attached women (M =.46, SD =.86) in pursuing the target. As predicted, this factor did not interact with condition, F(1, 96) = 2.19, p =.14. Unlike single women, attached women were not more interested in pursuing the attached target compared to the single target. Interest in Target Interest in Target 3 2 1 0 3 2 1 0 Single Individuals Single Target Men Women Participant's Gender Attached Target Attached Individuals Single Target Men Women Participant's Gender Attached Target Fig. 1. Single participants (top) and attached participants (bottom) interest in pursuing the target. Attractiveness ratings Participants attractiveness ratings of the target were analyzed using a 2 (gender: women versus men) 2 (relationship status: single versus attached) 2 (target: single versus attached) ANOVA. The only effect that was significant was a main effect of gender, F(1, 176) = 6.39, p =.01, such that men rated the female target (M = 1.29, SD = 0.78) as more physically attractive than the women rated the male target (M =.92, SD = 1.10). None of the other main effects or interactions was significant. Discussion Our results showed an interesting mate poaching pattern. Although men were more interested in the target than women, this was because men were more interested in the target in general, regardless of whether she was attached or single. However, as predicted, single women were more interested in poaching an attached man rather than pursuing a single man. Interestingly, this indicates that single women are more interested in pursuing a man that is less available to them. As predicted, this gender difference in interest was not evident when the participants were in a committed relationship themselves. The attractiveness ratings did not show this same pattern, suggesting that attraction ratings do not capture the same dynamic as our interest in pursuit questions. This suggests that research investigating mate poaching should avoid only relying on attraction questions and should include items that tap into the broader range of mate poaching experiences. One limitation of the present study was that it used a single male and female target photo and although our pretest indicated both photos were perceived as moderately attractive, our study showed men s attractiveness ratings for the female photo were higher than women s ratings for the male photo. To address this issue, future studies on this topic should incorporate multiple female and male photos to increase the generalizability of the results. Another limitation was that participants did not actually interact with the romantic target; they simply indicated their interest in future interactions. Future studies on mate poaching could try to capture the mate poaching process as it unfolds during a dyadic interaction. According to a recent poll, most women who engage in mate poaching do not think the attached status of the target played a role in their poaching decision (Knadler, 2008), but our study shows this belief to be false. Single women in this study were significantly more interested in the target when he was attached. This may be because an attached man has demonstrated his ability to commit and in some ways his qualities have already been pre-screened by another woman. These findings elucidate the way that gender and relationship status interact to influence mate poaching tendencies. Acknowledgements This research was based on the master s thesis of Jessica Parker, who thanks the members of her committee, Edward Burkley and James Grice, for their guidance. References Bressan, P., & Stranieri, D. (2008). The best men are (not always) already taken: Female preference for single versus attached males depends on conception risk. Psychological Science, 19, 145 151. Clark, R. D., & Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual offers. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 2, 39 55. Clark, C. L., Shaver, P. R., & Abrahams, M. F. (1999). Strategic behaviors in romantic relationship initiation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 709 722. Dugatkin, L. A. (1992). Sexual selection & imitation: Females copy the mate choice of others. The American Naturalist, 139, 1384 1389. Dugatkin, L. A. (2000). The imitation factor: Evolution beyond the gene. New York: Free Press.

J. Parker, M. Burkley / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 1016 1019 1019 Elliot, A. J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1150 1164. Galeg, B. G., & White, D. J. (1998). Mate-choice copying in Japanese quail. Animal Behavior, 55, 545 552. Hill, S. E., & Buss, D. M. (2008). The mere presence of opposite-sex others on judgments of sexual and romantic desirability: Opposite effects for men and women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 635 647. Johnson, D. J., & Rusbult, C. E. (1989). Resisting temptation: Devaluation of alternative partners as a means of maintaining commitment in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 967 980. Knadler, J. (2008). Invasion of the husband snatchers. Women s Health, 131 135. November. Schmitt, D. P. (2004). Patterns of universals of mate poaching across 53 nations: The effects of sex, culture, and personality on romantically attracting another person s partner. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 560 584. Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Human mate poaching: Tactics and temptations for infiltrating existing mateships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 894 917. Uller, T., & Johansson, L. C. (2002). Human mate choice and the wedding ring effect. Human Nature, 14, 267 276.