Habits A Repeat Performance David T. Neal, Wendy Wood, and Jeffrey M. Quinn



Similar documents
AMPHETAMINE AND COCAINE MECHANISMS AND HAZARDS

Epilepsy and Neuropsychology Dr. Sare Akdag, RPsych

Three Theories of Individual Behavioral Decision-Making

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

Growing Up With Epilepsy

Impacting the Brain of the Traumatized Child Dave Ziegler, PhD

CONTE Summer Lab Experience Application

Is Willpower a Limited Resource?

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

Chapter 7 Conditioning and Learning

University of Cincinnati Cognitive Behavioral Treatment for Substance Abuse Curriculum

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION

Coaching the team at Work

Building Resilience: An Integral Approach

Chapter 10 Social Psychology

Motivation What is it? Jay Gould 11/24/03

Executive Function in Context: The Role of Stress and Vulnerability and Opportunities for Intervention. Stephanie M. Jones

The Psychology of Personality

FACING YOUR FEARS: HOW TO SUCCESSFULLY PERFORM EXPOSURE THERAPY

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介

This historical document is derived from a 1990 APA presidential task force (revised in 1997).

Chapter 9 Overall Outline

11 Psychology MI-SG-FLD011-02

Chapter 13. Prejudice: Causes and Cures

Martha Brewer, MS, LPC,LADC. Substance Abuse and Treatment

References to Play in NAEYC Position Statements

Prevention & Recovery Conference November 28, 29 & 30 Norman, Ok

White House 806 West Franklin Street P.O. Box Richmond, Virginia

Types of Psychology. Alex Thompson. Psychology Class. Professor Phelps

Title: Conducting FBAs and Designing Behavior Interventions for Individuals with ASD: Keeping the Characteristics in Mind

Beef Demand: What is Driving the Market?

Chapter 10 Personality Name Period Date. MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question.

The Strength Model of Self-Control Roy F. Baumeister, 1 Kathleen D. Vohs, 2 and Dianne M. Tice 1

Classroom Management and Teaching Strategies. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Allison Gehrling ABSTRACT. Law & Disorder

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior

(back to the HAMS home page) ADDICTION AND LEARNING

Principles of Instruction. Teaching Letter-Sound Associations by Rebecca Felton, PhD. Introduction

SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: BASIC PRINCIPLES & APPLICATIONS

Why Study Psychology at The University of Western Ontario?

ACADEMIC DIRECTOR: Carla Marquez-Lewis Contact: THE PROGRAM Career and Advanced Study Prospects Program Requirements

Addiction: Learned Behaviour or a. Disease?

Principles of Learning for Traumatic Brain Injury Rehabilitation Mitchell Rosenthal Webinar Brain Injury Association of America June 7, 2011

Self-Concept and Self-Esteem in Adolescents

Theories of Intelligence and Learned Helplessness: The Role of Social Psychology in Schools

DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY

Simple Energy Techniques (SET)

WHAT IS COGNITIVE INTERVENTION/REHABILITATION?

Dr Sarah Blunden s Adolescent Sleep Facts Sheet

Motivation Early Work. What Is Motivation. Motivation Theories. Maslow s Hierarchy Of Needs. Alderfer s ERG Theory

Course Descriptions. Seminar in Organizational Behavior II

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function - Adult Version BRIEF-A. Interpretive Report. Developed by

SigmaRADIUS Leadership Effectiveness Report

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

12 Steps to Changing Neuropathways. Julie Denton

AN ELECTRONIC LEARNING ENVIRONMENT FOR APPLIED STATISTICS: QUALITY CARE AND STATISTICS EDUCATION IN HIGHER EDUCATION

Psychology. Academic Requirements. Academic Requirements. Career Opportunities. Minor. Major. Mount Mercy University 1

TREATMENTS FOR AUTISM

INTERNATIONAL FRAMEWORK FOR ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS CONTENTS

Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies,

Schema Theory Jeff Pankin Fall 2013

PTSD Ehlers and Clark model

Inventory for Client and Agency Planning Instructor Training Program

Sociology Central The Mass Media. 2. Ownership and Control: Theories

. new ideas are made use of, or used, in a. . solutions are extensive in their. . the impact of solutions extends to

& Global Climate Change

Specialisation Psychology

JOB PERFORMANCE NORTH CENTRAL MISSOURI COLLEGE

Post Traumatic Stress Disorder & Substance Misuse

Policy/Program Memorandum No. 8

Development Through the Lifespan. Psychosocial Stages During Infancy and Toddlerhood. First Appearance of Basic Emotions

Executive Functions: Developing an Independent Learner NOVEMBER 12, 2014 KATHLEEN BRUNSWICK, MA JEFFREY A. DETESO, PH.D.

1. Explain what causes the liquidity preference money (LM) curve to shift and why.

Recommendation, Loyalty - Retention. Market share Revenue. Negative WOM, Complaint, No Loyalty - Defection

Learning and cognitive effects of acquired brain injury caused by meningitis or septicaemia

Harm Reduction Strategies to Address Anxiety and Trauma. Presented by Jodi K. Brightheart, MSW

I am a principal and the Professional Training Coordinator at ACORN Food Dependency Recovery Service, a treatment and training program based in

VI TRAINING AND RETAINING VOLUNTEERS

Jack s Dyslexia Index indicates he has dyslexic difficulties that are mild in extent.

Executive Function and Student Success Karen Boutelle Associate Director

Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION Macomb1 ( )

Improvement of Visual Attention and Working Memory through a Web-based Cognitive Training Program

Documentation Guidelines for ADD/ADHD

Learning Theories 4- Behaviorism

Clinical Neuropsychology. Recovery & Rehabilitation. Alan Sunderland School of Psychology

DOMAIN 1 FOR READING SPECIALIST: PLANNING AND PREPARATION LEVEL OF PERFORMANCE COMPONENT UNSATISFACTORY NEEDS IMPROVEMENT PROFICIENT EXCELLENT

Community Support Program and Recovery Pathways. Skill Building. Goals, Objectives, Interventions and Skills

LESSON 7: LEARNING MODELS

AP ENGLISH LANGUAGE AND COMPOSITION 2008 SCORING GUIDELINES

Mindfulness in adults with autism spectrum disorders

A new direction for Delta Pacific: A case study

Pivotal Response Training: Parent Professional Collaboration

The Social Cognitive perspective and Albert Bandura

The Psychology Foundation of Australia (Incorporated in NSW) 26 February 2007

Cognitive and Motor Development. Four Domains. Interaction. Affective Cognitive Motor Physical. Why organize into domains?

The Wisconsin Comprehensive School Counseling Model Student Content Standards. Student Content Standards

PERCEIVED QUALITY IN THE DELIVERY OF BUSINESS SUPPORT SERVICES: A CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK (WITH PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS)

Course Descriptions Psychology

The Planning and Execution Assistant and Trainer (PEAT) The challenge of impaired executive functions

Transcription:

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Habits A Repeat Performance David T. Neal, Wendy Wood, and Jeffrey M. Quinn Duke University ABSTRACT Habits are response dispositions that are activated automatically by the context cues that co-occurred with responses during past performance. Experiencesampling diary studies indicate that much of everyday action is characterized by habitual repetition. We consider various mechanisms that could underlie the habitual control of action, and we conclude that direct cuing and motivated contexts best account for the characteristic features of habit responding in particular, for the rigid repetition of action that can be initiated without intention and that runs to completion with minimal conscious control. We explain the utility of contemporary habit research for issues central to psychology, especially for behavior prediction, behavior change, and self-regulation. KEYWORDS habit; automaticity; motivation; goals; behavior change; behavior prediction; self-regulation From self-help guru Anthony Robbins to the religion of Zen Buddhism, received wisdom exhorts people to be mindful, deliberative, and conscious in all they do. In contrast, contemporary research in psychology shows that it is actually people s unthinking routines or habits that form the bedrock of everyday life. Without habits, people would be doomed to plan, consciously guide, and monitor every action, from making that first cup of coffee in the morning to sequencing the finger movements in a Chopin piano concerto. But what is a habit? The cognitive revolution radically reshaped the behaviorist view that habits rely on simple stimulus response associations devoid of mental representation. Emerging is a more nuanced construct that includes roles for consciousness, goals, and motivational states. Fundamental questions persist, however, especially when comparing evidence across neuropsychology, animal-learning, and social-cognition literatures. Data from these fields support three views of habit, which we term the direct-context-cuing, implicit-goal, and motivated-context models. In this article, we consider these models Address correspondence to David Neal, Department of Psychology, Box 90085, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708; e-mail: dneal@ duke.edu. and explain the relevance for psychology of a reinvigorated habit construct. HABITS AFTER BEHAVIORISM Within current theorizing, habits are automated response dispositions that are cued by aspects of the performance context (i.e., environment, preceding actions). They are learned through a process in which repetition incrementally tunes cognitive processors in procedural memory (i.e., the memory system that supports the minimally conscious control of skilled action). The relatively primitive associative learning that promotes habits is shared in some form across mammalian species. Our own interest in habits has been fueled by the recognition that much of everyday action is characterized by repetition. In experience-sampling diary studies using both student and community samples, approximately 45% of everyday behaviors tended to be repeated in the same location almost every day (Quinn & Wood, 2005; Wood, Quinn, & Kashy, 2002). In these studies, people reported a heterogeneous set of actions that varied in habit strength, including reading the newspaper, exercising, and eating fast food. Although a consensual perspective on habit mechanisms has yet to develop, common to all views is the idea that many behavioral sequences (e.g., one s morning coffee-making routine) are performed repeatedly in similar contexts. When responses and features of context occur in contiguity, the potential exists for associations to form between them, such that contexts come to cue responses. In what follows, we outline three views of habitual control that build on this understanding. Direct Context Cuing According to the direct-context-cuing model, repeated coactivation forges direct links in memory between context and response representations. Once these links are formed via associative learning, merely perceiving a context triggers associated responses. Supporting evidence comes from research in which merely activating a construct, such as the elderly stereotype, influences the performance of relevant behaviors, such as a slow speed of walking (e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996). 198 Copyright r 2006 Association for Psychological Science Volume 15 Number 4

David T. Neal, Wendy Wood, and Jeffrey M. Quinn Readers might wonder if it is realistic that contexts cue responses through this simple mechanism in the absence of an implicit or explicit goal. The answer is not clear, given that social-cognition research has thus far demonstrated only a limited version of direct-cuing effects. For example, activating the elderly stereotype influences walking speed, but it remains to be demonstrated whether such activation can initiate walking itself. However, the direct cuing of repeated action by contexts is suggested by myriad findings in cognitive neuroscience that reveal reduced involvement of goal-related neural structures, such as the prefrontal cortex, when behaviors have come under habitual control (see Daw, Niv, & Dayan, 2005). Furthermore, animal-learning research using a clever paradigm in which reinforcers are devalued suggests direct control by context. When rats initially perform an instrumental behavior (e.g., pressing a bar for a food pellet), they appear to be guided by specific goal expectations; they cease the behavior if the reward is devalued (e.g., by pairing it with a toxin; Dickinson & Balleine, 1995). In contrast, when rats have extensively repeated a behavior, their responses appear to be cued directly by contextual stimuli (e.g., the bar); reward devaluation has little impact on continued performance. These data are commonly interpreted as indicating that habit formation involves a shift to direct context cuing. Implicit Goals Associative learning explains not only the direct binding of contexts and actions but also the binding of contexts and goals. In implicit-goal models, habits develop when people repeatedly pursue a goal via a specific behavior in a given context. An indirect association then forms between the context and behavior within the broader goal system. In support, Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2000) found in several experiments that the automatic activation of habitual responses (e.g., bicycle riding) only occurs when a relevant goal has first been made accessible (e.g., the thought of attending class). These studies did not measure people s real-world behavior, however, but focused instead on judgments about behavior. It remains to be seen whether such judgments tap the cognitive processes that actually drive behavior. In addition, there is good reason to think that habit performance itself does not depend on goal activation. Goaldriven responses tend to be dynamic and flexible, as evidenced by people sometimes substituting behaviors that serve a common goal. In contrast, habits emerge in a rigid pattern such that, for example, a habitual runner is unlikely to substitute a cycling class for running. Thus, although implicit goals provide potentially powerful guides to action, they do not plausibly explain the context cuing of habits. Motivated Contexts In another framework for understanding context-cued responses, contexts can acquire diffuse motivational value when they have preceded rewards in the past. When contexts predict rewards in this way, they energize associated responses without activating specific goals. Evidence of the motivating quality of contexts comes from animal studies of the neurotransmitters that mediate reward learning. For example, when monkeys first learn that a feature of the environment (e.g., a light) predicts a reward (e.g., a drop of juice) when a response is made (e.g., a lever press), neurotransmitter activity (i.e., dopamine release) occurs just after the reward (see Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, 1997). After repeated practice, the animal reaches for the lever when the light is illuminated. Furthermore, the neurotransmitter response is no longer elicited by the juice but instead by the light. In this way, environmental cues can acquire motivational value. Reward-predicting environments are thought to signal the cached (or long-run future) value of an action without signaling a specific outcome (e.g., juice; Daw et al., 2005). This diffuse motivation may explain the rigid nature of context cuing, given that cached values do not convey a specific desired outcome that could be met by substitutable means. Contributing further to the rigidity of habits, neural evidence indicates that, with repetition, whole sequences of responses become chunked or integrated in memory with the contexts that predict them (Barnes, Kubota, Hu, Jin, & Graybiel, 2005). Chunked responses are cued and implemented as a unit, consistent with the idea that habits require limited conscious control to proceed to completion. This quality of habitual responding is frustratingly evident when, for example, trying to fix a well-practiced but badly executed golf swing or dance-step sequence. As yet, the motivated-context idea has been tested primarily with animals. Its promise as a model of human habits comes from evidence that reward-related neurotransmitter systems are shared across species (e.g., in humans, dopamine is elicited by monetary reward). Multiple Habit Mechanisms The high degree of repetition in daily life observed in the diary research of Wood et al. (2002) is likely to be a product of multiple habit-control mechanisms that draw, in various cases, on direct context associations as well as on diffuse motivations. Although we consider implicit goals to be an implausible mediator of habitual behavior, they undoubtedly contribute to some types of repetition. Whether habits are cued directly or are diffusely motivated, they are triggered automatically by contexts and performed in a relatively rigid way. These features of responding have important implications for theories of behavior prediction, behavior change, and self-regulation. BEHAVIOR PREDICTION Predicting behavior is a central project in many areas of psychology. The automated repetition of habits sets limits on when mindful constructs such as intentions, attitudes, and decisions Volume 15 Number 4 199

Habits predict future action. In illustration, Ji Song and Wood (2006) predicted how often college students purchased fast food or watched TV news during a week-long period from their behavioral intentions and from their habits. Students acted on their intentions when their habits were weak or moderate in strength (as indicated by infrequent past performance or unstable contexts). Thus, for example, students who intended to purchase fast food in fact did so (Fig. 1). In contrast, students with strong habits (frequent past performance in stable contexts) repeated their past behavior regardless of their intentions. In short, the utility of intentions as predictors of behavior declined as habit strength increased. Intentions guided actions primarily when habits had not been formed. This pattern supports the claim that habitual responding can be cued independently of people s intentions. Researchers sometimes equate strong habits simply with frequent past performance. This may be appropriate for actions that tend to be performed narrowly in a given context (e.g., using seat belts). However, for actions performed across multiple circumstances, habits depend on contiguity between behavior and contexts. Thus, the pattern in Figure 1 emerged when habits were assessed from frequency of past purchasing with certain others, but not when assessed from measures of frequency alone. BEHAVIOR CHANGE The mechanisms of habitual control pose a particular challenge for changing behavior. Public health campaigns and other informational interventions are designed to change beliefs. However, for habits, changing minds does not necessarily mean changing behavior. In illustration, Webb and Sheeran s (2006) meta-analytic review compared the effectiveness of persuasive messages and other interventions in changing people s intentions versus changing their behavior. For course enrollment and Log of Fast Food Purchasing 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 Less favorable intentions Intentions to Purchase Fast Food Weak habit Moderate habit Strong habit More favorable intentions Fig. 1. Frequency of purchasing fast food over 1 week as a function of habit strength (based on frequency of past fast-food purchasing in the presence of particular other people) and the intention to purchase fast food. Habit strength moderated the extent to which intentions guided action. Data from Ji Song and Wood (2006). other actions that were not easily repeated into habits, interventions that changed intentions also changed behavior. But for exercising and other behaviors that people could repeat into habits, interventions that changed intentions had limited effect on behavior. Despite insensitivity to informational interventions, habit performance should be vulnerable to changes in the performance context. To test this idea, Wood, Tam, and Guerrero Witt (2005) examined change in college students habitual behaviors of exercising, reading the paper, and watching TV upon transferring to a new university. In general, students performed these actions when they intended to do so. However, a mark of strong habits is frequent performance regardless of people s intentions, provided that contexts remain stable. In support, habit performance continued to be cued independently of intention only when students perceived that the context of performance was stable across the transfer. When the performance context changed with the transfer, apparently strong habits were no longer cued automatically, and students continued to exercise only if they intended to do so. Thus, context change disrupted performance of strong habits, bringing them under intentional control. The performance of weak habits, in contrast, varied with intentions regardless of context stability. The data for exercising and for newspaper reading are presented in Figure 2. In general, change in performance contexts is likely to be an important ingredient in interventions to change many everyday behaviors. This is especially true within the health domain, given that five of the leading health risks in the U.S. emerge from everyday repetition of action substance abuse, obesity, tobacco use, risky sexual behavior, and inadequate exercise. Verplanken and Wood (2006) proposed that effective habitchange strategies might target interventions for times when people are naturally changing performance contexts. For example, to increase bus ridership, some metropolitan transit services provide free passes and route information to new residents, a group yet to establish relevant habits and hence open to such influence. Also, given the importance of context cues in changing habits, public policy can be oriented toward structural changes and supports for desired behaviors (e.g., sidewalks to encourage exercise). SELF-REGULATION Self-regulation involves monitoring and adjusting responses in the service of the self. In current theorizing, this process occurs by comparing current states with goals and engaging control processes when the two are discrepant. Given that habits are not guided by goals, how are they regulated? One answer comes from research on the brain systems involved with conflict monitoring (Yeung, Botvinick, & Cohen, 2004). This work suggests that negative behavioral outcomes can be detected indirectly through the activation of multiple conflicting responses. For example, people asked to identify the ink color when they see the 200 Volume 15 Number 4

David T. Neal, Wendy Wood, and Jeffrey M. Quinn 1.2 0.8 Frequency of exercising 0.8 0.4 0.0 0.4 0.8 1.2 Frequency of reading paper 0.4 Stable context - existing habit Stable context - existing habit 0.6 Stable context - no habit Stable context - no habit Changed context - existing habit 0.8 Changed context - existing habit Changed context - no habit Changed context - no habit 1.0 Less favorable intentions More favorable intentions Less favorable intentions More favorable intentions 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 0.2 Fig. 2. Frequency of students exercise (left panel) and newspaper-reading (right panel) behavior after moving to a new university as a function of (a) whether students had an existing habit for the behavior at their old university, (b) their intentions to perform the behavior at the new university, and (c) change in the contexts in which they performed the behavior after the move. Students with strong habits whose performance contexts changed little across the transfer carried over their habits to the new university regardless of their intentions. In contrast, students with strong habits who experienced a change in performance contexts carried over their habits only when the behavior was accompanied by a favorable intention. For those without habits, behavior performance was predicted by favorable intentions irrespective of context change. Data from Wood, Tam, and Guerrero Witt (2005). word red written in green ink (i.e., the Stroop task) can become aware of incorrect responses by detecting the activation of two incompatible responses (i.e., red and green ). The presence of multiple conflicting responses is apparently a common feature of situations in which negative outcomes occur, and therefore can be used as a proxy for evaluating outcomes. Thus, conflict monitoring provides a precedent for self-regulation without goals. Self-regulation also involves controlling the implementation and inhibition of behavior. The automatic qualities of habits influence regulatory success through the ease with which responses are executed versus withheld. These effects of habits are most apparent when people have limited self-control resources. Thus, Vohs, Baumeister, and Ciarocco (2005) showed experimentally the difficulty of inhibiting habitual modes of selfpresentation when self-control resources have been reduced. Our four-day diary study (Neal & Wood, 2006) extended this investigation to the regulation of behavior in real life. For two of the days, we depleted people s control resources by requiring them to use their nondominant hand for everyday activities. When depleted, participants were less likely to perform nonhabitual behaviors but continued to perform habits successfully. They not only maintained beneficial habitual behavior, such as attending the gym, but also maintained bad habits, such as an afternoon trip to Krispy Kreme. Thus, habits represent a doubleedged sword within self-regulation; when self-control is limited, people perform desirable habits but fail to inhibit undesirable ones. Finally, habits influence the role of the self within self-regulation. Because people perform these actions often, it makes sense that habits would be central components of the self-concept. However, people generally consider their habits less informative about themselves than nonhabitual actions (Wood et al., 2002). In part, this may reflect that behaviors that are cued by context are unlikely to feel self-willed. In addition, the disconnection between habits and the self could emerge from their independent representation in memory. People s sense of self is informed by autobiographical memories, which are stored in the declarative system. Given that habits rely on procedural memory, they may be somewhat removed from these autobiographies and hence from the self-concept. CONCLUSION Having moved beyond its behaviorist connotations, the habit construct has a valuable and expanding role in many topics of concern to contemporary psychologists. Exciting research programs address how habits drive consumer choices, influence large-scale social processes, become shared across individuals and groups, and inform moral reasoning. Such applications need to be informed by continued work addressing the basic mechanisms underlying habit performance, especially the mechanisms by which contexts motivate responding and by which responses, once initiated, can proceed independently of intentions and with minimal conscious control. Particularly needed is research that applies these specific mechanisms to understand the powerful effects of everyday habits observed in behavior prediction, change, and regulation. More generally, the pervasive effect of habits in everyday behavior is a key to understanding the difficulty people frequently experience in changing their behavior. People often fail in their attempts at changing everyday lifestyle habits such as their diet and level of exercise. Such failures are understandable given that cues such as time of day and location trigger repetition of past responses. Failures to change do not necessarily indicate poor willpower or insufficient understanding of health issues but instead the power of situations to trigger past responses. Habits Volume 15 Number 4 201

Habits keep us doing what we have always done, despite our best intentions to act otherwise. Recommended Reading Verplanken, B., & Wood, W. (2006). (See References) Wood, W., Quinn, J.M., & Kashy, D.A. (2002). (See References) Wood, W., Tam, L., & Guerrero Witt, M. (2005). (See References) REFERENCES Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53 63. Bargh, J.A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230 244. Barnes, T.D., Kubota, Y., Hu, D., Jin, D.Z., & Graybiel, A.M. (2005). Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature, 437, 1158 1161. Daw, N.D., Niv, Y., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1704 1711. Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. (1995). Motivational control of instrumental action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 162 167. Ji Song, M., & Wood, W. (2006). Habitual purchase and consumption: Habits and intentions guide behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication. Neal, D.T., & Wood, W. (2006). Ego-depletion and habits in everyday life. Unpublished manuscript, Duke University. Quinn, J.M., & Wood, W. (2005). Habits across the lifespan. Unpublished manuscript, Duke University. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P.R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 1593 1599. Verplanken, B., & Wood, W. (2006). Breaking and creating habits: Consequences for public policy interventions. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25, 90 103. Vohs, K.D., Baumeister, R.F., & Ciarocco, N.J. (2005). Self-regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory resource depletion impairs impression management and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 632 657. Webb, T.L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249 268. Wood, W., Quinn, J.M., & Kashy, D. (2002). Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1281 1297. Wood, W., Tam, L., & Guerrero Witt, M. (2005). Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 918 933. Yeung, N., Botvinick, M.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111, 931 959. 202 Volume 15 Number 4