SLEEP LATENCY AND DURATION ESTIMATES AMONG SLEEP DISORDER PATIENTS



Similar documents
SLEEP DISTURBANCE AND PSYCHIATRIC DISORDERS

A Healthy Life RETT SYNDROME AND SLEEP. Exercise. Sleep. Diet 1. WHY SLEEP? 4. ARE SLEEP PROBLEMS A COMMON PARENT COMPLAINT?

Tara Leigh Taylor, MD, FCCP Intensivist, Wyoming Medical Center

HEALTH EVIDENCE REVIEW COMMISSION (HERC) COVERAGE GUIDANCE: DIAGNOSIS OF SLEEP APNEA IN ADULTS DATE: 5/9/2013 HERC COVERAGE GUIDANCE

NREM Sleep EEG Frequency Spectral Correlates of Sleep Complaints in Primary Insomnia Subtypes

Polysomnography in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea. OHTAC Recommendation. Polysomnography in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea

Glossary Of Terms Related To The Psychological Evaluation Pain

MINISTERIO DE SALUD PUBLICA DIRECCION PROVINCIAL DE SALUD DEL GUAYAS HOSPITAL DE INFECTOLOGIA DR. JOSE DANIEL RODRIGUEZ MARIDUEÑA Guayaquil - Ecuador

Sleep Difficulties. Insomnia. By Thomas Freedom, MD and Johan Samanta, MD

Sleep-Wake Patterns in Brain Injury Patients in an Acute Inpatient Rehabilitation Hospital Setting

Maharashtra University of Health Sciences, Nashik. Syllabus. Fellowship Course in Sleep Medicine

Sleep Medicine. Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint. Purpose of the exam

DSM-IV PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSES OF PSYCHOGENIC NON-EPILEPTIC SEIZURES

Snoring and Obstructive Sleep Apnea (updated 09/06)

Medical Information to Support the Decisions of TUECs INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS

PREDICTORS OF NON-EPILEPTIC SEIZURES IN AN INPATIENT EPILEPSY PROGRAM

Rapid Resolution of Intense Suicidal Ideation after Treatment of Severe. From the Department of Psychiatry and Psychology (L.E.K.

DSM-5: A Comprehensive Overview

General Symptom Measures

Primary Care Management of Sleep Complaints in Adults

Sleep Medicine and Psychiatry. Roobal Sekhon, D.O.

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness

Psychological Questionnaires and Kernel Extension

Changes in the Evaluation and Treatment of Sleep Apnea

UNDERSTANDING THE TWO-WAY ANOVA

Dr Sarah Blunden s Adolescent Sleep Facts Sheet

Running Head: INTERNET USE IN A COLLEGE SAMPLE. TITLE: Internet Use and Associated Risks in a College Sample

DIAGNOSING SLEEP APNEA. Christie Goldsby Florida State University PHY /09/14

Trauma Center Assessment Package

Running head: ASPERGER S AND SCHIZOID 1. A New Measure to Differentiate the Autism Spectrum from Schizoid Personality Disorder

Corporate Medical Policy

A PROSPECTIVE EVALUATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN REASONS FOR DRINKING AND DSM-IV ALCOHOL-USE DISORDERS

Challenges to Detection and Management of PTSD in Primary Care

Pathological Gambling and Age: Differences in personality, psychopathology, and response to treatment variables

Don t just dream of higher-quality sleep. How health care should be

Anxiety and depression appropriate terms for cancer survivorship research?

Mental health issues in the elderly. January 28th 2008 Presented by Éric R. Thériault

Headache and Sleep Disorders 屏 東 基 督 教 醫 院 沈 秀 祝

Insomnia affects 1 in 3 adults every year in the U.S. and Canada.

Diseases and Health Conditions that can Lead to Daytime Sleepiness

Overview of DSM-5. With a Focus on Adult Disorders. Gordon Clark, MD

DISCRIMINANT FUNCTION ANALYSIS (DA)

Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Karen Elmore MD Robert K. Schneider MD Revised by Robert K. Schneider MD

Initial Evaluation for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Examination

PSYCHOLOGICAL AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING

Measurement & Data Analysis. On the importance of math & measurement. Steps Involved in Doing Scientific Research. Measurement

`çããçå=jéåí~ä= aáëçêçéêëw=^åñáéíó=~åç= aééêéëëáçå. aêk=`=f=lâçåü~ jéçáå~ä=aáêéåíçê lñäé~ë=kep=cçìåç~íáçå=qêìëí=

Dual Diagnosis or Dual Confusion: Limitations When Utilizing Non-specialist Clinical Data. Abstract

Preferred Practice Guidelines Bipolar Disorder in Children and Adolescents

Fatigue, Extended Work Hours, and Safety in the Workplace

Treatment of Obstructive Sleep Apnea (OSA)

MENTAL IMPAIRMENT RATING

EXPECTATIONS OF PHYSICIANS INTENDING TO PRACTISE SLEEP MEDICINE CHANGING SCOPE OF PRACTICE PROCESS BACKGROUND

SLEEP. Sleep Sleep disorders Lifestyle SCIENCE FAIR JUNE 11, Polysomnography (PSG) Polygraphy (PG) Neurophysiological parameters in PSG

Chemobrain. Halle C.F. Moore, MD The Cleveland Clinic October 3, 2015

The Importance of Psycho-social Aspects in Developing Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Professor Trudie Chalder King s College London

Sleep Deprivation and Post-Treatment (CBD)

Comparison of External Correlates of MMPI Substance Abuse Scales Across Sex and Race

The Clinical Use of the MSLT and MWT

The Do s & Don'ts of Mental Health Coding

Courses Descriptions. Courses Generally Taken in Program Year One

Our faculty has been hand-picked for their knowledge, experience, and enthusiasm for teaching

Depression & Multiple Sclerosis

Oncology Nursing Society Annual Progress Report: 2008 Formula Grant

Examination Content Blueprint

Underwriting Sleep Apnea

Statistics. Measurement. Scales of Measurement 7/18/2012

Sleep Disturbance in Children and Adolescents with ADHD: Unique Effects of Medication, ADHD Subtype, and Comorbid Status

Multivariate Analysis of Variance. The general purpose of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is to determine

Report on the Ontario Principals Council Leadership Study

Weighted Quilts and Their Effect on Sleep in Patients with Depression, Anxiety or Bi-polar Disorder: A Pilot Study

University of St. Thomas Health Services and Counseling ADD/ADHD Guidelines

Elizabeth A. Crocco, MD Assistant Clinical Professor Chief, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Miller

Scoring (manual, automated, automated with manual review)

SLEEP STUDIES AND THERAPY MANAGEMENT

kaiser medicaid uninsured commission on The Role of Medicaid for People with Behavioral Health Conditions November 2012

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) and Chronic Pain

1.0 Abstract. Title: Real Life Evaluation of Rheumatoid Arthritis in Canadians taking HUMIRA. Keywords. Rationale and Background:

National Depressive and Manic-Depressive Association Constituency Survey

Further Validation of Actigraphy for Sleep Studies

With Age Comes Knowledge? Sleep Knowledge in Australian Children

Diagnosis and Treatment

ADHD AND ANXIETY AND DEPRESSION AN OVERVIEW

Proceedings of the International MultiConference of Engineers and Computer Scientists 2009 Vol I IMECS 2009, March 18-20, 2009, Hong Kong

Introduction to the DSM-IV and Psychological Testing

Using the MMPI-2 to Detect Substance Abuse in an Outpatient Mental Health Setting

CHAPTER 2: CLASSIFICATION AND ASSESSMENT IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY KEY TERMS

MODULE. POSITIVE AIRWAY PRESSURE (PAP) Titrations

What is sleep apnea? 2/2/2010

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores

Update on Home Sleep Studies Is there a Home for Portable Monitoring?

Vulnerability to Stress-related Sleep Disturbance and Hyperarousal

Chris Bedford, Ph.D. Licensed Psychologist Clinic for Attention, Learning, and Memory

Written Example for Research Question: How is caffeine consumption associated with memory?

practitioners and physician assistants.advanceweb.com/features/articles/alcohol Abuse.aspx

DEPRESSION CARE PROCESS STEP EXPECTATIONS RATIONALE

Sleep. Drug and Alcohol Services South Australia. Progressive stages of the sleep cycle. Understanding the normal sleep pattern

NEW TRENDS AND ISSUES IN NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Postconcussive Syndrome Cases

MODULE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST (MSLT) AND MAINTENANCE OF WAKEFULNESS TEST (MWT)

News on sleep apnea for the commercial trucking industry. Support system leads to CPAP success

Transcription:

SLEEP LATENCY AND DURATION ESTIMATES AMONG SLEEP DISORDER PATIENTS Sleep Latency and Duration Estimates Among Sleep Disorder Patients: Variability as a Function of Sleep Disorder Diagnosis, Sleep History, and Psychological Characteristics Peter A. Vanable PhD, James E. Aikens PhD, Lakshminarayana Tadimeti MD, Brendan Caruana-Montaldo MD and Wallace B. Mendelson MD University of Chicago Study Objectives: Insomnia patients often report greater sleep disturbance than found via polysomnography; yet the specific patient factors related to such sleep time misperceptions are poorly understood. We sought to characterize the extent to which a diverse group of patients complaining of insomnia (n = 104) misperceive overnight total sleep time and sleep latency, and to identify patient factors associated with these variations. Design: Cross-sectional. Setting: University based sleep disorders center. Patients: Sleep disorder groups consisted of patients with psychophysiological insomnia (n=19), sleep state misperception (n=8), insomnia with depressive disorder (n=11), insomnia secondary to Axis I psychiatric disorder other than depression (n=21), periodic limb movement disorder (n=24), and obstructive sleep apnea (n=21). Measurement and Results: Patients completed a sleep history questionnaire and the MMPI, underwent overnight diagnostic polysomnographic assessment, and then estimated their total sleep time and sleep latency the subsequent morning. On average, patients overestimated sleep latency, but were equally likely to underestimate vs. overestimate total sleep time. Sleep time misperception was associated with longer periods of wakefulness following sleep onset, greater self-perceived sleep impairment, as well as several psychological dimensions. Conclusions: Patient factors, including sleep quality, perceptions of habitual sleep time, and current psychopathology, potentially influence sleep time estimation. Whereas psychological factors may lead to exaggeration of sleep disturbance among some patients, sleep quality itself may also influence the congruence between subjective and objective indices of sleep. Key Words: Sleep time estimation; sleep state misperception; insomnia; anxiety; sleep self report; total sleep time; sleep latency; sleep disorders, MMPI Abbreviations: D: depression; Hs: hypochondriasis; Hy: hysteria; Ma: hypomania; MMPI: Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; Mf: masculinity/femininity; Pa: paranoia; Pd: psychopathic deviate; PLMD: periodic limb movement disorder; PSG: polysomnography; Pt: psychasthenia; Sc: schizophrenia; Si: social introversion INTRODUCTION SUBJECTIVE JUDGMENTS ABOUT SLEEP LATENCY and total sleep time are of central importance to the diagnosis and treatment of insomnia and related sleep disorders. However, it is widely recognized that subjective recollections of sleep experience provide only a partial, and often inaccurate, portrayal of the nature and severity of objective sleep difficulties. Whereas many normal sleepers provide estimates of total sleep time and sleep latency that agree Accepted for publication July 1999 Correspondence to: Peter A. Vanable, Ph.D., who is now at Department of Psychology, 430 Huntington Hall, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York, 13244-2340. Electronic mail may be sent to pvanable@psych.syr.edu SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 1 with polysomnographic (PSG) measures, 1,2 insomnia patients often report greater sleep disruption than is evidenced by objective laboratory data. Indeed, as many as 10% to 25% of insomniacs meet diagnostic criteria for sleep state misperception, a classification assigned to individuals who complain of marked insomnia but show relatively normal sleep on PSG. 3,4,5 Other reports indicate that less extreme sleep misperceptions are common among insomnia patients generally, 6,7,8,9 leading some to argue that patients with sleep state misperception may simply represent the outer end of a normal sleep misperception continuum. 10 Yet, while several investigators have argued that sleep time underestimation and sleep latency overestimation is

common to many insomnia patients, 10,11,12 at least two studies suggest considerable variability in sleep time perception among sleep disorder patients. One recent report showed, for example, that while group-based averages indicate a general tendency for patients with complaints of insomnia to underestimate total sleep time, a substantial number of patients produced estimates of sleep time that were congruent with PSG-observed sleep time, and nearly 20% overestimated total sleep. 13 Carskadon and colleagues 6 reported similarly that, as a group, insomnia patients produced overestimates of total sleep time and underestimates of sleep latency, but also noted that nearly 20% of the sample produced estimates of total sleep time that were congruent with PSG readings and 12% of the sample produced substantial overestimates of total sleep time. Despite the obvious clinical importance of subjective perceptions of sleep disturbance, research has provided relatively little insight into patient factors potentially affecting sleep time perceptions. Laboratory work suggests that sleep time underestimation may be due in part to physiologic or perceptual deficits that limit insomniacs' capacity to differentiate between sleep and wakefulness. 14,15,16 Upon being awakened from PSG-defined sleep, poor sleepers are more likely than good sleepers to report that they were actually awake, 17,18,19 an effect attenuated by triazolam and zolpidem. 20,21 In addition, there is some evidence suggesting that alterations in perceptions of wakefulness vary as a function of time of night. In a laboratory study of patients with sleep state misperception, expected alterations in perceptions of wakefulness were observed for stage 2 sleep during the first sleep cycle, whereas when awakened during stage 2 of the third sleep cycle, patients were more likely to report that they were asleep after being awakened. 22 Finally, Edinger and Fins 13 found evidence suggesting that sleep time perception varies by sleep disorder and specific sleep self-report factors. That is, psychophysiological insomniacs tended to underestimate total sleep time whereas PLM patients tended to overestimate sleep time. Overestimation was also associated with a complaint of non-restorative sleep and the conviction that one's sleep difficulty is organically based. We hypothesized that psychological factors may also play a role in distorting the self-reported severity of sleep disturbance, since psychological distress tends to be associated with the magnification of somatic complaints in general. 23 The most common psychiatric syndromes (i.e., mood and anxiety disorders) might be exacerbated by, as well as contribute to, sleep deprivation, and psychiatric symptoms (such as fatigue, poor concentration, panic, and irritability) could readily be misattributed by patients to poor sleep. Psychologically distressed patients may furthermore enhance the severity of sleep disturbance reports, which could serve to elicit a more aggressive medical treatment of their sleep difficulties, psychological difficulties, SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 2 or both. Indirect support for this possibility is provided by data documenting elevated MMPI psychopathology and neuroticism among sleep state misperception patients. 11,24 There are also some data suggesting that patients suffering from insomnia associated with depression may be more prone to sleep time underestimation. 13,25 In the present study we have sought to characterize the extent to which a diagnostically diverse group of sleep disorder patients misperceive total sleep time and sleep latency. Furthermore, we aimed to identify diagnostic, polysomnographic, and psychological factors associated with the overestimation and underestimation of total sleep time and sleep latency. Our goal was to broaden the inquiry to include sleep-disordered patients representing a variety of diagnostic subgroups. METHODS Subjects Subjects were drawn from an initial pool of 592 consecutive patients who underwent diagnostic PSG following clinical referral to a university based sleep disorders center. Selection criteria consisted of: 1) a completed overnight polysomnography evaluation; 2) a completed MMPI; and 3) self-reported sleep initiation and/or sleep maintenance difficulties or a complaint of non-restorative sleep. Fourhundred twenty eight participants underwent an overnight PSG and had a completed MMPI, among whom 350 (82%) reported sleep initiation/maintenance difficulties or complained of non-restorative sleep. From this pool of eligible participants, we then selected patients meeting criteria for one of five sleep disorder diagnoses of primary interest to this investigation: psychophysiological insomnia (n=19); insomnia associated with depressive disorder (n=11); insomnia secondary to an Axis I psychiatric disorder other than depression (n=21); sleep state misperception (SSM, n=8); and periodic limb movement in sleep disorder (PLMD, n=24). Finally, to arrange for approximately equivalent cell sizes relative to other diagnostic subgroups, we randomly selected 21 of the 152 eligible obstructive sleep apnea patients (14%) who met inclusion criteria. Patients whose primary sleep disorder diagnosis was poor sleep hygiene (n=11), or central apnea (n=7) were excluded from the study, as were several patient groups with too few cases to analyze (e.g., insomnia secondary to a medical condition). The final study N was 104 (37% female; M age=45, SD=11.4). Men and women were equally represented across sleep diagnosis categories, χ 2 (5)=7.47, ns. There were no age differences across diagnostic groups, with one exception: patients with PLMD were older than other diagnostic groups (M=54.3 vs. 42.2), t (97)=-4.75, p<.001. With regard to medical comorbidity, 26% had hypertension, 14% had cardiac disease, 9% had pulmonary disease,

11% had neurological disease, and 54% had other medical diseases (primarily endocrine, hematologic, and gastrointestinal). Mean periodic limb movement arousal index for PLMD patients was 27.4 (SD=20.0). OSA patients had a mean apnea-hypopnea index of 74.4 (SD=35.2), a mean oxygen saturation of 92% (SD=4.3), and a mean minimum oxygen saturation of 80% (SD=6.5). Procedure During their initial visit, patients underwent an extensive interview and examination by a board certified psychiatrist and sleep physician (WBM). The clinical interview included screening for current or past psychiatric disturbance and for evidence of a conditioned element to the sleep disturbance. Patients also completed the MMPI 26 and the sleep habit questionnaire. 27 The MMPI consists of 550 true/false format items covering a wide variety of behaviors, thoughts, and attitudes, somatic difficulties, and emotions. Responses reduce to 10 empirically derived clinical scales with reasonably good test-retest reliability, adequate internal consistency, and meaningful correlations with behavior and scores on related psychological tests. Scale scores are represented as t-scores, with a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10, and are referenced against the responses of a large "Minnesota normal" sample; more detailed description of instrument development and psychometric properties is published elsewhere. 26 The Sleep Habit Questionnaire is a self-administered clinic form developed for research purposes. The questionnaire consists of 16 items assessing a range of sleep-related experiences, including the nature and duration of the presenting sleep complaint, usual bedtime on weeknights and weekends, past use of prescription and non-prescription hypnotics, and patient perceptions of their habitual (usual) sleep latency and total sleep time. Items from the sleep habit questionnaire used in this study were: patient estimates of usual total sleep time and sleep latency (e.g., "How many hours, on average, do you think you sleep?"), years of sleep complaint, and presenting sleep complaint. Patients underwent a one-night diagnostic 12-channel PSG following technical procedures described in detail by Mendelson, 5 consisting of: EEG (C3 - A2 and O1 - A2), right and left EOG, EMG (chin, anterior tibialis), airflow thermistors (nasal, oral), oximeter, thoracic and abdominal stretch gauges, and EKG. PSG data were scored according to the criteria published by Rechtschaffen and Kales 28 and Mendelson. 5 Total sleep time was scored as the sum of all 30-second epochs of either REM or non-rem (NREM) sleep. Sleep latency was scored as the duration between "lights out" and sleep onset, defined as the first three consecutive epochs of stage 1, or first epoch of a deeper NREM stage. Sleep disorders were diagnosed using the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD). 3 In determining the presence of psychiatric disorder, the interviewer applied DSM-IIIR criteria 29 to interview data, but was blind to MMPI results. Following overnight PSG, patients completed an additional brief questionnaire including items assessing perceived (estimated) total sleep time and sleep latency. Variable Computation and Data Analysis Overview Congruence between subjective estimates of total sleep time and sleep latency and PSG derived times was computed using a ratio score approach, as described by Edinger and colleagues 13 (also see Mendelson 21,22 ). Sleep time and sleep latency ratio scores were computed for each patient using the formulas: 1) subjective sleep ratio = (estimated sleep time/ PSG sleep time) x 100 2) subjective sleep latency ratio = (estimated sleep latency/psg sleep latency) x 100 Using this approach, a value of 100% indicates perfect congruence between the subjective (patient-based) estimate Table 1 Subjective estimates of total sleep time and sleep latency versus objective findings (complete sample). N Mean SD Range Total Sleep Time (in minutes) Estimate (patient) 104 311.9 a 128.0 1-599 Observed (PSG) 104 328.8 89.0 75-555 Subjective sleep ratio: % (estimated / observed) 104 96.0 37.1.3-234 Sleep Latency (minutes) Estimated (patient) 92 44.4 b 44.9.5-239 Observed (PSG) 92 21.7 31.2.5-161 Subjective sleep latency ratio: % (estimated / observed) 92 119.0 22.1 80-187 Sleep Habit Questionnaire Data Habitual total sleep time estimate 104 334.9 167.4 0-900 Habitual sleep latency estimate 97 28.3 45.6 0-300 a Subjective estimates of total sleep time did not differ significantly from observed total sleep time, t (103) = 1.56, p =.11. b Subjective estimates of sleep latency significantly differed from observed sleep latency, t (91) = -6.01, p <.0001). SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 3

and the observed (PSG) data, values <100% reflect relative underestimation, and values >100% reflect relative overestimation. Because both the estimated and observed sleep latency distributions were positively skewed, both variables were transformed to their logarithm value using the formula Log 10 (x+10) prior to computation of subjective sleep latency ratio scores, to reduce the influence of extreme outliers. 30 This transformation resulted in substantial improvement in the normality of the distribution (skewness=.74, kurtosis=.78). By combing subjective estimates and PSG-based sleep times into a single measurement unit, ratio scores can usefully characterize the distribution of sleep time over- and under-estimations. Moreover, ratio scores can readily be analyzed to identify patient variables of relevance to sleep time misestimation. 13 However, because ratio scores do not indicate the absolute magnitude of differences between subjective and objective indices (i.e., the number of minutes by which estimates diverge from PSG observed times), we also report some descriptive data concerning the range of sleep time misestimations, expressed in minutes. Primary study hypotheses are tested using ratio scores. To facilitate the interpretation of group effects for several analyses, participants were also categorized into one of three subjective sleep ratio groups, based on equal cutpoints in the distribution. Subjective sleep ratio scores were normally distributed around the 100% midpoint; patients were grouped according to their subjective sleep ratio scores as follows: those with scores below 86% were placed in the "low" subjective sleep ratio group (relative underestimators of sleep time), those with scores between 86% and 112% were placed in the "middle" sleep ratio Number of Patients 20 15 10 5 0 0.0 (100%) Underestimates Accurate Overestimates 20.0 40.0 60.0 80.0 120.0 160.0 200.0 100.0 140.0 180.0 220.0 Std. Dev = 37.12 Mean = 96.0 N = 104.00 Figure 1 Distribution of subjective sleep ratio scores (N = 104). Subjective sleep ratio scores= (estimated sleep time / observed sleep time) 100 SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 4 group (relatively accurate estimators of sleep time), and those with scores above 112% were categorized in the "high" sleep ratio group (relative overestimators of sleep time). A similar grouping scheme was applied to sleep latency ratio scores, with the following tertile-based cutpoints: "low" (<110%), "middle" (110% to 127%), and "high" (>127%). Statistical analyses for continuous outcome measures and categorical independent variables were performed using independent and paired sample t-tests and one way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Analyses involving continuous predictor and outcome measures are reported using the Pearson R correlation. Our sample included relatively few patients diagnosed with SSM (n=8) and insomnia with a depressive disorder (n=11), relative to other diagnostic categories. Such cell size differences potentially violate assumptions of homogeneity of variance for comparisons involving group means, and could restrict our ability to detect group differences. 31 We therefore assessed for heterogeneity of within-cell variance for comparisons of subjective sleep and sleep latency ratio data among each sleep disorder group using the Levene test, a robust diagnostic procedure developed to detect unequal variances. 31 Results showed no differences in variance across diagnostic categories, both for subjective sleep ratio scores, F (5, 98) =.87, ns and subjective sleep latency ratio scores, F (5, 86) =.93, ns, indicating that the homogeneity of variance assumption was not violated for the two analyses involving mean comparisons of subjective sleep ratio data by sleep disorder diagnosis. RESULTS Sleep Time Estimation Across the Entire Sample Table 1 lists descriptive data for key PSG and self-report measures. Estimated total sleep time was moderately correlated with observed total sleep time (r=.56, p<.0001) and, similarly, estimated sleep latency was moderately correlated with observed sleep latency (r=.59, p<.0001). Paired t- tests showed, however, that mean estimated sleep latency was significantly higher than observed sleep latency, (M's= 44.4 vs. 21.5 minutes), t (91)=-6.01, p <.0001. In contrast, estimated total sleep time did not differ from observed total sleep time, t (103)=-1.56, p =.11, although examination of the raw means suggests a possible trend for patients to underestimate total sleep time (see Table 1). The distributions for subjective sleep ratio and subjective sleep latency ratio scores are shown in Figures 1 and 2. As Figure 1 illustrates, congruence between sleep time estimates and observed total sleep time varied widely across the sample (M=96.0%, SD=37.1). The median subjective sleep ratio score was 100.3, indicating that many participants provided accurate sleep time estimates, and a roughly equal proportion of patients overestimated and

underestimated total sleep time. Expressed in minutes, findings showed that 24% of the sample provided estimates that were within ± 30 minutes of the PSG-observed total sleep time. In addition, while a full 30% of patients underestimated their sleep time by 60 minutes or greater, a similar percentage (26%) overestimated total sleep time by more than 60 minutes. Whereas subjective sleep ratio scores were normally distributed around the 100% mark, both mean and median subjective sleep latency ratio scores fell well above 100% (M=119.0, SD=22.1, Median= 117.3), with the majority of patients overestimating sleep latency (see Figure 2). Expressed in minutes, nearly half of the sample (49%) overestimated sleep latency by 15 minutes or greater, whereas only 8% underestimated sleep latency times by 15 minutes or more. A significant minority of patients (22%) provided estimates of sleep latency that were within ± 5 minutes of PSG-observed sleep latency duration. Sleep Time Estimation by ICSD Diagnosis Table 2 lists subjective sleep ratio and subjective sleep latency ratio data, stratified by sleep disorder diagnosis. Subjective sleep ratios varied by diagnostic group, F (5, 98) =3.99, p<.005. Planned comparisons showed that psychophysiological insomniacs overestimated total sleep time relative to the other groups combined, t (102)=-2.84, p<.01. Expressed in minutes, the average difference between subjective estimates of sleep time and PSG determined total sleep time was +33 minutes (SD=73.8) among patients with psychophysiological insomnia, as compared to -28 minutes (SD = 111.4) among the other diagnostic groups combined. Only sleep state misperception patients tended to underestimate total sleep time relative to all other Number of Patients 20 15 10 5 0 80.0 (100%) Accurate Overestimate Extreme overestimate 90.0 100.0 110.0 120.0 130.0 140.0 SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 5 150.0 160.0 170.0 180.0 Std. Dev = 22.14 Mean = 119.0 N = 92.00 Figure 2 Distribution of subjective sleep latency ratio scores (n = 92). Subjective sleep latency scores = (estimated sleep latency / observed sleep latency) 100 diagnostic groups, t (102)=3.53, p<.005, a finding that was expected based on the criteria used to assign patients to this diagnostic group. Subjective sleep latency ratio scores did not differ across ICSD diagnostic groups, F (5, 86) =.75, ns. Sleep Parameter and Patient History Influences on Sleep Time Perceptions Next, we examined the relationship between several sleep parameter measures and sleep time perception. Using continuous outcome measures, subjective sleep ratio and sleep latency ratios were not correlated with sleep efficiency, wake time after sleep onset, latency of REM onset, or time in each sleep stage. However, when PSG parameters were analyzed using three level groupings of sleep ratio data (i.e., low, middle, and high), several noteworthy differences emerged. In particular, wake time after sleep onset, F (2, 99) = 4.24, p<.02, and sleep efficiency, F (2, 99)=5.50, p<.005, varied by subjective sleep ratio grouping. Planned comparisons revealed a non-linear relationship between wake time after sleep onset and subjective sleep ratios, indicating that patients in the "middle" subjective sleep ratio group (those producing "accurate" estimations of total sleep time) had shorter durations of wakefulness after sleep onset (M=51.9, SD=44.0) as compared to patients in the sleep time overestimation group (M=92.4, SD=60.5; t (65)=-3.15, p<.001), and (at trend level) the sleep time underestimation group (M=74.1, SD=64.6; t (68)=1.68, p<.10). Similarly, patients in the "middle" group whose sleep time estimates were relatively congruent with PSG measures had higher sleep efficiency (M= 82.2, SD=14.2) as compared to the combined means for sleep time underestimators (M=77.6, SD=17.5) and sleep time overestimators (M=68.9, SD=18.2), t (102)=2.38, p<.02. We also examined the relationship between habitual sleep time estimation (i.e., average time slept at home) and sleep estimation ratio scores during PSG monitoring. Habitual total sleep time estimation was positively correlated with subjective sleep ratio scores (r=.38, p <.0001) and negatively correlated with subjective sleep latency ratio scores (r=-.31, p<.005), suggesting that as reported habitual sleep time decreased (i.e., fewer average hours slept at home), patients were more likely to produce underestimates of sleep time and overestimates of sleep latency in the lab during the overnight study. These findings were further characterized by examining mean habitual sleep time for low, middle, and high sleep ratio groupings. There were clear differences in reported habitual sleep time across both subjective sleep ratio, F (2, 99)=8.04, p<.005, and subjective sleep latency subgroups, F (2, 89) = 4.01, p<.05. Planned comparisons showed that habitual sleep times reported by patients who underestimated total sleep time in the lab were significantly lower than were the habitual

times provided by sleep time overestimators, (M's = 260.6 vs. 415.3), t(65)=-3.87, p<.0001. Similarly, habitual sleep times reported by patients grouped in the high subjective sleep latency ratio group (i.e., those producing large overestimates of sleep latency in the lab) were significantly lower than habitual sleep times reported by patients in the low subjective sleep latency ratio group (M's=276.6 vs. 386.0), t (60)=2.46, p<.05. In terms of demographic characteristics, men had significantly higher subjective sleep latency ratio scores relative to women, indicating a greater tendency among men to overestimate the time it took them to get to sleep (M's= 123.6 vs. 109.5 minutes), t(90)=2.99, p<.005. Subjective sleep ratio and sleep latency ratios were not related to age, years of sleep complaint, or presenting sleep complaint (all p's>.30). Sleep Time Estimation as a function of MMPI Scores and Psychiatric History To test the hypothesis that psychological factors would relate to sleep time misperceptions, we compared MMPI subscale scores across the three subjective sleep ratio and sleep latency ratio subgroups. Our prediction was that patients reporting significantly greater sleep disturbance than found via objective measures would have higher MMPI elevations relative to patients whose subjective reports of sleep time were more congruent with PSG-based times. An analysis of variance showed that Psychasthenia (Pt) scores differed as a function of subjective sleep ratio subgroup, F (2, 99)=3.24, p<.05. Planned comparisons showed that sleep time underestimators had higher scores on the Pt scale relative to patients in the middle and high subjective sleep ratio groups combined (M s=67.7 vs. 81.9), t (102)=2.29, p<.03). Subjective sleep ratio subgroups did not differ on any additional MMPI subscales and there were no MMPI differences across the three subjective sleep latency ratio subgroups. Likewise, patients with current and/or past history of either depressive disorder (n=28) or other psychiatric disorders (n=41) did not differ from the remainder of the sample in the degree of sleep time or sleep latency misestimation. We also tested the hypothesis that psychological factors would be more predictive of sleep time misperception for patients with a current or past history of depressive disorder or other psychiatric illness, as compared to patients with no psychiatric history. Contrary to expectations, when the sample was limited first to patients with a history of depression, and second, to only those with a history of psychiatric problems other than depression, no MMPI clinical scale scores differed by either subjective sleep or sleep latency ratio group. However, several MMPI associations were detected among patients without current or past treatment for depression (n=76). As can be seen in Table 3, among patients without current or past depression, total sleep time underestimators had more overall MMPI elevations and higher Hypochondriasis (Hs), Hysteria (Hy), and Pt scores (all p's<.05). Sleep time understimators also demonstrated a possible trend towards higher Depression (D) and Psychopathic deviate (Pd) scores (p's<.06). DISCUSSION This study investigated discrepancies between patients' subjective perceptions of sleep time and sleep latency and PSG-based measures, testing the potential roles played by several diagnostic, sleep history, and psychological factors in influencing these judgments. In a diagnostically diverse sample, we confirmed and extended prior research 13 showing wide variability in the degree of congruence between subjective sleep time estimation and PSG derived sleep time. Whereas earlier studies indicated a clear tendency for insomnia patients to underestimate total sleep time and overestimate sleep latency, 6,7,8 patients in this diverse clinical sample were in fact just as likely to overestimate as to underestimate total sleep time, but showed a tendency to Table 2 Total sleep time and sleep latency estimation accuracy by ICSD diagnosis. Subjective Sleep Ratio Subjective Sleep Latency Ratio ICSD diagnosis n M SD n M SD Sleep state misperception 8 53.7 a 23.6 7 131.8 19.8 Psychophysiological insomnia 19 117.1 b 35.5 17 119.6 21.7 Insomnia with psychiatric disorder, not depression 21 90.2 40.4 19 115.0 24.8 Insomnia with depressive disorder 11 92.3 43.5 8 119.0 27.3 Obstructive sleep apnea 21 100.1 24.0 20 115.4 14.7 PLMD 23 96.4 35.1 21 121.5 25.0 Note. Subjective sleep ratio scores varied across diagnostic subgroups, F (103) = 3.98, p <.005. a Sleep state misperception patients significantly underestimated total sleep time relative to combined subjective sleep ratio averages across the other patient subgroups, t (102) = 3.53, p <.01. b Psychophysiological insomniacs overestimated total sleep time relative to the combined subjective sleep ratio average across the other patient subgroups, t (102)= -2.84, p <.01. SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 6

overestimate sleep latency. Indeed, both sleep latency and total sleep time estimation were moderately correlated with PSG-determined time, suggesting that the estimation of sleep quality provided by this diverse sample of sleep disorder patients provided a reasonably accurate, albeit incomplete, portrayal of the nature and severity of their sleep difficulties. Several interesting findings emerged with regard to the relation between PSG-derived sleep parameters, patient sleep history, and subjective perceptions of sleep time. Patients whose total sleep time estimates were congruent with PSG-observed total sleep time experienced fewer minutes of wake time after initial sleep onset and demonstrated better sleep efficiency. Although prior research suggests that sleep time underestimation may be related to frequent nocturnal arousals, 6 patients who experienced more prolonged periods of wakefulness did not uniformly underestimate their sleep time, as might be expected. Rather, there was a trend for patients in both the sleep time underestimation and overestimation group to experience longer periods of wakefulness and poorer sleep efficiency, relative to those whose estimates were congruent with PSGobserved times. One hypothesis which might be tested in further studies would be that longer periods of wakefulness may simply limit patients' capacity to gauge the passing of time, resulting in either over- or underestimates of total sleep time. Estimates of habitual sleep time and sleep latency at home were related to sleep time perceptions in a more linear fashion. That is, those who underestimated total sleep time during overnight PSG reported sleeping fewer hours at home compared to patients whose estimates were consistent with PSG-observed sleep time or who overestimated sleep time in the lab in all diagnostic groups. Similarly, patients who produced overestimates of sleep latency in the lab also reported sleeping the fewest hours at home. These findings suggest that patients who present with the most severe subjective sleep complaints during an initial clinical interview are also more likely to misperceive, or possibly exaggerate, the extent to which they struggle to initiate and maintain sleep. Taken together, these findings underscore the possibility that, in addition to standard pharmacological and behavioral treatments, patients with relatively severe insomnia complaints may benefit from additional intervention aimed at improving discrimination between sleep and wakeful states. 32 The finding that psychophysiological insomnia patients produced modest overestimates of total sleep time relative to other diagnostic subgroups was unexpected, especially given other reports in the literature. 13 Given the conditioned element of their sleep disturbance, it may be that psychophysiological insomnia patients experienced some unexpected improvement in their sleep during their overnight stay in the lab. Consequently, a portion of these patients may have awakened with the perception that they had slept even longer than indicated via PSG-measured sleep. However, this finding should be interpreted with some caution, since the magnitude of sleep time overestimation among psychophysiological insomniacs in this sample was relatively small. Apart from clear and substantial total sleep time underestimation among patients with sleep state misperception, we found no other differences in the accuracy of sleep time or sleep latency estimation across other sleep disorder diagnoses. Additional research is needed to clarify the extent to which etiologic factors underlying different sleep disorders contribute to sleep time Table 3 Mean MMPI Scores by subjective sleep ratio subgroup. 1 Subjective Sleep Ratio Group MMPI variable Low Middle High (Underestimators) (Overestimators) F p Total elevations b 2.70 1.4 1.4 4.03.02 Hs b 71.2 64.7 61.6 5.20.008 D 71.4 66.0 63.3 3.10.051 Hy 70.2 66.6 66.1 4.55.014 Pd 65.4 59.3 58.5 3.07.053 Mf 59.1 54.9 52.7 1.95 ns Pa 58.2 54.6 54.0 1.32 ns Pt 65.6 59.3 57.4 4.82.01 Sc 62.3 57.0 58.9 2.03 ns Ma 55.8 50.8 55.4 1.67 ns Si 54.1 52.9 52.6.176 ns a Analysis excludes patients with a history of depression, resulting in n = 74. b Mean MMPI score for the low subjective sleep ratio group was significantly higher than the mean MMPI score for middle and high subjective sleep ratio groups combined, all t's >2.3, p's <.03. SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 7

misperception. While recent research emphasizes the potential role of physiological and cognitive factors in influencing perceptions of sleep time, 15,16 findings from our study suggest that psychological factors also may influence patients' subjective reports of sleep quality. Consistent with the hypothesis that psychologically distressed patients would be more prone to overestimates of sleep disturbance, we found that the MMPI scale Pt (Scale 7, or "Psychasthenia") was positively associated with sleep time underestimation. This finding suggests that cognitive rumination, physiological symptoms of tension/anxiety, and catastrophic expectations about the impact of poor sleep may play an important role in the exaggeration of sleep disturbance self-reports. This hypothesis is certainly well represented in clinical observation, and raises the possibility that behavioral approaches aimed at improving anxiety management skills might improve the accuracy of subjective reports of sleep. This finding also raises the possibility that self-reported sleep improvement (e.g., following behavioral 33 or pharmacological insomnia treatment) might be partially accounted for by alterations in perceived sleep quality and duration. We also found preliminary evidence suggesting that personality variables have a greater impact on sleep misperceptions among non-depressed patients relative to patients with a current or past history of depression. When the sample was limited to those patients with no history of depression, sleep time underestimators had more clinical elevations and scored higher on the HS, HY, and PT subscales of the MMPI relative to patients who were either accurate or provided overestimations of total sleep time. Although requiring replication, these initial findings suggest that frequent somatic complaints, rumination, and a preference for medically-oriented explanations of psychological distress may be most strongly associated with exaggerated estimates of sleep disturbance among patients without comorbid depressive symptomatology that has required treatment. Important study limitations include the reliance upon a single-night sleep study, which may be associated with a first-night effect and other alterations in sleep architecture. 34 Second, although the MMPI is widely used and well validated as a psychopathology measure, MMPI responses, particularly those involving somatic complaints, may be influenced by the presence of a medical disorder. In this respect, comorbid medical diseases (e.g., hypertension, cardiac disease) were relatively common in our sample and may have had some influence on MMPI responding. Finally, while our utilization of ratio scores provided a useful means of characterizing subjective versus objective indices of sleep, ratio scores provide less information about the absolute magnitude of differences between subjective and objective measures, and could therefore lead to an attenuation of between group differences. Although not a SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 8 focus in the present study, it would be worthwhile in future studies to more fully explore the relative strengths and weaknesses of different methodological approaches to characterizing the congruence between sleep time estimates and PSG-observed sleep times. In most clinical settings, it is the nature and severity of patients' subjective complaints of sleep disturbance, rather than PSG-derived indices, that guide treatment decisions. As such, this study represents an initial attempt to identify factors that lead some patients to perceive greater sleep disturbance than is indicated by PSG. Whereas psychological factors may lead to over reporting or even exaggeration of sleep disturbance among a subset of patients, finding also suggest that sleep quality itself may influence the degree of congruence between subjective and objective indices of sleep time. Thus, for patients with pre-existing or newly emerging psychopathology, self-reports of sleep disturbance may be indicative of more global distress or interpersonal difficulties. For others (e.g., those with relatively severe sleep impairment), sleep misperception may be related in part to the inherent difficulties associated with estimating sleep time following a night of fitful sleep. Finally, while our results confirm that a majority of patients presenting with insomnia overestimate how long it takes for them to fall asleep, findings also caution against the routine assumption that patients judgements of total sleep duration represent an overestimation of the severity of their sleep disturbance, since many patients in this sample were reasonably accurate in estimating how long they slept. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was partially supported by NIH grant 1KO7HL03640 to Wallace B. Mendelson, M.D. REFERENCES 1. Kales A, Kales JD. Evaluation and treatment of insomnia. New York: Oxford, 1984. 2. Baekeland F, Hoy P. Reported vs recorded sleep characteristics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1971;24:548-51. 3. Diagnostic Classification Steering Committee, Thorpy MJ, chairman. International classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic and coding manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association, 1990. 4. Coleman RM, Roffwarg HP, Kennedy SJ, et al. Sleep-wake disorders based on a polysomnographic diagnosis. A national cooperative study. JAMA 1982;247:997-1003. 5. Mendelson WB. Human sleep: research and clinical care. New York: Plenum Medical Book Co/Plenum Press, 1987. 6. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Guilleminault C, Zarcone VP, Spiegal R. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drugfree subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiatry 1976;133:1382-88. 7. Frankel BL, Coursey RD, Buchbinder, R Snyder F. Recorded and reported sleep in chronic primary insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33:615-623. 8. Bixler EO, Kales A, Leo LA, Slyle, T. A comparison of subjective estimates and objective sleep laboratory findings in insomniac patients. Sleep Research 1983;2:143.

9. Chervin RD, Guilleminault C. Overestimation of sleep latency by patients with suspected hypersomnolence. Sleep 1996;2: 94-100. 10. Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, Buysee DJ, Coble PA, Yeager, A Subtyping of DSM-IIIR primary insomnia: a literature review by the DSM-IV work group on sleep disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:432-438. 11. Salin-Pascual RJ, Roehrs TA, Merlotti LA, Zorick F, Roth T. Longterm study of the sleep of insomnia patients with sleep state misperception and other insomnia patients. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149:904-8. 12. Trinder J. Subjective insomnia without objective findings: a pseudo diagnostic classification? Psychological Bulletin 1988; 103:87-94. 13. Edinger JD, Fins A. The distribution and clinical significance of sleep time misperceptions among insomniacs. Sleep 1995;18:232-239. 14. Mendelson WB. Insomnia and related sleep disorders. Psychiatric Clin of North America 1993;4:841-851. 15. Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, Bootzin RR, Wyatt JK. Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioral model and a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Research 1997;3:179-188. 16. Bonnet MH, Arand, DL. Hyperarousal and insomnia. Sleep Medicine Reviews 1997;2: 97-108. 17. Rechtschaffen A. Polygraphic aspects of insomnia. In Gastaut H, Lugaresi E, Berti-Ceroni G; eds. The abnormalities of sleep in man. Bologna, Gaggi, 1968:109-125. 18. Coates TJ, Killen JD, Silverman S, George J, Marchini E; Hamilton S, Thoresen CE. Cognitive activity, sleep disturbance, and stage specific differences between recorded and reported sleep. Psychophysiology 1983;20:243-50. 19. Mendelson WB, James SP, Garnett D, Sack DA, Rosenthal NE. A psychophysiological study of insomnia. Psychiatry Research 1986; 19:267-84. 20. Mendelson WB. Pharmacologic alteration of the perception of being awake or asleep. Sleep 1993;16:641-6. 21. Mendelson WB. Effects of flurazepam and zolpidem on the perception of sleep in insomniacs. Sleep 1995;18:92-6. 22. Mendelson WB. Effects of time of night and sleep stage on perception of sleep in subjects with sleep state misperception. Psychobiology 1998;26:73-8. 23. Aikens JE, Wagner LI, Saelinger LJ. Reduced medical utilization and costs in anxious and depressed primary care patients receiving behavioral interventions. Advances in Medical Psychotherapy and Psychodiagnosis 1999;10:169-178. 24. Bonnet M H Arand DL Physiological activation in patients with sleep state misperception. Psychosomatic Medicine 1997;59:533-540. 25. Bliwise DL Friedman LY, Jerome A. Depression as a confounding variable in the estimation of habitual sleep time. J Clin Psychol 49; 1993:471-477. 26. Hathaway SR, McKinley JC. A multiphasic personality schedule (Minnesota): I. Construction of the schedule. J Psychol 1940;10:249-254. 27. Mendelson WB, Garnett D, Gillin JC, Weingartner H. The experience of insomnia and daytime and nighttime functioning. Psychiatry Research, 1984;12:235-250. 28. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, 1968:1-60. 29. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders:third edition., Revised. Washington, DC:American Psychiatric Association, 1987. 30. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. Harper Collins: New York, 1989. 31. Stevens J. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Laurence Earlbaum and Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1986. 32. Downey R 3d, Bonnet MH. Training subjective insomniacs to accurately perceive sleep onset. Sleep 1992;15:58-63. 33. Lacks P, Bertelson AD, Gans L, Kunkel J. The effectiveness of three behavioral treatments for different degrees of sleep onset insomnia Beh Ther 1983;14:593-605. 34. Toussaint M Luthringer R, Schaltenbrand N, Carelli G, Lainey E, Jacqmin A, Muzet A, Macher JP. First-night effect in normal subjects and psychiatric inpatients. Sleep 1995;18:463-9. SLEEP, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2000 9