The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study



Similar documents
Published online: 17 Jun 2010.

Online publication date: 19 May 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Daring Greatly: How the Courage to Be Vulnerable Transforms the Way We Live, Love, Parent, and Lead. Click for updates

NASPE Sets the Standard

California Published online: 09 Jun 2014.

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Using Learning from Work for Progression to Higher Education: a degree of experience

Rens van de Schoot a b, Peter Lugtig a & Joop Hox a a Department of Methods and Statistics, Utrecht

Beijing, China b CMOE Key Laboratory of Petroleum Engineering in China University

Online publication date: 15 March 2010

How To Understand The History Of Part Time Business Studies

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Business Security Architecture: Weaving Information Security into Your Organization's Enterprise Architecture through SABSA

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Online publication date: 20 November 2009

L. Jay Thornton a & Sharon Johnson b a Department of Kinesiology and Health Science,

Online publication date: 11 October 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Purchase, New York, USA Published online: 27 Mar Full terms and conditions of use:

Full terms and conditions of use:

Nilpotent Lie and Leibniz Algebras

Andrew J. Finch a & Holly L. Karakos a a Peabody College of Vanderbilt University. Published online: 14 Apr 2014.

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Parental Relationships and Teenage Substance Abuse - An Examination

Alan J. Hawkins a, Brian J. Willoughby a & William J. Doherty b a School of Family Life, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Blaming the Victim in the Case of Rape

The term Broadway Pet Stores refers we to the owner of the website whose registered office is 6-8 Muswell Hill Broadway, London, N10 3RT.

Shih-Hua Chang a & Christine Suniti Bhat b a Department of Educational Psychology and Counselling, National

The use of this website is subject to the following terms of use:

Rafael Leal-Arcas a a Centre for Commercial Law Studies (CCLS), Queen Mary,

Published online: 17 Feb 2007.

Online Business Terms and Conditions - A Brief Glossary

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

CONSUMER CREDIT DEFAULT RATES DECREASE IN SEPTEMBER 2015 ACCORDING TO THE S&P/EXPERIAN CONSUMER CREDIT DEFAULT INDICES

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Terms & Conditions. In this section you can find: - Website usage terms and conditions 1, 2, 3. - Website disclaimer

National Credit Default Rates Continued Their Upward Trend in December 2014 According to the S&P/Experian Consumer Credit Default Indices

Rakesh N. Veedu a, Birte Vester b & Jesper Wengel a a Nucleic Acid Center, Department of Physics and Chemistry, Southern Denmark, Odense M, Denmark

2013 Distribution Summary Investor, Premium & e -Series Breakdown of Cumulative Distributions for the Period January 1, 2013 to December 31, 2013

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Journal of Marketing Thought Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Online Banking Agreement and Disclosures

Website Disclaimer Disclaimer 1

Rethinking Schools Limited Institutional Site License

Nordic Institute for Studies in Innovation, Research and Evaluation, Online publication date: 10 February 2011

Online publication date: 21 October 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Long Island IVF Terms and Conditions of Use

The Scientific Literature page 1. The Scientific Literature In Psychology

Legal Notices. Purpose and Scope of Website. StanCorp Financial Group, Inc. Contact Us. Public Affairs. Special Investigations Unit

Tuition Online Banking Agreement and Disclosure

You are authorised to view and download one copy to a local hard drive or disk, print and make copies of such printouts, provided that:

Terms and Conditions. Terms & Conditions. 1. Definitions. 2. Use of the website. 3. Privacy. 4. Purchase of products & gift vouchers

TaxSaverNetwork. Terms of Service

Philosophy Program, La Trobe University Bundoora, VIC, Australia b Computer Science Department, Washington and Lee University Lexington, USA, VA

C. System Requirements. Apple Software is supported only on Apple-branded hardware that meets specified system requirements as indicated by Apple.

Published online: 30 Jul 2014.

Sample Customer Reports

Trinity Online Application - Terms and Conditions of Use

Robin E. Jensen a, Katheryn Christy b, Patricia E. Gettings c & Louise Lareau c a University of Utah

The Psychology Lab Checklist By: Danielle Sclafani 08 and Stephanie Anglin 10; Updated by: Rebecca Behrens 11

Shelly Wimpfheimer DSW a b a Youth and Family Services, YMCA of Greater New. To link to this article:

QE, Credit Markets and Bubbles

To link to this article:

Private Tutoring and Social Cohesion Stephen P. Heyneman a a

Cameron M. Weber a a New School for Social Research, New York, USA. Available online: 25 Oct 2011

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT

Online publication date: 19 April 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Exploring Hospitality Program Rankings

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Insuring your business

Terms of Use (basic) 1

Skills Route Limited. Terms and Conditions of Use

Carolyn Smith-Morris a & Lenore Manderson b c a Department of Anthropology, Southern Methodist

To link to this article:

These TERMS AND CONDICTIONS (this Agreement ) are agreed to between InfluencersAtWork,

Agency Pack for Sales Partners

Terms and Conditions. Introduction

TERMS AND CONDITIONS USE OF SERVICES

Protective Indexed Annuity II

VATSIM USER AGREEMENT

AMERICAN PHYSICAL SOCIETY Institution Single-Site User License

IMPORTANT IT IS DEAMED THAT YOU HAVE READ AND AGREE TO ALL TERMS & CONDITIONS BEFORE USING THIS WEBSITE.

UK Vendor Website - Terms of Use Agreement

PESKY GNATS CLINICIAN LICENSING AGREEMENT

Data Mining for Web Site Evaluation Janaki Santhiveeran a a

ELECTRONIC RECORDS DISCLOSURE AND AGREEMENT READ AND SCROLL DOWN PLEASE READ THIS AGREEMENT CAREFULLY AND KEEP A COPY FOR YOUR RECORDS.

Available online: 18 Aug To link to this article:

Paychex Accounting Online Terms of Use

Starting a Commodity Pool

Dental Care Professionals Pay Findings from the Dental Business Trends survey

Cobalt Benchmark Report

Service Agreement: January 2008

Cash In On Mobile Marketing And Reach Out To New Customers

Managing FX Risk when trading with Australia. Mark Coulam Senior Manager, Treasury Solutions.

Age and Health Insurance: Pricing Out the Decades of Adult Life Looking at the difference in health insurance premiums between ages 20 and 60.

The London School of Architecture Website Terms & Conditions

Application. ACHE Higher Education Network

Progress Report: Integrating Enterprise Risk Management Analysis Into Corporate Credit Ratings

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [65.186.78.206] On: 10 April 2014, At: 17:16 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Journal of Social Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20 The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study John E. Edlund a, Austin Lee Nichols b, Bradley M. Okdie c, Rosanna E. Guadagno d, Cassie A. Eno e, Jeremy D. Heider f, Edward J. Hansen g, Brad J. Sagarin g, Ginette Blackhart h, Catherine A. Cottrell i & Kenneth Tyler Wilcox a a Rochester Institute of Technology b University of Navarra c The Ohio State University at Newark d National Science Foundation e Bellevue University f Southeast Missouri State University g Northern Illinois University h East Tennessee State University i New College of Florida Accepted author version posted online: 13 Dec 2013.Published online: 08 Apr 2014. To cite this article: John E. Edlund, Austin Lee Nichols, Bradley M. Okdie, Rosanna E. Guadagno, Cassie A. Eno, Jeremy D. Heider, Edward J. Hansen, Brad J. Sagarin, Ginette Blackhart, Catherine A. Cottrell & Kenneth Tyler Wilcox (2014) The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study, The Journal of Social Psychology, 154:3, 181-185, DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.872596 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.872596 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or

howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/termsand-conditions

The Journal of Social Psychology, 154: 181 185, 2014 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0022-4545 print / 1940-1183 online DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.872596 REPLICATIONS AND REFINEMENTS The Prevalence and Prevention of Crosstalk: A Multi-Institutional Study JOHN E. EDLUND Rochester Institute of Technology AUSTIN LEE NICHOLS University of Navarra BRADLEY M. OKDIE The Ohio State University at Newark ROSANNA E. GUADAGNO National Science Foundation CASSIE A. ENO Bellevue University JEREMY D. HEIDER Southeast Missouri State University EDWARD J. HANSEN BRAD J. SAGARIN Northern Illinois University GINETTE BLACKHART East Tennessee State University Address correspondence to John E. Edlund, Rochester Institute of Technology, Department of Psychology, 18 Lomb Memorial Dr., Rochester, NY 14623, USA. E-mail: john.edlund@rit.edu

182 THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CATHERINE A. COTTRELL New College of Florida KENNETH TYLER WILCOX Rochester Institute of Technology ABSTRACT. It is a common problem in psychology subject pools for past study participants to inform future participants of key experimental details (also known as crosstalk). Previous research (Edlund, Sagarin, Skowronski, Johnson, & Kutter, 2009) demonstrated that a combined classroom and laboratory treatment could significantly reduce crosstalk. The present investigation tested a laboratory-only treatment for the prevention of crosstalk at five universities, along with institutional-level moderators of crosstalk. Results indicated the presence of crosstalk at all universities and that the laboratory-based treatment was effective in reducing crosstalk. Importantly, crosstalk rates were higher (but successfully neutralized) in research pools with higher research credit requirements. Therefore, this research provides valuable guidance regarding crosstalk prevalence and its minimization by researchers. Keywords: crosstalk, data integrity, participant pools, research methods, social influence NON-NAÏVE RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS ARE a significant problem for psychological researchers (Aronson, Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Gonzales, 1990). A non-naïve participant is a participant who (through any number of means) knows critical information related to a particular study. Importantly, non-naïve participants may behave differently from naïve participants (Nichols & Maner, 2008). The primary reasons why research participants lose their naïveté to a particular research study are that they decipher the hypothesis of the researcher through the research protocol (Aronson et al., 1990) or arrive with foreknowledge of the research. Participant crosstalk is problematic and occurs when previous participants reveal key experimental information to future participants (Edlund, Sagarin, Skowronski, Johnson, & Kutter, 2009). Edlund and colleagues (2009) investigated two techniques for reducing crosstalk (a classroom based technique and a combined classroom and laboratory-only technique). In combination, these two techniques successfully decreased detectable crosstalk from 3% to 0.004%. The current study tested a laboratory-only treatment for preventing crosstalk at five different universities. We predicted the detection of crosstalk at every university, and that a laboratory-only treatment would significantly reduce the prevalence of crosstalk across the different universities. Additionally, we examined contextual variables across the universities to explain differences in crosstalk prevalence and treatment effectiveness. METHOD Participants A total of 3,607 students participated across five universities.

EDLUND ET AL. 183 Materials A pretest determined the normal range of guesses for the number of gumballs in a glass jar. Using this knowledge, we artificially set the reported number of gumballs in the jar to a non-common guessing point (i.e., not a multiple of 100), within one standard deviation of the average guess during the pretest, and at a value that was different from the actual number of gumballs in the jar. Procedure At the conclusion of unrelated experiments, participants were given the opportunity to guess the number of gumballs in a glass jar. Following the methodology laid out in Edlund and colleagues study (2009), we tested an experimental condition where only a laboratory-based treatment was used (p. 639). In the experimental condition experimenters said: Thanks for trying. I would like to ask that you not tell anyone about this experiment to help keep guesses normal. Is that okay with you? After receiving verbal agreement the experimenters dismissed the participants from the laboratory. In the control condition, no verbal request was made (p. 637). Additionally, we assessed how onerous the requirement was for the research alternative (number of papers) and the length of time required to complete the research participation requirement. 1 RESULTS First, we examined the effectiveness of our treatment by collapsing across all universities and did not find an overall effect of the laboratory-only treatment of crosstalk, χ 2 (1, N = 3607) = 1.40, p Fisher s exact =.16 (see Table 1 for a detailed breakdown of the crosstalk prevalence rates by condition). Further, condition order did not impact the results b =.63, S.E. b =.46, Wald (1 df ) = 2.11, p >.10. An inspection of the means suggested that the baseline rate of crosstalk varied by institution. Therefore, we explored potential moderators of crosstalk by entering each moderator into separate logistic regression analyses. The nature of the alternative requirements (number of papers) interacted with experimental condition to predict the prevalence of crosstalk, such that higher TABLE 1 Rates of Crosstalk Across Conditions Condition University Control Experimental East Tennessee State University 3/146 (2.0%) 3/83 (3.6%) Northern Illinois University 2/478 (0.5%) 1/243 (0.5%) Stephen F. Austin State University 2/403 (0.5%) 2/223 (1.0%) University of Alabama 5/392 (1.3%) 1/243 (0.5%) University of Florida 8/680 (1.2%) 2/716 (0.3%)

184 THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY requirements led to increased rates of crosstalk in the control condition. Rates in the experimental condition were similar regardless of the requirements (Table 1), b =.78, S.E. b =.40, Wald (1 df ) = 4.70, p =.03, Cramer s V = 0.52. The length of time (number of hours to complete the requirement) required to complete the research requirement also interacted with experimental condition to predict the prevalence of crosstalk such that higher participant requirements led to increased rates of crosstalk in the control condition, while rates in the experimental condition were similar to the rates found in universities that did not have high requirements; b =.45, S.E. b =.21, Wald (1 df ) = 4.73, p =.03, Cramer s V = 0.53. DISCUSSION We investigated the prevalence of participant crosstalk across five different universities and found evidence of crosstalk at each. Given that our institution characteristics (location, size, etc.) varied greatly, our data indicate the importance of crosstalk and of its consideration by psychology subject pool coordinators and researchers using introductory psychology participants. More importantly, the rate of crosstalk and success of its treatment varied between institutions. Specifically, institutions that demanded more out of their research participants had higher rates of crosstalk. Of course, the astute reader might note that several other variables appear to co-vary with institutional requirements (such as the size of the university s student body or prestige of the university). However, we measured variables related to these institutional-level variables (e.g. the size of the introduction to psychology pool), and they did not impact the results. Additionally, Northern Illinois University (NIU) reported substantially less crosstalk in the current sample than was reported in Edlund and colleagues study (2009). We believe this difference further supports our argument that the requirements of introduction to psychology courses matter; NIU s research requirements are lower now than they were in Edlund and colleagues study (2009). These findings have important implications for researchers collecting data from psychology subject pools. First, crosstalk occurred at every university suggesting that researchers need to be concerned with its detection independent of institution. Second, crosstalk occurs more frequently in participant pools that demand more from their participants indicating the importance of employing techniques to reduce crosstalk for researchers at universities with more demanding psychology subject pools. In our study, we demonstrated that a laboratory-based treatment (consisting of one sentence) successfully reduced rates of crosstalk at these high demand schools. Future research should consider these findings and examine factors that will help reduce and detect participant crosstalk. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Preliminary results were presented at the 2011 Southeastern Society of Social Psychologists annual meeting. We wish to thank our many undergraduate research assistants who allowed us to complete this project.

EDLUND ET AL. 185 NOTE 1. We also assessed several other potential variables that varied between the universities. These were non-significant (and available from the authors upon request). AUTHOR NOTES John E. Edlund is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at the Rochester Institute of Technology. His research interests include research methodology, romantic relationships, and personality. Austin Lee Nichols is an Assistant Professor at the University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain. His research interests include leadership, personality, impression management, and methodology. Bradley M. Okdie is affiliated with The Ohio State University at Newark. Rosanna E. Guadagno is affiliated with the National Science Foundation. Cassie A. Eno is affiliated with Bellevue University. Jeremy D. Heider is affiliated with the Department of Psychology at Southeast Missouri State University. Edward J. Hansen is affiliated with Northern Illinois University. Brad J. Sagarin is affiliated with Northern Illinois University. Ginette Blackhart is affiliated with East Tennessee State University. Catherine A. Cottrell is affiliated with the New College of Florida. Kenneth Tyler Wilcox is affiliated with Rochester Institute of Technology. REFERENCES Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Gonzales, M. H. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Edlund, J. E., Sagarin, B. J, Skowronski, J. J., Johnson, S., & Kutter, J. (2009). Whatever happens in the laboratory stays in the laboratory: The prevalence and prevention of participant crosstalk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 635 642. doi:10.1177/0146167208331255 Nichols, A. L. & Maner, J. (2008). The good-subject effect: Investigating participant demand characteristics. The Journal of General Psychology, 135(2), 151 165. doi:10.3200/genp.135.2.151-166 Received November 2, 2013 Accepted December 1, 2013