ALTERING THE TIMING OF ACADEMIC PROMPTS TO TREAT DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY ESCAPE MERCEDES E. EBANKS WAYNE W. FISHER



Similar documents
was sensitive to social negative reinforcement.

ISER G. DELEON AND JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2013, 46, NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2013) KENNEDY KRIEGER INSTITUTE

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 36, NUMBER 1(SPRING 2003)

ABA & Teaching Methods

Functional Behavior Assessment: Characteristics and Recommendations

7/17/2014. Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Therapy Overview. Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov

An experimental comparison of the effects of positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement during skill acquisition

THE ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT OF VOCAL STEREOTYPY IN A CHILD WITH AUTISM

OFFICE OF MENTAL HEALTH AND SUBSTANCE ABUSE SERVICES BULLETIN

EVALUATION OF MULTIPLE SCHEDULES WITH NATURALLY OCCURRING AND CONTRIVED DISCRIMINATIVE STIMULI FOLLOWING FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2000, 33, NUMBER 4(WINTER 2000)

AN ABA APPROACH TO TEACHING THE DIFFICULT CHILD. By: Lindsay Rice and Stephanie Beaulieu

PROPOSAL FOR CREATING AND EVALUATING TEACHER TRAINING MODULES IN APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS. By, Summer Navarrete

The ABC s of ABA. Claire Benson Kimberly Snyder Sarah Kroll Judy Aldridge

Applied Behavior Analysis Reinforcement. Elisabeth (Lisa) Kinney, M.S. September 19, 2007

Applied Behavior Analysis Reinforcement. Elisabeth (Lisa) Kinney, M.S. September 26, 2007

Moving Beyond the Function of Behavior: Working with Students who Engage in Escape/Avoidance

EVALUATION OF A SELF-INSTRUCTION PACKAGE FOR CONDUCTING STIMULUS PREFERENCE ASSESSMENTS RICHARD B. GRAFF AMANDA M. KARSTEN

ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE ASSESSMENT, TREATMENT, AND PREVENTION OF PROBLEM BEHAVIOR PETER MCGILL

Applied Behavior Analysis. Erik Arntzen 1. Akershus University College

Behavior Analysis and Strategy Application after Brain Injury: Addressing the long-term behavioral outcomes of brain injury

Positive Behaviour Support Plan for Jane. Brief Summary of the Critical System Strengths and Concerns (for school):

Functional Behavior Assessment. Denise Duke, Ed. S. Behavior Specialist Houston County Board of Education

FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF PROBLEM BEHAVIOR: A REVIEW GREGORY P. H ANLEY BRIAN A. IWATA BRANDON E. MCCORD

4/25/2014. What is ABA? Do I use ABA? Should I use ABA?

Efficient Functional Behavior Assessment: The Functional Assessment Checklist for Teachers and Staff: Part A

1 The following is adapted from Division of Mental Retardation Services, State of Tennessee, Provider Manual,

Evaluating the Effectiveness of a Tablet Application to Increase Eye Contact in Children Diagnosed with Autism

For a variety of reasons, behavior patterns develop differently in children,

Teaching & Behavioral Challenges

Professional Development: Applied Behavior Analysis Video Series

USING ESTABLISHING OPERATION MANIPULATIONS TO IMPROVE FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING. Nicole M. DeRosa. B.A. Western New England College, 2004

FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF EAR PLUGGING AND TREATMENT ANALYSIS OF NOISE DAMPENING HEADPHONES. Erin M. Beardsley. B.A. Bates College, 2003

Functional behavioral assessment (FBA) is the process of identifying

Preventing and Responding to Challenging Behaviors in the Home. Michael Boardman, MA, BCBA

1993, NUMBER 3 (FAu 1993)

Behavior Strategies, Progress Monitoring & Data Collection

Pivotal Response Training: Parent Professional Collaboration

Sample Fraction Addition and Subtraction Concepts Activities 1 3

o noise levels o brightness o o

Instructions for Completing the. FBA: Visual Contingency Diagram

TREATMENTS FOR AUTISM

Dimensions of ABA. Applied Behavior Analysis for Educational Settings. Underlying Assumptions of ABA

Training Caregivers of Elders with Dementia Who Exhibit Challenging Behaviors to Take a Functional Approach

Positive Behavior Support Strategies:

Steps for Implementation: Discrete Trial Training

You, Too, Can Do ABA ABA at its Foundation

Behavior Observation Forms

TARGETING THE BIG THREE CHALLENGING BEHAVIORS CAREGIVER MANUAL

Functional behavioural assessment (FBA) and challenging behaviours;

CONSISTENT VISUAL ANALYSES OF INTRASUBJECT DATA SUNGWOO KAHNG KYONG-MEE CHUNG KATHARINE GUTSHALL STEVEN C. PITTS

Chapter 2 Applied Behavior Analysis and Its Application to Autism and Autism Related Disorders

SAN MATEO COUNTY SELPA POSITIVE BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTIONS POLICY

Possible Interventions for the Function of Escape/Avoidance/Delay

Practical Principles Using Applied Behavior Analysis

Contact Information. Phone

The Role of Time-Out in a Comprehensive Approach for Addressing Challenging Behaviors of Preschool Children

Adaptive Behavior Assessments and Treatment Descriptors for July 1, 2014 Reporting

Assessment Method 1: Direct Skills Assessment

Using ABA for the Treatment of Autism: The CARD Program

PSY 5248: Applied Behavior Analysis 2 Thursdays 2:00PM 4:45PM, PSY 3

Amanda M. Karsten (née Firth), Ph.D., BCBA-D 595 Parker Street East Longmeadow, MA (413)

Behavior Impedes Learning

School Authority: Society For Treatment of Autism (Calgary Region)

FELLOWSHIP IN PROFESSIONAL POSTDOCTORAL PSYCHOLOGY KENNEDY KRIEGER INSTITUTE DEPARTMENT OF BEHAVIORAL PSYCHOLOGY

Volume1 Issue1. Behavior Function: Staying Close to What We Know. George Sugai and Rob Horner

CARD Rochester Autism Conference. Successful Collaboration in Language Intervention for Children with Autism: A Team Approach to SLP and ABA

Using the Natural Language Paradigm (NLP) to increase vocalizations of older adults with cognitive impairments

Autism Spectrum Disorder Series: Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Sylvia J. Acosta, PhD May 23, 2016

P r o f e s s i o n a l P s y c h o l o g y

Task Analysis: Steps for Implementation

Differential Reinforcement of Other Behaviors: Steps for Implementation

A Brief Explanation of Applied Behavior Analysis. conditioning to identify the contingencies affecting a student s behavior and the functions of the

Effectively Using Applied Behavior Analysis Interventions in Home Settings. Prepared by Sheri Kingsdorf, MA, BCBA, CABAS TI, TII, MT

Table Required Assessments and Qualified Examiners by Type of Disability Disability Assessments Required Qualified Examiners

CHAPTER 14 THE EFFECTS OF AUGMENTED FEEDBACK ON SKILL LEARNING. Chapter 16 1

Cristine Deaver, MS, BCBA, LABA Behavior Analyst

Decreasing Self-Injurious Behaviors in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Susan L. McCorkle. Lynchburg College

BRIDGE STUDIES IN BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS: EVOLUTION AND CHALLENGES IN JABA David P. Wacker The University of Iowa

Behaviorism & Education

Functional Communication Training: A Review of the Literature Related to Children with Autism

Bristol Intensive PBS Service for children with learning disabilities at risk of school exclusion

Crisis Prevention and Management Tool

Evidence-Based Practice in Autism Spectrum Disorders: What Does it Mean? CIGNA Autism Education Series

Addressing challenging behaviour in children with Down syndrome: The use of applied behaviour analysis for assessment and intervention

Creating Stimulus Control. In a Classroom for Children with Autism. Emily Levy. California State University, Fresno

Behavior & Sensory Strategies for Individuals with ASD

Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis

Department of Psychology PSY 421: Advanced Behavior Modification Fall 2012 Syllabus

Function of Problem Behavior. Elisabeth Kinney October 24, 2007

Things to Do. Data Collection. Data collection is collecting specific information about

A New Vision of Rehabilitation Recovering cognitive abilities with Dynavision

Heather Maurin, MA, EdS, PPS, LEP, BICM School Psychologist-Stockton Unified School District THE ABC S OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS

A Behavioral Perspective of Childhood Trauma and Attachment Issues: Toward Alternative Treatment Approaches for Children with a History of Abuse

Psychological Theory and theory-based intervention. Marie Johnston University of Aberdeen

Research in Developmental Disabilities

EAB 3764: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (Spring 2013) MWF 3 rd period, Psychology 151

The Effectiveness of Applied Behavior Analysis

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) from Pre-K through Transition

ALTERING THE NEAR-MISS EFFECT IN SLOT MACHINE GAMBLERS MARK R. DIXON, BECKY L. NASTALLY, JAMES E. JACKSON, AND REZA HABIB

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 36, 355 359 NUMBER 3(FALL 2003) ALTERING THE TIMING OF ACADEMIC PROMPTS TO TREAT DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY ESCAPE MERCEDES E. EBANKS MARCUS INSTITUTE AND HOWARD UNIVERSITY AND WAYNE W. FISHER MARCUS AND KENNEDY KRIEGER INSTITUTES AND JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE Following a functional analysis showing that destructive behavior was reinforced by escape, we altered the aversiveness of task demands by interspersing easy and difficult tasks and by presenting a corrective prompt as an antecedent event the next time a previously failed item was presented; this procedure was compared with one in which the corrective prompt was provided as an immediate consequence. Results of a reversal design showed that the antecedent prompt acted as an establishing operation and reduced destructive behavior to zero. DESCRIPTORS: academic instruction, antecedent intervention, destructive behavior, escape, establishing operation, feedback, functional analysis One approach to the treatment of destructive behavior reinforced by escape from demands is to manipulate establishing operations (EOs), antecedent variables that alter the effectiveness of escape as reinforcement (Iwata, Smith, & Michael, 2000). Prior research has shown that (a) a variety of task properties (e.g., task difficulty, rate of demands) can establish escape as reinforcement for destructive behavior (e.g., Horner, Day, Sprague, O Brien, & Heathfield, 1991; Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995) and (b) interspersing tasks with less aversive properties (Horner et al., 1991), allowing a choice among tasks (Romaniuk et al., 2002), or gradually fading in aversive tasks (Pace, Ivancic, & Jefferson, 1994) can re- This investigation was supported in part by Grant 5 R01 HD37837-03 from the Department of Health and Human Services, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Requests for reprints should be addressed to Wayne W. Fisher, Marcus Behavior Center, 1920 Briarcliff Road, Atlanta, Georgia 30329. duce destructive behavior maintained by escape. In the current case study, anecdotal observations during a functional analysis suggested that demands were aversive primarily when the participant received immediate corrective feedback following an error because destructive behavior often occurred when the second (modeled) prompt was delivered in the demand condition, and because similar reactions occurred in the play condition (e.g., when the participant put a puzzle piece in the wrong place and the therapist offered immediate assistance). Therefore, we subsequently developed and evaluated the effects of a teaching strategy in which easy tasks were interspersed with difficult ones and corrective feedback was delayed and made less salient by providing it as an antecedent prompt the next time a failed item was presented. We compared this antecedent intervention to the more common approach of providing corrective feedback as a consequence immediately after an error occurred. 355

356 MERCEDES E. EBANKS and WAYNE W. FISHER METHOD AND RESULTS Participant and Setting Jim, a 19-year-old man with mental retardation and pervasive developmental disorder secondary to traumatic brain injury, had been admitted to a day-treatment program for treatment of destructive behavior. All sessions lasted 10 min and were conducted in a room (3 m by 3 m) with padded walls, a table and a chair, and a one-way mirror for unobtrusive observation. Response Definitions and Measurement Jim s destructive behavior consisted of self-injurious behavior (SIB; head hitting, face scratching, hitting teeth with hand, knee biting), aggression (grabbing, pushing), and property destruction (banging or throwing objects). Observers collected frequency-within-interval data on laptop computers from behind the one-way mirror. These data were converted to a rate (responses per minute) by dividing the frequency by 10, the number of minutes in each session. Two observers simultaneously but independently recorded responses during 43.3% of the functional analysis sessions and during 41.2% of the treatment evaluation sessions. Exact agreement coefficients were calculated by first partitioning each session into 60 10-s intervals and then dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100%. An agreement was scored if both observers recorded the same number of responses in an interval. Agreement coefficients averaged 99.1% and 99.3% for SIB, 99.4% and 100% for aggression, and 97.9% and 99.3% for property destruction during functional analysis and treatment evaluation sessions, respectively. Functional Analysis Procedure. Functional analysis procedures were similar to those of Iwata, Slifer, Dorsey, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994) except that (a) a tangible condition was included in which access to a puzzle was presented for 30 s contingent on destructive behavior, and (b) a pairwise comparison between the play condition and a demand condition (with difficult tasks, which are described below) was conducted to clarify whether an escape function maintained destructive behavior. Results. Results of the functional analysis are depicted in the top panel of Figure 1. Rates of destructive behavior were higher and somewhat more consistent in demand (M 1.5) relative to the other conditions (Ms 0.5, 0, 1.3, and 0.8 for attention, alone, tangible, and play, respectively). In addition, when the demand and play conditions were conducted in a pairwise manner in the second phase of the functional analysis, the suspected escape function was confirmed (Ms 1.4 and 0.3 in demand and play, respectively). We observed in these sessions that difficult demands tended to lead to errors, which resulted in corrective feedback, which in turn, evoked destructive behavior. Based on these observations, we compared two antecedent interventions for destructive behavior, one that altered task difficulty (by interspersing easy tasks among the difficult tasks) and a second that altered task difficulty in the same manner but also eliminated corrective feedback by providing an academic prompt as an antecedent the next time a previously failed item was presented. Treatment Evaluation Procedure. The treatment analysis consisted of a comparison (using a reversal design) between two demand conditions that were identical except for the manner in which academic prompts were delivered. During each condition, prompts to complete nonpreferred tasks (matching colors with printed names of the colors in a four-option match-

ALTERING INSTRUCTIONAL PROCEDURES 357 Figure 1. The top panel shows the rates of destructive behavior during the functional analysis, and the bottom panel shows the rates of destructive behavior during the treatment evaluation that compared antecedent prompting with consequent feedback. ing-to-sample arrangement) were presented about once every 30 s, and destructive behavior resulted in a break from the tasks for 30 s. During odd-numbered trials, easy colors were presented (ones he had previously matched accurately). During even-numbered trials, more difficult colors were presented (novel items or ones he had frequently matched incorrectly in the past). Easy tasks were interspersed with difficult tasks to reduce task difficulty across each session and also so that praise was delivered on at least half of the trials in both conditions. In the consequent feedback condition, each trial began with a verbal prompt (e.g., Match the color with the word. ). Praise followed correct responses, and corrective feedback followed incorrect responses (which occurred almost exclusively with difficult colors). Corrective feedback consisted of a vocal statement ( No, that s not right. Match the color teal with the word teal, like

358 MERCEDES E. EBANKS and WAYNE W. FISHER this. ) combined with the therapist modeling the correct response. This condition differed from the demand condition of the functional analysis in that a physical prompt was not included, and the difficulty of the task was alternated on successive trials as described above. These two changes were included in the consequent feedback condition so that this condition was equivalent to the antecedent prompting condition, except for the manner in which academic instruction was provided. In the antecedent prompting condition, each trial began with a verbal prompt (e.g., Match the color with the word. ). Praise followed correct responses, but incorrect responses were not followed immediately by corrective feedback. Rather, the next scheduled trial was presented (which was almost always an easy item). Following this, the last task that was failed was repeated, but was preceded by the corrective prompt. The prompt consisted of a vocal statement (e.g., This is the color teal and this is the word teal. These two go together. ) combined with the therapist modeling the correct response. Results. Results of the treatment evaluation are depicted in the bottom panel of Figure 1. Destructive behavior occurred at low but relatively consistent rates in the consequent feedback condition (M 0.3), whereas destructive behavior never occurred during the antecedent prompting condition (M 0). In both conditions, Jim responded correctly and received praise during almost all of the odd-numbered trials when easy items were presented (Ms 94% and 97% for easy colors in consequent feedback and antecedent prompting, respectively). By contrast, Jim responded correctly to a lower percentage of the difficult colors presented during even-numbered trials, but correct responding increased somewhat across successive phases (Ms 45%, 55%, 68%, and 77% in Phases 1 through 4, respectively). DISCUSSION Functional analysis results showed that Jim s destructive behavior was maintained by negative reinforcement. Anecdotal observations during the functional analysis suggested that difficult demands evoked destructive behavior primarily when Jim received immediate corrective feedback. Therefore, a treatment was developed, called antecedent prompting, to reduce the EO for destructive behavior by (a) decreasing task difficulty (by interspersing easy tasks) and (b) eliminating corrective feedback as an immediate consequence and instead delivering it as an antecedent to previously failed tasks while making no mention of the prior error. This intervention immediately reduced destructive behavior to zero even though the escape contingency remained intact. By contrast, destructive behavior occurred at higher rates when easy and difficult tasks were interspersed and correction immediately followed errors in the consequent feedback condition. The current study adds to the literature on the effects of EOs on destructive behavior reinforced by escape (e.g., Horner et al., 1991; Smith et al., 1995) by showing that altering the timing of remedial procedures may reduce the influence of an escape contingency in some cases. It should be noted, however, that the rates of destructive behavior were considerably lower in both treatment conditions than in the demand condition of the functional analysis, which was probably due to the interspersing of easier tasks during the two treatment conditions. One limitation of the current investigation is the fact that it involved only 1 participant. A second limitation is that it is not possible to determine whether altering the time of the corrective prompts decreased the aversiveness of the demands by eliminating

ALTERING INSTRUCTIONAL PROCEDURES 359 corrective feedback (i.e., no longer calling attention to the participant s errors) or by simply making the task easier. That is, in the antecedent prompting condition, the therapist modeled the correct answer just prior to the next presentation of a previously failed task. Future research should be directed at replicating these findings with additional participants and conducting analyses to determine more precisely how the EO for escape-maintained behavior was altered in the antecedent prompting condition. REFERENCES Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., Sprague, J. R., O Brien, M., & Heathfield, L. T. (1991). Interspersed requests: A nonaversive procedure for reducing aggression and self-injury during instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 265 278. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197 209. (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3 20, 1982) Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., & Michael, J. L. (2000). Current research on the influence of establishing operations on behavior in applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 411 418. Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., & Jefferson, G. (1994). Stimulus fading as treatment for obscenity in a brain-injured adult. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 301 305. Romaniuk, C., Miltenberger, R., Conyers, C., Jenner, N., Jurgens, M., & Ringenberg, C. (2002). The influence of activity choice on problem behaviors maintained by escape versus attention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 349 362. Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Goh, H., & Shore, B. A. (1995). Analysis of establishing operations for self-injury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 515 535. Received January 15, 2003 Final acceptance May 7, 2003 Action Editor, Brian Iwata