Gender Stereotypes Are Alive, Well, and Busy Producing. workplace discrimination. COMMENTARIES



Similar documents
Reviewing Applicants. Research on Bias and Assumptions

Triangulation Across Methodologies: All Signs Point to Persistent Stereotyping. and discrimination influence organizational

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal

IPDET Module 6: Descriptive, Normative, and Impact Evaluation Designs

I m good, she s good: The relationship between. Self-Esteem and Attitudes Towards Women in the Work Force. Alexandria Peach. McKendree University

No Credit Where Credit Is Due: Attributional Rationalization of Women s Success in Male Female Teams

Creating a Culture of Inclusion

Olivia A. O Neill 1 and Charles A. O Reilly III 2

When Fit Is Fundamental: Performance Evaluations and Promotions of Upper-Level Female and Male Managers

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

HealthStream Regulatory Script

Teachers as Adult Learners: A New Perspective

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Battery Bias

MAY Legal Risks of Applicant Selection and Assessment

Overview In this lecture we will focus on the difference between sex and gender, and review the emergence of the study of gender as a discipline.

Increasing Diversity in your Department

Validity, Fairness, and Testing

Three Theories of Individual Behavioral Decision-Making

Egon Zehnder International. The Leading Edge of Diversity and Inclusion. 11th International Executive Panel October 2012

National Quali cations SPECIMEN ONLY

Evaluating the Negative Impact of Gender Stereotypes on. Women's Advancement in Organizations. Mayia Corcoran

Where is the Romance for Women in Leadership? Gender Effects on the Romance of Leadership and Performance-Based Pay

CFSD 21 ST CENTURY SKILL RUBRIC CRITICAL & CREATIVE THINKING

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION

Statistics 2014 Scoring Guidelines

Review of Diversity and Public Administration: Theory, Issues, and Perspectives, 2nd ed.

Activity 1 Myth Busters Disability 13. Activity 15 Growing Up and Growing Older Age 85. Activity 16 Similarities Between Old and Young Age 88

APPRAISAL POLICY 1. BACKGROUND

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING 540 AUDITING ACCOUNTING ESTIMATES, INCLUDING FAIR VALUE ACCOUNTING ESTIMATES, AND RELATED DISCLOSURES CONTENTS

What holds women back? Gender barriers at work Olga Epitropaki, PhD

Gender and Racial Bias in Hiring

THE BEHAVIORAL-BASED INTERVIEW

Introducing Social Psychology

IQ Testing: A critique for parents of children with developmental disabilities

UNC Leadership Survey 2012: Women in Business

Why are there so few women in the tech industry in Oslo, Norway?

INTERNATIONAL FRAMEWORK FOR ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS CONTENTS

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender,

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介

Human Resources Training. Performance Management Training Module 2: Managing Employee Performance

Test Bias. As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but

Running head: EFFECTS OF ADVERTISEMENTS ON PERCEPTIONS OF WOMEN S LEADERSHIP

Journal of Issues in Intercollegiate Athletics, 2011, 4, College Sport Research Institute

Training Module: Managing Diversity

Psychology of Women PSY-270-TE

CHAPTER 2. Business Ethics and Social Responsibility

ENCOURAGING STUDENTS IN A RACIALLY DIVERSE CLASSROOM

Unifying Epistemologies by Combining World, Description and Observer

Bloomsburg University Midterm and Final Competency Field Evaluation. Task Supervisor (if appropriate) :

A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF WORKFORCE DIVERSITY IN SERVICE AND MANUFACTURING SECTORS IN INDIA

ADAPTATION OF EMPLOYEES IN THE ORGANIZATION AND ITS IMPORTANCE IN TERMS OF HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT

What is a P-value? Ronald A. Thisted, PhD Departments of Statistics and Health Studies The University of Chicago

Women, careers and leadership roles: lessons for the academy from other professional workers. (0271)

In an experimental study there are two types of variables: Independent variable (I will abbreviate this as the IV)

DOING YOUR BEST ON YOUR JOB INTERVIEW

MEN, WOMEN, AND MANAGERS: ARE STEREOTYPES FINALLY CHANGING?

Self-Efficacy in the Workplace: Implications for Motivation and Performance

Contents. Introduction About the Activities 2. Introduction Activity Descriptions 3. Introduction Where does equality & diversity fit in?

A. Introducing Social Psychology. Introduction

AN AUDIT OF INTERNAL CONTROL OVER FINANCIAL REPORTING THAT IS INTEGRATED WITH AN AUDIT OF FINANCIAL STATEMENTS:

Practical-Theoretical Approach in the Application of Theory Models of Organizational Behavior Dr. Robert DeYoung - Saint Thomas University

Structured Interviewing:

HIGH SCHOOL MASS MEDIA AND MEDIA LITERACY STANDARDS

Chapter 2. Theories of Psychosocial and Cognitive Development

A. Majchrzak & M. L. Markus

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY

Executive Summary. Summary - 1

Test-Retest Reliability and The Birkman Method Frank R. Larkey & Jennifer L. Knight, 2002

Emotional Competence and Leadership Excellence at Johnson & Johnson: The Emotional Intelligence and Leadership Study

FORMAL AND INFORMAL DISCRIMINATION AGAINST WOMEN AT WORK

Audit Sampling. AU Section 350 AU

AN INTRODUCTION TO SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES

The Future of Female CEOs and Their Glass Ceiling

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING (UK AND IRELAND) 240 THE AUDITOR S RESPONSIBILITY TO CONSIDER FRAUD IN AN AUDIT OF FINANCIAL STATEMENTS CONTENTS

WORKING PAPER SERIES. WP 79/2008 June An Exploration of Gender Stereotypes in Perception and Practice of Leadership. Discipline: Management

REPORT OF THE ACTIVITIES OF THE DISCRIMINATION AND HARASSMENT COUNSEL FOR THE LAW SOCIETY OF UPPER CANADA

The contribution of the Saudi woman in economic development

Social Bias: Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination Sabrina Keene, MSP*

Enhancing Long-term Retention of South Africa Women in Chemical and Metallurgical Engineering Careers

A Robustness Simulation Method of Project Schedule based on the Monte Carlo Method

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING 530 AUDIT SAMPLING AND OTHER MEANS OF TESTING CONTENTS

The Auditor's Responsibilities Relating to Other Information

Introduction to industrial/organizational psychology

Entrepreneurship is attractive to many youth in the abstract. Key Messages. Data and methodology

A Study on Consumer Behavior of Aavin Milk in Bhel Township: Trichy

Course Description Course Textbook Course Learning Outcomes Credits Course Structure Unit Learning Outcomes: Unit Lesson: Reading Assignments:

She s Got the Look: Inferences from Female Chief Executive Officers Faces Predict their Success

Sexual Ethics in the Workplace

Valuing Diversity. Cornerstones. 1. Diversity is about inclusion and engagement!

Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1

Organization Culture and Effective Work CHAPTER 8

Gender Sensitive Data Gathering Methods

IMPLEMENTING BUSINESS CONTINUITY MANAGEMENT IN A DISTRIBUTED ORGANISATION: A CASE STUDY

Manager briefing. Gender pay equity guide for managers GENDER P Y EQUITY

Best Practices for Search Committees

t1ll j I f7 07 FEB -6 PM 1:40

Why Me? Why My Classroom? The Need for Equity in Coed Math and Science Classes

DRAFT MODEL POLICY ON ACCOMMODATION. TO: All employers in the legal profession in Nova Scotia

Paid and Unpaid Labor in Developing Countries: an inequalities in time use approach

Transcription:

Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1 (2008), 393 398. Copyright ª 2008 Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. 1754-9426/08 COMMENTARIES Gender Stereotypes Are Alive, Well, and Busy Producing Workplace Discrimination MADELINE E. HEILMAN New York University ALICE H. EAGLY Northwestern University A critical examination of research on the relationship between stereotyping and workplace discrimination must meet three requirements. The first requirement is an understanding of the theory that guides this research. The second requirement is an unbiased review of relevant research. The third requirement is comprehension of the ways that different types of research are informative about behavior in organizations. Landy (2008) meets none of these requirements. He misstates the consensual social scientific theory about the relation between stereotyping and discrimination, presents only a selective portion of the relevant research, and misconstrues the basis for generalizing research findings to organizations. As a result, Landy misrepresents the evidence for stereotype-based workplace discrimination. For brevity, we consider only sex discrimination. Also, consistent with Landy s emphasis, we address the consequences of Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Madeline E. Heilman. E-mail: madeline. heilman@nyu.edu Address: Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Pl Room 576, New York, NY 10003 Madeline E. Heilman, Department of Psychology, New York University; Alice H. Eagly, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University. stereotypes that describe women and men as opposed to stereotypes that prescribe normatively acceptable behavior for them and thus sanction behavior deviating from gender norms (see Eagly & Karau, 2002; Heilman, 2001). The Prevailing Theory of Discrimination Landy asserts that psychological theory views stereotypes negativity as the basis of workplace discrimination. This assertion is an inaccurate simplification of contemporary theory concerning discrimination in general and sex discrimination in particular (e.g., Glick & Fiske, 2007). Theory has moved beyond the idea of discrimination as simply an outcome of antipathy (see Dovidio, Glick, & Rudman, 2005). In fact, women, who are regarded as the nicer, kinder sex, have a cultural stereotype that is in general more positive than that of men (Eagly & Mladinic, 1994), yet women often are victims of prejudice. Theorists resolved this paradox by recognizing that it is not the negativity of gender stereotypes but their mismatch with desirable work roles that underlies biased workplace evaluations. Heilman (1983, 2001) articulated this lack of fit model (see also Dipboye, 1985), and 393

394 M.E. Heilman and A.H. Eagly Eagly and Karau (2002) promoted this idea in their role incongruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Consistent with prevailing theory, biased personnel decisions about women can flow from positive or negative attributes ascribed to women. Female stereotypical attributes, including the positive communal qualities of warmth and niceness, tend to be inconsistent with the attributes believed to be required for success in many key organizational positions. People associate such roles with more masculine than feminine attributes (e.g., Schein, 2001). What results from this mismatch between a group stereotype (e.g., women) and a job role (e.g., manager) is negative performance expectations, which in turn produce biased evaluations. And because individuals are commonly assimilated to group stereotypes, this evaluative penalty is exacted even from women who do not possess the qualities that are stereotypical of their group but do possess those regarded as necessary for success in the job role. Discrimination is a behavioral expression of this evaluative penalty. Differing Research Paradigms Relevant to Stereotype-Based Sex Discrimination Landy errs in identifying relevant research. He buttresses his claim that the research has relied on an artificial work paradigm by singling out such studies and excluding other types of studies. Among his glaring omissions is a body of research in which persons responsible for personnel selection in organizations receive job applications or resumes (or sometimes phone calls or in-person applicants) through ordinary channels. These research participants do not realize that they are providing data for a research project or that certain applications have been constructed to differ only in sex or other attributes to meet the requirements of an experimental design. This omission of these natural setting experiments, which are known as audit studies, is puzzling, given their popularity (see Pager, 2007) and their direct implications for understanding labor markets. Although most of these field experiments have addressed racial and ethnic discrimination, some have examined sex discrimination (see review by Riach & Rich, 2002). Their findings conformed to the lack-of-fit formulation: There was a high incidence of sex discrimination against women in the more senior jobs that yield higher status and wages and against both sexes when they applied for jobs dominated by the other sex. Other researchers have exploited natural variation in conditions that deter discrimination. For example, Goldin and Rouse (2000) conducted a quasi-experimental study of major symphony orchestras hiring of musicians, positions traditionally held almost exclusively by men. This project compared the success of women and men when the orchestras did (vs. did not) use a screen to hide the identity of the musicians who were auditioning. The screen increased women s success by approximately 50% in the initial round of auditions and made them 1.6 times more likely to win an orchestral position. Field studies focusing on performance evaluation also support the lack-of-fit principle, including the one such study that Landy cites (Lyness & Heilman, 2006). Using archival organizational data from 489 upper- and senior-level managers from a large financial services organization, this study compared women and men after controlling for age, education, organizational level, and organizational tenure (which Landy incorrectly implied was not controlled). The results showed that women in line jobs (male gender typed positions) received lower performance ratings than women in staff jobs (female gender typed positions) or men in either line or staff jobs. Moreover, a metaanalysis of 96 studies of the effectiveness of leaders, as assessed mainly by performance evaluations in organizational field studies, found that men fared better than women in male-dominated leadership roles or culturally masculine settings but that women surpassed men in settings that were less male dominated or less culturally masculine (Eagly, Karau, & Makhijani, 1995). Thus, the lack-of-fit principle prevails in natural settings, whether field experiments, quasi-experiments, or

Gender stereotypes are alive, well, and busy 395 correlational field studies are the method of investigation and whether selection or performance evaluation is the outcome of interest. This natural setting research is consistent with laboratory experiments, which also show that evaluative bias follows from lack of fit to job roles. With respect to performance evaluations of leaders, a meta-analysis of experiments that held leaders behavior constant while varying only their sex showed that men were evaluated more favorably than women in male-dominated leadership roles but equally in other leadership roles (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992). Also, taking 49 experiments in simulated employment contexts into account, Davison and Burke (2000) classified the research according to stereotype-driven mismatch between job candidates and roles. In male sex typed jobs (e.g., auto salesperson, life insurance agent), men were preferred over women. However, in female sex typed jobs (e.g., secretary, director of a day care center) women were preferred over men. If natural setting research and laboratory experimental research show basically the same pattern of findings, what do these experiments add? Laboratory experiments have the advantage of the more perfect control that follows from randomly assigning participants to conditions that differ only in the sex of the target job applicant or employee. This control reduces the chances that extraneous variables are correlated with sex and thus eliminates alternative interpretations of bias effects. Given the inevitable trade-offs between the control of laboratory experiments and the realism of studies conducted in natural settings, the best support for lack-of-fit principles emerges from consistent findings across these different design types. Also, as we elaborate in the next sections, another advantage of experiments is that they can allow the precise identification of conditions that favor (vs. deter) stereotype-based discrimination. Both laboratory and field findings are highly supportive of the idea that sex bias in evaluations of women arise not from their sex but from the perceived mismatch between their inferred female attributes and the requirements of male gender typed jobs. Nonetheless, we agree with Landy s point that a meta-analysis that integrates research from all the relevant research paradigms, treating paradigm as one of many moderator variables, would speak to the issue of the relative magnitude of sex effects in different paradigms. However, such a project is not required to recognize the converging laboratory and field support for the lack-of-fit hypothesis. Also, the lack-of-fit principle and supportive findings provide a plausible explanation for the absence of pro-male bias in studies in which the focal jobs are not male in gender type or in which the gender type of positions is not taken into account. In fact, sex discrimination can penalize men but only when the position is female gender typed positions men seldom seek. Generalization of Evaluative Bias Findings to Organizational Settings Landy s failure to recognize the findings that have emerged across varied research designs is compounded by his lack of comprehension of how laboratory experiments inform understanding of behavior in work settings. Instead, he offers a one-dimensional critique of laboratory research that rests on its presumed lack of generalizability to the real world because of descriptive differences between the laboratory and work settings and the greater complexity of typical work environments. Landy s discussion of generalizability contains two erroneous assumptions. The first is that research from any one setting can be generalized to another setting only if the two settings appear to be similar. His second assumption is that work organizations are similar to one another but different from psychology laboratories. However, there are many differences among work organizations, for example, in the average time employees spend in a given position; human resources practices and procedures; and organizational history, climate, and culture. In fact, many of what Landy assumes

396 M.E. Heilman and A.H. Eagly are common characteristics of workrelated decisions in organizations are actually quite variable and may differ more from one organization to another than from one organization to a laboratory setting. If, for instance, evaluators in one organization feel accountable because they have to publicly justify their evaluations, their decision making is apt to be different from that of evaluators in another organization who do not feel accountable, no matter how similar the organizations appear to be. However, the decision making of accountable organizational evaluators may not differ much from that of laboratory study participants for whom accountability has been created experimentally. In short, the similarity of constraints on evaluative behavior across social settings is not accurately described by Landy s dichotomy between artificial and real-world settings. The bridge between research and application is theory. Only theoretical principles supported by research reveal the essential similarity or dissimilarity of various types of settings. Theory guides the search for the conditions that promote or hinder discrimination. Once such conditions are identified, they expand the scope of theory. For example, studies have identified features of situations that make female stereotypes more salient (e.g., scarcity of women) and characteristics of women that highlight their femininity (e.g., physical attractiveness or motherhood). Such conditions increase evaluative bias because they exaggerate lack of fit with male sex typed roles (Heilman & Parks-Stamm, 2007). Also, research has identified situational factors that can affect subjectivity in judgment and reliance on stereotypes. These factors include degree of accountability, anticipated interdependence, and ambiguity in the decision process (e.g., in information, measures, criteria, standards, or source of performance; see Heilman & Haynes, 2008). It is similarity or dissimilarity in such moderating conditions that appropriately guides generalizations from research to organizational settings. Research thereby provides a road map for assessing, predicting, and remedying discrimination. Related to these issues of generalizability is Landy s assertion that dramatic changes in the nature of work render earlier findings irrelevant to contemporary work settings. Yet, recent findings as well as earlier findings produce the signature lack-of-fit pattern (e.g., Lyness & Heilman, 2006). Nonetheless, workplaces are always changing, and the implications of these changes for discrimination can be predicted from theory that explains their relevance. For example, the effects of virtual environments or work in teams can be anticipated by understanding the moderating effects of important theoretical constructs such as ambiguity, accountability, or the decision makers stake in their decisions. Therefore, relevant theory and supporting research are essential to explaining the effects of work conditions even rapidly changing ones on the relation between gender stereotyping and workplace discrimination. The Relevance of Individuating Information to Stereotyping Another of Landy s major points is that individuating information deters stereotyping in organizational settings. Landy points out that knowing someone in an organizational setting usually entails more than the limited information available to decision makers in laboratory studies and that access to this additional information nullifies the effects of stereotypes. Consistent with robust evidence of stereotype-based discrimination in the field research that we have cited, there are multiple reasons why stereotypes are often not quashed by additional information. Landy would have us believe that overriding stereotypes is an uncomplicated process and a common event. But neither is the case. Although individuating information can indeed deter stereotyping, there are many circumstances under which it does not have this effect. As well established in research on social cognition, whether stereotypes are activated and applied depends on a complex of cognitive and motivational variables (see Kunda & Spencer, 2003), including whether

Gender stereotypes are alive, well, and busy 397 individuals have the cognitive resources to inhibit stereotyping and whether they are motivated to be accurate. The conditions that deter stereotyping are often absent in work settings because of decision makers cognitive overload, motivation to maintain the status quo, and limited dependence on subordinates for valued outcomes. Thus, stereotype-based perceptions of lack of fit are likely to take hold despite the availability of additional information. Moreover, the expectations that these perceptions produce can be tenacious and self-sustaining, even in the face of disconfirming information. They can affect what information social perceivers attend to, how they interpret the information, what they remember, and how they engage in social interaction, often in a manner that reinforces their prior expectations. Therefore, the individuating information available to decision makers in organizational settings does not necessarily prevent discriminatory decision making. Landy s misunderstanding about the power of individuating information leads him to argue that the simplicity and control of laboratory research make it easier to find bias in the laboratory than in the field. However, there are some features of experimental laboratory research that make bias less rather than more likely. Specifically, the participants tend to be younger and therefore more accepting of contemporary cultural norms that espouse gender equity than many people who populate organizations. Also, laboratory participants are not likely to be as pressed for time or cognitively busy or distracted as organizational decision makers. Consequently, laboratory participants ordinarily have the cognitive resources to be more deliberative and use individuating information in making inferences. Therefore, it is not at all clear that the deck is stacked in favor of discrimination in laboratory experiments. Bottom Line In summary, Frank Landy falls short of making his case. He fails to accurately present the prevailing theory of how gender stereotypes produce sex discrimination. Moreover, by providing a highly selective review of the relevant research, he wrongly suggests that evidence for workplace sex discrimination derives almost exclusively from experiments using artificial work paradigms when in fact there is converging evidence from laboratory and field studies using a broad range of research paradigms. He also misinterprets research on the mitigating effects of individuating information on stereotyping and its consequences. And he shows no appreciation of the widely accepted methodological principle that the best bridge to applications derives from deploying theory. If Landy had thoroughly reviewed the sex discrimination literature and paid heed to the scientific issues involved in his claims, the result would no doubt have been a more productive exchange of views. References Davison, H. K., & Burke, M. J. (2000). Sex discrimination in simulated employment contexts: A metaanalytic investigation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 225 248. Dipboye, R. L. (1985). Some neglected variables in research on discrimination in appraisals. Academy of Management Review, 10, 116 127. Dovidio, J. F., Glick, P., & Rudman, L. (Eds.). (2005). On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573 598. Eagly, A. H., Karau, S. J., & Makhijani, M. G. (1995). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 125 145. Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3 22. Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1 35). New York: Wiley. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2007). Sex discrimination: The psychological approach. In F. J. Crosby, M. S. Stockdale, & S. A. Ropp (Eds.), Sex discrimination in the workplace: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 155 187). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Goldin, C., & Rouse, C. (2000). Orchestrating impartiality: The impact of blind auditions on female musicians. American Economic Review, 90, 715 741. Heilman, M. E. (1983). Sex bias in work settings: The lack of fit model. Research in Organizational Behavior, 5, 269 298.

398 M.E. Heilman and A.H. Eagly Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 657 674. Heilman, M. E., & Haynes, M. C. (2008). Subjectivity in the appraisal process: A facilitator of gender bias in work settings. In E. Borgida & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Beyond common sense: Psychological science in the courtroom (pp. 127 155). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Heilman, M. E., & Parks-Stamm, E. J. (2007). Gender stereotypes in the workplace: Obstacles to women s career progress. In S. J. Correll (Ed.), Social psychology of gender: Advances in group processes (Vol. 24, pp. 47 77). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 522 544. Landy, F. J. (2008). Stereotypes, bias, and personnel decisions: Strange and stranger. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 1, 379 392. Lyness, K. S., & Heilman, M. E. (2006). When fit is fundamental: Performance evaluations and promotions of upper-level female and male managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 777 785. Pager, D. (2007). The use of field experiments for studies of employment discrimination: Contributions, critiques, and directions for the future. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 609, 104 133. Riach, P. A., & Rich, J. (2002). Field experiments of discrimination in the market place. Economic Journal, 112, F480 F518. Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women s progress in management. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 675 688.