Lexicalization in Signed Languages: When is an Idiom not an Idiom?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Lexicalization in Signed Languages: When is an Idiom not an Idiom?"

Transcription

1 Lexicalization in Signed Languages: When is an Idiom not an Idiom? TREVOR JOHNSTON Macquarie University LINDSAY FERRARA Macquarie University Copyright 2012 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings Vol 1: The internal and often iconic structure of the signs in a signed language (SL) the rough equivalent of words in spoken languages present interesting challenges for linguistic theory. Specifically, lexicalisation in SLs occurs when a signed unit acquires a clearly identifiable and replicable citation form which is regularly and strongly associated with a meaning. The meaning is unpredictable and/or more specific than that implied by the value (meaning) that each component may contribute to the overall form of the signed unit. SL linguists continue to struggle with the dual nature of sign components they appear to be simultaneously phonemes and morphemes, depending on one s perspective. Cognitive linguistics and construction grammars offer a principled analysis of this situation. It holds that the use of linguistic symbols in patterned ways involves constructions that can be differentiated along two continua: one of size or simplicity (from atomic to complex), and one of lexical specificity (from substantive to schematic or abstract). Idioms, famously, are the most obvious manifestation of this lexico-grammatical continuum. In this presentation, we show how lexicalization in SLs can be best understood as a type of idiomaticity: fully-lexicalized signs (atomic and substantive) are idiomatic in SLs in much the same way as multi-word constructions (complex and substantive) can be idiomatic in spoken languages. Lexical signs are in a sense idioms: the components in a lexical construction do not (just) mean what they should mean based on its components. In SLs there thus appears to be a large role for idiomaticity at the level of lexical constructions, and very little use of idiomaticity on the level of multi-sign (multi-word) constructions, unlike spoken languages. The lexico-grammatical continuum exists in all languages, it is just that idiomaticity occurs at a different level in this continuum in SLs. Keywords: idioms, sign language, lexicalization, constructions, Australian sign language (Auslan) 1. Introduction An idiom in any language is an expression whose meaning is partially unpredictable because of regular and idiosyncratic properties. In the words of

2 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 230 Fillmore et al. (1988) an idiomatic expression or construction is something a language user could fail to know while knowing everything else in the language (p. 504). It has been suggested by Johnston and Schembri (2010) that lexical signs in signed languages (SLs) are idiomatic expressions in much the same way as some multi-word constructions are idiomatic expressions in spoken languages (SpLs). This paper explores Johnston and Schembri s claim in more detail and offers supporting evidence of why idiomaticity surfaces at the lexical level in SLs but not in SpLs. We show here, using a cognitive grammar approach, that idiomaticity in SLs occurs when partly-lexical depicting signs become fully-lexicalized. In other words, when signs that have gestural and iconic components become conventionalized within a community, they acquire an idiomatic reading. First we discuss the types and characteristics of symbolic units along the lexico-grammar continuum in SpLs and SLs. Then we describe how partlylexical depicting signs integrate gestural and linguistic components through real-space blending. The meanings of these signs are more or less componential and analyzable as part of a particular usage event. However, we go on to argue that as these signs become lexicalized their meaning becomes less predictable and thus idiomatic. We conclude with a discussion of how this relates to the sources of linguistic units and processes of meaning construction in Auslan and other SLs. 2. Symbolic Units along the Lexico-grammar Continuum A basic tenet of cognitive grammar posits that the lexicon forms a continuum with grammar and syntax to represent the progressively more complex and abstract symbolic units that exist within a language (Langacker 2002: 102). These symbolic units are bipolar structures that link a phonological pole and semantic pole via a symbolic association. An example of a symbolic unit in Auslan is illustrated in Figure 1; it is comprised of the semantic unit [AUSTRALIA] linked to the phonological unit [australia]. i Figure 1. The Auslan lexical unit [[AUSTRALIA]/[australia]]

3 231 Johnston and Ferrara Symbolic units can be characterized according to two scales: one of content and one of size (Croft and Cruse 2004; Langacker 2005). The content scale ranges from substantive to schematic and characterizes both phonological and semantic units. Substantive units are concrete and specified while schematic units are abstract and less specified. The size scale ranges from atomic (i.e., singular, small) to complex (i.e., multiple, large) and can also be used to characterize phonological and semantic units. In this way, distinctions can thus be made between simple expressions (in English) like the word [cockatoo] and more complex structures, such as the passive construction [SBJ be-tns VERB-en by OBL]. ii The Auslan lexical unit [[AUSTRALIA]/[australia]] is both atomic and substantive phonologically and semantically. That is, this sign is concrete and fully specified for content and it also cannot be further broken down into smaller phonological or semantic units. Contrast this with the Auslan lexical unit [[TOMATO]/[tomato]] that is phonologically and semantically more complex in size but still substantive in content (Figure 2). Figure 2. The Auslan lexical unit [[TOMATO]/[tomato]] In this sign, the complexity in size results from compounding; this sign derives from the integration of the two Auslan lexical units [[RED]/[red]] and [[BALL]/[ball]]. Complexity also results from lexical units being integrated into larger units such as phrases and clauses. In cognitive and construction grammars, a symbolic unit is therefore any pairing of a form with a meaning, while a construction is a symbolic unit that is itself made up of two or more smaller symbolic units. Strictly speaking therefore a symbolic unit which is not composed of any smaller recognizable symbolic units (e.g., a simple word or morpheme) is not also a construction. Thus in most of the literature construction is only used to refer to units which

4 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 232 are at the complex end of the lexico-grammar continuum. Nonetheless, in some accounts of construction grammar a minimal symbolic unit, such as a morpheme, is sometimes also described as a construction, e.g. Croft and Cruse (2004: 259). Croft and Cruse (2004) discuss how different constructions with different values of size and content can be mapped onto traditional linguistic labels. For example, constructions that are complex and mostly schematic in size and content (e.g. the passive construction cited above) are traditionally considered a part of syntax, whereas constructions that are complex in size and mostly substantive in content (e.g. the [kick-tns the bucket] construction in English) are traditionally treated as idioms. Croft and Cruse (2004: 255) attempt to exemplify the lexico-grammar continuum in tabular form in one vertical dimension (Table 1). Table 1. Examples of lexico-grammar continuum associated with traditional grammatical labels (adapted from Croft and Cruse 2004) These observations on the lexico-grammar continuum are represented by Langacker (2005) using two axes, one for each of the scales of schematicity and complexity, in a two-dimensional graph (Figure 3). As seen by this representation, what is traditionally known as the lexicon occupies the atomic end of the content scale but can range from simple to complex on the size scale. Grammar, on the other hand, is made up of constructions that fall on the schematic end of the content scale but also range from atomic to complex on the size scale (Langacker ibid.). In Figure 4 we map Croft and Cruse s examples and their matching traditional grammatical labels from Table 1 on to an adapted form of Langacker s two-dimensional scale of content and size.

5 233 Johnston and Ferrara Figure 3. Construction types along the size and content scales (reproduced from Langacker 2005) Figure 4. Example English constructions mapped onto the scales of size and content

6 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 234 Important for the discussion to follow is the part of the graph comprised of constructions that are more substantive than schematic and more complex than atomic, i.e. idioms. Idioms, as stated previously, are traditionally described as having both regular and idiosyncratic features. For this reason, idioms are relegated to the lexicon in generative grammar frameworks. However, within a cognitive grammar framework, idioms are not descriptively or analytically problematic because they simply represent different types of constructions that vary along the content and size scales of the lexicogrammar continuum (Langacker 1987). Furthermore, in this framework there is no assumption of componentiality and thus the unpredictable semantic structure of these composite structures is also unproblematic. For example, the English construction and idiom [kick-tns the bucket] is complex and mostly substantive except for the unspecified tense of the verb kick. Other English idioms may be fully substantive, e.g. [by and large] and [all of a sudden]. Still other idioms may be more schematic, e.g. [the X-er the Y-er] or [X let alone Y]. We now replace the English constructions with Auslan constructions on the graph of content and size (see Figure 5). Similar to the mapping of the English constructions, the more atomic and substantive Auslan units are placed in the area associated with the lexicon. This includes signs for Australia, green, lock, paper, and tomato. As constructions become more schematic and complex, they move towards the realm of traditional morphology and then syntax. Of interest here is the lack of constructions in Auslan that are complex yet more or less substantive. In other words, there seems to be virtually no multi-sign idioms in Auslan. This also appears to be the case in all known SLs. One of the very rare examples of a potential multi-sign idiom in Auslan (TRAIN GO SORRY the train has left, sorry or its variant PRO-2 MISS TRAIN you missed the train ) is actually borrowed from American Sign Language (ASL). The ASL idiom, in turn, appears to be a modified translation calque of the English to miss the boat. The ASL/Auslan idiom, like its apparently related English idiom, also means to miss out on something by being too late but is used in a more restricted way; namely it is used as response to a request for clarification about an on-going conversation from a new entrant to that conversation to mean you are too late and I am not inclined to repeat or recontextualize. We can see that this phrase has non-compositional meaning, because the overall meaning cannot be based on the signs TRAIN, GO, SORRY or MISS in combination alone. In the rest of this paper we use a cognitive grammar framework to examine if there is a place for idioms, or idiomaticity, in SLs nonetheless. However, before proceeding, we first need to clarify the status of the signs that can be found in any SL. Of course, comprehensive and detailed discussion of the question of sign types in SLs is beyond the scope of this paper. In the following section, we simply wish to summarise the main points as presented in Johnston and Schembri (1999, 2010) and Johnston (2010) regarding lexicalization in SLs insofar as they are needed to understand the ensuing discussion of lexical idioms (see Janzen, 2012, for a related discussion).

7 235 Johnston and Ferrara Figure 5. Example Auslan constructions mapped onto the scales of size and content 3. Sign Types in SLs: Partly- and Fully-lexicalized Signs We need to know if all signs are conventional symbolic units and, if so, if they are all conventional in the same way or to the same degree. In other words, we need clear a notion of what constitutes a token and the types that each may instantiate. On examination it is clear that the signs uttered when communicating in a SL are not all of the same type. Unproblematically, from one point of view, the atomic conventionalised units of a SL can be divided into the two broad classes, just as they can in SpLs: an open class of content (or lexical) signs/words and a closed class of function (or grammatical) signs/words. Discovering the language-specific conventionalized semantic content of these forms is an empirical question, requiring detailed textual analysis and fieldwork. From another point of view, however, there is a further sign/wordlevel distinction that needs to be made for SLs: a distinction between fullylexical signs and partly-lexical signs. In this second-order distinction, a fully-lexical sign is roughly equivalent to the commonsense notion of word generally used to refer to the atomic conventionalized form/meaning pairings found in a language (i.e. the free

8 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 236 morphemes). Of course, a fully-lexical sign may be either a content sign or a function sign. Fully-lexical signs constitute the listable lexicon of a SL in the strictest sense of the word. Partly-lexical signs, on the other hand, are signs that, though conventionalized at the level of the form and meaning of some of their formational features, do not have associated with them in any usage event a meaning that is additional to or unpredictable from the value of those components given that context. Lexicalization in SLs essentially occurs when a signed symbolic unit acquires a clearly identifiable and replicable citation form which is regularly and strongly associated with a meaning which is more specific than the sign s componential meaning potential, even when cited out of context; cannot be predicted based on these components alone; or is quite unrelated to its componential meaning potential, i.e., it may be arbitrary. Of course, this does not preclude the possibility that a sign may also be lexicalized instantaneously through the assertion, and acceptance, of any form/meaning link within a linguistic community. Finally, there exists a third category of sign type non-lexical signs. This includes gestures (both manual and non-manual) and other communicative bodily acts with are the least conventionalized in terms of form and meaning. They rely on context to be construed as signs in the first instance, let alone to be correctly interpreted. 4. Where have all the Idioms Gone? Given the recognized lack of traditional idioms in SLs, we may well ask if idiomaticity plays a significant role in SLs at all. From a construction grammar point of view and in the light of the distinction between partlylexical and fully-lexical signs just discussed, it would appear that idiomaticity does indeed have a significant role to play in the lexico-grammar of SLs because, as we explain in this section, idioms have not disappeared in SLs; rather, idiomaticity manifests itself in a different area of the language design space than for SpLs, as mapped by Langacker in Figure 3. While we explained above that SLs rarely have idioms in the traditional sense, the position we advance in this paper is that idiomaticity is actually found at the lexical level in SLs. Therefore, following Johnston and Schembri (2010) we propose that fully-lexical signs should be analysed as idioms or, better, lexical idioms in order to distinguish them from traditional multi-word idioms: [ ] there is a sense in which fully-lexicalised signs (constructions which are atomic and substantive) are idiomatic in SLs in much the same way as multiword idioms (constructions which are complex and substantive) can be idiomatic in SpLs and rarely in SLs [ ]. In other words, given the meaningfulness and/or iconicity of sign components, the idiosyncratic meaning of a fully-lexical sign suggests that lexicalization in SLs is similarly idiomatic. Lexical signs are in a sense idioms. (p ) To do this, we first suggest that partly-lexical signs in Auslan (and other SLs) are transparently complex structures. Then we describe how these signs may

9 237 Johnston and Ferrara become fully-lexical and that the resulting semantic structures of these signs are not straightforwardly predictable from component contributions. Next we describe how all fully-lexical signs, no matter what the provenance of the lexicalization in each case, have essentially two faces : the one as a unitary symbolic unit, the other as a construction in its own right. In other words, component symbolic units are latent in fully-lexical signs if lexicalized from partly-lexical signs, or they can be read into the sign s formational components despite the fact that this may bear no relation whatsoever to its overall symbolic value as a fully-lexical sign. 4.1 Partly-lexical Signs are Complex Structures In SpLs, the complexity of idiomatic expressions is manifested as a sequence of multiple linguistic units. For instance, the complex symbolic unit and idiom [paint the town red] is the integration of four atomic units in a particular order. If this assumption about complexity is extended to SLs, then as we have already noted most SLs have very few or no idioms in the traditional sense. However, the position advanced in this paper is that signs of SLs are different from words of SpLs, because they can contain complexity that stems from gestural or iconic sources. Such complexity is sub-atomic in size and involves the arrangement of bundles of formational/articulatory features (e.g. hand configuration, orientation, location, timing sequence, and other nonmanual features) both simultaneously and sequentially across one sign. Thus, what appear to be minimal symbolic units in SLs are actually themselves constructions because they, too, are composed of identifiable symbolic units that link forms to meanings. As we have seen, some are partly-lexical and some are fully-lexical. To accommodate this type of sign internal complexity, the scales of content and size along the lexico-grammar continuum need to be adjusted (Figure 6). Figure 6. Adjusted scales for size and content to accommodate the sub-atomic components of signs

10 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 238 We adopt Liddell s (2003a) model of real space blending to explain how parts of a sign may be meaningful and integrated to form a single complex sign. Real space blending is a type of mental space blending, or conceptual integration, that involves projecting elements from input mental spaces and elements from the signer s conceptualization of their immediate physical environment (real space), into another mental space, the blend, where parts of the signer, the space around the signer, and the signs themselves can be conceptualized as something else (Liddell 1995, 2003a, 2003b). iii Liddell (2003a) proposes the term depicting verbs for one group of blended signs and describes them as having lexically fixed features combined with additional meaningful, gradient aspects of form (p. 269). We suggest here that it is this group of signs that gives rise to lexical idioms. To present an example, the blend structure of a sign that depicts an instance of a person locking a door with a key is illustrated in Figure 7. Figure 7. Real space blend structure of to turn a small object against a flat surface The signer s right hand depicts a small object (or a person holding a small object) while the signer s left hand depicts a portion of a flat vertical surface, which prompts a conceptualization, via mappings between the input mental space, real space, and the blend, of a person locking a door with a key. iv While the same predication would require a complex expression involving multiple linguistic units in a SpL (consider the English expression [a person locks a door with a key]), a SL like Auslan can partially demonstrate and encode the same meaning with a single sign. Our view that depicting signs are complex stems from this ability to depict multiple blended elements and the relations between them. The description of partly-lexical signs given in section 3 above also applies to the signs Liddell (2003a) calls depicting verbs, because the components acquire meaning through correspondences in a real space blend. Extending Langacker s (1987) position on novel complex utterances in SpLs to partly-

11 239 Johnston and Ferrara lexical depicting signs, he explains that they are necessarily fully analyzable, because the speaker, in putting it together, must attend specifically to each component and its contribution to the desired composite sense (p. 297). This means that the semantic structure of a partly-lexical sign reflects each of the sub-atomic components in addition to the complete blend structure. The blend structure is an essential component of a partly-lexical sign s semantic structure, because it provides instructions for how correspondences are to be made between parts of the sign in real space to elements in the other input mental spaces. For instance, the form of the sign in Figure 7 could, depending the mental space input, depict a person unlocking a door with a key, screwing a nail into a wall, a person twisting a nail out of a bookcase, or turning a knob to turn on/off a light or appliance. We explain above that the meaning constructed by a partly-lexical depicting sign arises through the integration of linguistic and gestural components as they correspond to elements in the input mental spaces. Generally, linguistic status is given to the handshapes of these signs as they conventionally are used to depict certain types of things (Brennan 1992; Engberg-Pedersen 1993; Johnston 1989; Johnston and Schembri 1999; Schembri 1996). For example, in the (Auslan) sign above, the configuration of the right hand is one used to depict small objects (or a person holding a small object) while the configuration of the left hand depicts flat surfaces. Thus, we expect the sign s meaning to include a small object and a flat surface. As another example, in Auslan, a hand with index and middle fingers extended downward, palm facing the signer is conventionally used to depict a person s two legs. Depicting signs with this handshape then may be used to depict, for example, a person jumping, walking, laying down, etc. The gestural, or gradient, features of these signs (often related to the location and movement of the sign) also correspond to elements in the blend. They do so via iconic mappings. In the sign from Figure 7, the relation of the hands to each other as well as the movement of the right hand are the gradient features of this sign that map iconically onto the sign s semantic structure. As such, an Auslan signer would expect the spatial arrangement of the hands to iconically represent the arrangement of elements in the mental space input. That is, if the mental space element being depicted is indeed the surface of a door, then it would be unexpected to see a signer position a flat hand with the palm facing downward. In a similar fashion, we expect the timing and orientation of the sign to predicate a relation of turning, as this is what the signer is depicting. If in fact the intended predication involved the flipping of a switch on a wall, instead of the turning of a key in a door, a signer would have to indicate this by changing features to demonstrate that action. In a sign that depicts a person jumping, the general handshape (index and middle fingers spread and extended) and to some extent orientation (palm towards signer) are linguistically determined, although still motivated by iconicity. As the gestural components of the sign, the timing segments/movement, location features, and orientation features are determined by iconic mappings to partially show the predication. In this case, the movement of the hand up and down along with the fingers bending and straightening is meant to (partially) show a person jumping and bending his

12 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 240 legs at the knees. Because there are conventions for composing and interpreting the components of partly-lexical signs (guided by symbolic and iconic mappings), it is possible to identify a generalized semantic structure that is instantiated during usage events. Again, continuing with our example sign in Figure 7, a general meaning of this sign could be summarized as to turn a small object against a flat surface. This general and literal meaning is a result of more or less straightforward integration of the components of this sign. This meaning is not idiomatic but rather analyzable and predictable. However, in the next section we show that as partly-lexical depicting signs become conventionalized and entrenched within a signing community and thus become fully-lexical signs they exhibit idiomaticity. 4.2 From Partly- to Fully-lexical Signs: Lexicalization through Idiomaticity Langacker (1987, 2002, 2005) discusses lexicalization in cognitive grammar as a dual-process of entrenchment and conventionalization. Entrenchment is related to automatization, or the development of cognitive routines. With repeated use a novel linguistic structure becomes a pre-packaged assembly and thereby achieves what is called unit status. Once a unit, a language user can access and use the structure as a whole without having to attend specifically to its possibly complex internal composition. It is in this way that it constitutes an automatic cognitive routine. When a linguistic structure has reached unit status for a substantial number of language users, then it may be described as conventional linguistic unit and considered a part of a language s lexicon; it has been lexicalized. Johnston and Schembri (1999) explain that any sign can be lexicalized. And the partly-lexical signs just discussed are no exception. In fact, it has been suggested that the lexicalization of depicting signs is an important source for new signs across SLs (Brennan 1992; Johnston and Schembri 2007; Sandler & Lillo-Martin 2006; Schembri 2003; Schick 1990). In Auslan, there are numerous examples of signs that originated as partly-lexical signs. Take for instance the signs shown in Figure 8; all began as real-space depicting blends that over time lexicalized and came to act as a single meaningful units (Johnston and Schembri 2007: 174). It has been noted that once a partly-lexical depicting sign reaches unit status, its meanings that are originally general and compositional (as described in the above section) are backgrounded to those that are more specific and less predictable (Johnston and Schembri 2010). The lexical idiomatic reading is not only foregrounded it becomes the automatic default reading in most usage events. For example, the sign shown in Figure 7 that depicts turning a small object against a flat surface has been lexicalized in Auslan and now has the more specific meaning to lock or a key. And while the lexical meaning of this sign is motivated by its sub-atomic components, it cannot be predicted. It could just have easily been lexicalized with the meanings unlock, a door, or to screw, etc. In this sense it is idiomatic, not merely compositional. The sign

13 241 Johnston and Ferrara TICKET, shown above in Figure 8, provides another illustration of what we mean. Its use as a partly-lexical depicting sign prompts a literal reading of a smallish flat rectangular object that acquires a more specific meaning in context as part of a real-space depicting blend. Over time this sign has become conventionally associated with a set of more specific readings that include a card, a ticket, a receipt, or a sales docket. And even though it may seem rather obvious for this sign to be associated with these meanings, they are not the only possibilities. This sign could have been lexicalized to mean photograph or envelope or some other small rectangular object. And in fact, in ASL, one of the primary readings of this sign is a cheque/check, which also is congruent with the literal reading of this sign. However, in Auslan there is another sign that means a cheque, just as in ASL there is another sign meaning a ticket. Figure 8. Examples of lexicalized signs that originated as partly-lexical depicting signs (reproduced from Johnston and Schembri 2007) Another way the literal and general meanings of partly-lexical depicting signs become more specific and less predictable is through metaphorical or metonymical extension (Taub 2001; Janzen 2012). Take for example the Auslan sign THRILLED. This sign originated as a partly-lexical sign depicting a person jumping up and down. Then as it became lexicalized, the depiction of a person jumping up and down became associated with jumping up and down with excitement leading to one of its primary lexical readings to be thrilled or delighted. In other instances the same literal (partly-lexical) meaning of a form found in two or more unrelated sign languages has the same or identical idiomatic (fully-lexical) meaning (e.g., MEET in ASL and Auslan, see Figure 8). In both languages, however, the lexical idiom could have been agree, couple, partner, club or any number of non-predictable but metaphorically or metonymically related plausible meanings. Indeed, metonymic associations can easily produce a lexicalization that appears (and to all intents and purposes is) completely arbitrary to users of the language. For example, a depiction of meet/bump-into standing for the place I met you could become the fully-lexical sign the name of the city (in which is found the place I met you), which in turn could become the name of the country (in which is found the city in which is found the place I met you). The steps in the metonymic chain can be lost leaving a fully-lexical sign which is completely opaque and arbitrary. This is indeed what happened with the Auslan sign MELBOURNE which is the old sign for town.

14 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 242 Above we presented traditional multi-word idiomatic expressions as complex symbolic units whose meanings are unpredictable from their components. Then we justified the existence of lexical idioms in Auslan by explaining that partly-lexical depicting signs have a meaning which is predictable from their components (symbolic units) and the usage environment. If and when they become fully-lexical signs they acquire a default meaning which is not entirely predictable. It may even be arbitrary. In the following section, we address how signers can prompt either the componential meaning or the lexical meaning of these signs. 4.3 The Two Faces of Fully-lexical Signs (Lexical Idioms) From Fully- to Partly-lexical Signs: De-lexicalization In cognitive grammar, semantic structure is conceived of as encyclopedic and involves a structured network of related senses (Evans & Green 2006, Ch. 5). This view entails that the semantic structure of complex expressions includes all component structures, the relations between components and the resulting composite structure (Langacker 1987). Accordingly, the semantic structure of a sign which is a lexical idiom will contain an entrenched, schematized blend structure and related componential meanings alongside the meanings specific to the lexical unit as a whole. What is interesting here is that these signs never lose their componential structure; the componential structure just becomes backgrounded to the lexical associations. Thus, the components of a partly-lexical depicting sign are still retrievable if necessary after lexicalization. When needed a signer can at any time, using Johnston and Schembri s (1999) term, de-lexify (or delexicalize) the sign in context. De-lexicalization is a process of re-activating a sign s underlying component blend structure. In Figure 9 this process is represented superimposed on to the scales of size and content in the lexicogrammar continuum discussed above. Figure 9. The cycle of lexicalization and de-lexicalization

15 243 Johnston and Ferrara Consequently, the sign can be used by a signer to prompt either the componential, token meaning that arises in a usage event as part of a realspace blend or the idiomatic lexical meaning that is now conventionally associated with the sign. Janzen (2012) speaks of signs having a continuum of articulation possibilities from fully compositional to fully lexicalized and non-productive. We suggest here that these dual meanings, literal and lexical, exist because the semantic structure of a lexical idiom never loses its original blend structure. The different meanings simply relate to the salience afforded the blend structure in a given usage event. The lexical meaning will background the original blend structure while profiling the less predictable structure (e.g., the meaning of the sign LOCK as to lock or a key ). However, the blend can be (re)-activated when needed by the signer and will represent the use of the sign as a token symbolization of a specific, context-dependent conceptualization, i.e., a novel partly-lexical depicting sign. Figure 10 shows this type of reactivation. Figure 10. Illustration of a lexical sign's blend structure re-activated Thus, we suggest this sign signals a token depiction of a door being unlocked (if we assume that the reactivation of the blend involves similar inputs to the lexicalized sign) or, say, someone screwing a fastener into a wall, if the mental space inputs are different. In the latter case, the sign is not a result of reactivating the blend structure specific to the sign LOCK but rather a more general real space blend that has not yet developed into a lexicalized sign and just happens to share the same formational properties. In summary, partly-lexical depicting signs are composite structures and as such they are componential and analyzable. Different bundles of articulatory features are mapped onto mental space elements; some of these features are

16 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 244 regularly used for certain types of elements, the lexical part of the sign, and some are gestural/gradient features which map iconically. However as these partly-lexical signs become fully-lexical signs they are perceived as unitary structures rather than composite structures. Additionally, instances of these signs no longer represent mere tokens but now represent tokens of types (the lemma). Langacker (1987) speaks of this transition and the effect this process has on the meaning of composite structures in English: A novel expression is necessarily fully analyzable, because the speaker, in putting it together, must attend specifically to each component and its contribution to the desired composite sense. Once a complex expressions reaches unit status, however, there is at least the potential that its composite structure itself now a unit may be activated autonomously (i.e. without the supporting activation of the component structures). Certainly this does not happen immediately; the component structures and composite structures may continue to be co-activated indefinitely. It is nevertheless the composite structure that furnishes the primary content of an expression, the role of the component structures being subsidiary and dispensable. With continued use, then, a fixed expression will tend to gravitate towards the negative pole of the analyzability scale (though local reversals are possible), until its compositionality fades away entirely (p. 297). In contradistinction to the phenomenon in English, we maintain that the compositionality of signs never fades away entirely, even in fully-lexical signs. This link is maintained, we suggest, mostly through iconic mappings that motivate both the lexical and gestural inputs to these signs Fully-lexical Signs are Complex Structures too As described above, lexical idioms begin as partly-lexical depicting signs which then become idiomatic with lexicalization. However, not all signs originate as real space blends and, in these fully-lexical signs, there is no underlying blend structure that is associated with the lexical meaning of the sign. In normal usage events they are un-analyzable at a sub-atomic level. As a result, their meaning would not be seen as non-componential but rather simply the associated semantic pole of a lexical unit. For example, the sign JOKE did not arise as part of a real space blend (Figure 11). With respect to its conventional lexical meaning, the formational features cannot normally be divided into meaningful components, gestural or otherwise, like the sign LOCK can. That is, the bundle of features that includes the hand with thumb, index, and middle finger extended, the orientation of the palm to the side, the brushing movement just under the chin with the thumb, and the double arc movement away from the signer, do not together contribute (via correspondences in a real space blend) to the semantic structure of JOKE. They simply serve an articulatory function and constitute the phonological pole of this symbolic unit.

17 245 Johnston and Ferrara Figure 11. JOKE Figure 12. SISTER However, what distinguishes even these fully-lexical signs from atomic monomorphemic symbolic units in SpLs is that just like traditional multi-word idioms of SpLs they also have a possible literal interpretation, by which we mean a meaning that is predictable from, and congruent with, the sign s component symbolic units. Consider the English idiom [he kicked the bucket]. It has a default idiomatic reading (to die), but it also has a latent literal meaning that simply requires a suitable context to activate: He kicked the ladder, he kicked dog, and then he kicked the bucket. He was furious. Similarly, fully-lexical signs are constructions, and not simple atomic symbolic units ( mono-morphemic signs ), because the formational components of the sign are each potential symbolic units in their own right, even if none can be independently articulated. Of course, a suitably robust context is needed in order to force a literal interpretation of a fully-lexical sign that is not transparently derived from a partly-lexical sign or has no apparent link between its form and meaning, i.e. it appears to be completely arbitrary as in SISTER (Figure 12). (Recall, however, that every extant sign was probably iconically or gesturally motivated in some way in its original sense, even if semantic shifts through metaphor or metonymy completely obscure this fact in the current sense.) To force the abandonment of the default ( idiomatic ) lexical reading of this type of fullylexical sign to a contextually congruent literal meaning (e.g., SISTER as the depiction DS:TAP-ON-NOSE-WITH-HOOKED-OBJECT) one has to activate the right context of use. Of course, it may be very difficult to construct such a context of re-interpretation for some extremely common fully-lexical signs, but it is always, in principle, possible. Indeed, SL users do experience signs as having two faces as simultaneously unitary symbolic units and constructions in their own right. For instance, offering folk etymologies for almost any sign is quite irresistible for most signers. In other words, though a sign may actually be opaque with respect to iconicity it is arbitrary signers are often willing to hypothesise a gestural/iconic motivation, and hence origin, for almost all signs, even where historical evidence actually shows these folk etymologies to be quite false. This supports that our claim that even fully-lexical signs which have no apparent depicting origin or association can also be de-lexicalized. The latent presence of these symbolic units in fully-lexical signs is thus psychologically real for most signers.

18 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings Conclusion If our description of some lexical signs in SLs as idioms is correct, what does the existence of lexical idioms tell us about the sources of linguistic units and processes of meaning construction found in these languages? We suggest we are able to make non-lexical signs or parts of signs meaningful because we have a cognitive ability to construct real space blends. This ability, it should be noted, is not limited to users of SLs. Users of SpLs also construct real space blends while talking. For example, imagine a person telling a story about her commute to work one day. She says, And this crazy person came out of nowhere. He was speeding and weaving in and out of traffic. Very dangerous. While saying weaving in and out of traffic she also produces a manual gesture a flat hand with fingers extended, palm towards the side, moves forward and left to right a few times. It is apparent that during this usage event, the speaker constructs a real space blend where her hand is the car and the path is the weaving in and out. However, there is a key difference. In SpLs, gestures are never considered a source for new words, but in SLs, they are considered a source for new signs. Wilcox (2004) explains the reason is because gestures and SLs share articulatory and perceptual systems. For example, the gesture described above would probably be produced as a partly-lexical depicting sign by an Auslan user, as there is a conventional handshape for depicting vehicles. The gesture-sign interface that results from a shared modality and an ability to construct real space blends also explains why the signs discussed in this paper exhibit sub-atomic or internal complexity. Signers are adept at integrating multiple lexical and gestural elements within a single sign. The subsequent lexicalization of these signs provides further evidence that gesture is essential to the emergence of conventional symbolic units in SLs. Finally, the fact that it is often difficult to determine whether a sign belongs to the category of fully-, partly-, or non-lexical sign is precisely due to the prominent role gesture/iconicity and real space blending plays in SL grammar. For example, we mentioned how lexical idioms, which are conventional/fully-lexical signs, can be de-lexicalized in context by reactivating component blend structures prompting literal, non-idiomatic readings. The process of de-lexicalization in discourse gives SL users a powerful tool to modify and qualify conventional meaning non-lexically. It also works to re-strengthen connections to the gestural substrate and highlights the tight integration of gesture and language in Auslan, and most likely other SLs. Notes i Each Auslan sign referred to with an English gloss in this paper can be viewed as a video clip by visiting and typing the gloss in the search box. The sign

19 247 Johnston and Ferrara ii iii iv transcription of the phonological unit in Figure 1 is written in HamNoSys (Hamburg Notation System) from the Institute for German Sign Language, Hamburg University, Germany. See Table 1 for an explanation of the abbreviations and format. For an in-depth discussion of blending as a general cognitive process, see Fauconnier & Turner (2002) and Fauconnier (1997). Vertical brackets signal blended entities. References Brennan, M. (1992). The visual world of BSL: An introduction. In D. Brien (ed.), Dictionary of British Sign Language/ English. London and Boston: Faber and Faber. pp Croft, W. and A. Cruse (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Engberg-Pedersen, E. (1993). Space in Danish Sign Language: The semantics and Morphosyntax of the Use of Space in a Visual Language. Hamburg: Signum Press. Evans, V. and M. Green (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fauconnier, G. and M. Turner (2002). The Way we Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind's Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books. Fillmore, C.J., P. Kay and M.C. O Connor (1988). Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64: Janzen, T. (2012). Lexicalization and grammaticalization. In R. Pfau, M. Steinbach and B. Woll (eds.), Sign Languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp Johnston, T. (1989). Auslan Dictionary: A Dictionary of the Sign Language of the Australian Deaf Community. Sydney: Deafness Resources Australia. Johnston, T. (2010). Degree, not kind: Non-lexicalized points are symbolic indexicals regardless of whether they occur in the composite utterances of spoken languages or signed languages. 4th Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies (SGS). European University Viadrina Frankfurt/Oder. Johnston, T. and A. Schembri (1999). On defining lexeme in a signed language. Sign Language and Linguistics 2: Johnston, T. and A. Schembri (2007). Australian Sign Language: An Introduction to Sign Language Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Johnston, T. and A. Schembri (2010). Variation, lexicalization and grammaticalization in signed languages. Langage et société 131: Langacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Volume 1: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

20 Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 248 Langacker, R.W. (2002). Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.. Langacker, R.W. (2005). Construction Grammars: Cognitive, radical, and less so. In F.J.R.D.M. Ibanez and M.S.P. Cervel (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: Internal Dynamics and Interdisciplinary Interaction. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp Liddell, S. (1995). Real, surrogate, and token space: Grammatical consequences in ASL. In K. Emmorey and J. Reilly (eds.), Language, Gesture, and Space. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp Liddell, S. (2003a). Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language. New York: Cambridge University Press. Liddell, S. (2003b). Sources of meaning in ASL classifier predicates. In K. Emmorey (ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp Sandler, W. and D. Lillo-Martin (2006). Sign Language and Linguistic Universals. New York: Cambridge University Press. Schembri, A. (1996). The Structure and Formation of Signs in Auslan (Australian Sign Language). Sydney: North Rocks. Schembri, A. (2003). Rethinking 'classifiers' in signed language. In K. Emmorey (ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages. Mahwah, NH: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp Schick, B.S. (1990). Classifier predicates in American Sign Language. International Journal of Sign Linguistics 1: Wilcox, S. (2004). Gesture and language: Cross-linguistic and historical data from signed languages. Gesture 4:

Sign Language Linguistics Course texts Overview Assessment Week 1: Introduction and history of sign language research

Sign Language Linguistics Course texts Overview Assessment Week 1: Introduction and history of sign language research Sign Language Linguistics 27910/37910 Instructor: Jordan Fenlon (jfenlon@uchicago.edu) Office hours: Thursdays, 10:45-11:45 Winter quarter: Tuesdays and Thursdays, 9:00-10:20 The course introduces students

More information

KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION

KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION Gabi Reinmann Germany reinmann.gabi@googlemail.com Synonyms Information organization, information classification, knowledge representation, knowledge structuring Definition The term

More information

Depth-of-Knowledge Levels for Four Content Areas Norman L. Webb March 28, 2002. Reading (based on Wixson, 1999)

Depth-of-Knowledge Levels for Four Content Areas Norman L. Webb March 28, 2002. Reading (based on Wixson, 1999) Depth-of-Knowledge Levels for Four Content Areas Norman L. Webb March 28, 2002 Language Arts Levels of Depth of Knowledge Interpreting and assigning depth-of-knowledge levels to both objectives within

More information

A Survey of ASL Tenses

A Survey of ASL Tenses A Survey of ASL Tenses Karen Alkoby DePaul University School of Computer Science Chicago, IL kalkoby@shrike.depaul.edu Abstract This paper examines tenses in American Sign Language (ASL), which will be

More information

From Gesture to Sign Language: Conventionalization of Classifier Constructions by Adult Hearing Learners of British Sign Language

From Gesture to Sign Language: Conventionalization of Classifier Constructions by Adult Hearing Learners of British Sign Language Topics in Cognitive Science (2014) 1 20 Copyright 2014 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN:1756-8757 print / 1756-8765 online DOI: 10.1111/tops.12118 From Gesture to Sign Language:

More information

Culture and Language. What We Say Influences What We Think, What We Feel and What We Believe

Culture and Language. What We Say Influences What We Think, What We Feel and What We Believe Culture and Language What We Say Influences What We Think, What We Feel and What We Believe Unique Human Ability Ability to create and use language is the most distinctive feature of humans Humans learn

More information

Morphology. Morphology is the study of word formation, of the structure of words. 1. some words can be divided into parts which still have meaning

Morphology. Morphology is the study of word formation, of the structure of words. 1. some words can be divided into parts which still have meaning Morphology Morphology is the study of word formation, of the structure of words. Some observations about words and their structure: 1. some words can be divided into parts which still have meaning 2. many

More information

Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language

Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language Scott K. Liddell Department of Linguistics and Interpretation Gallaudet University Washington, DC PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

Langue and Parole. John Phillips

Langue and Parole. John Phillips 1 Langue and Parole John Phillips The distinction between the French words, langue (language or tongue) and parole (speech), enters the vocabulary of theoretical linguistics with Ferdinand de Saussure

More information

Language Meaning and Use

Language Meaning and Use Language Meaning and Use Raymond Hickey, English Linguistics Website: www.uni-due.de/ele Types of meaning There are four recognisable types of meaning: lexical meaning, grammatical meaning, sentence meaning

More information

Arguments and Dialogues

Arguments and Dialogues ONE Arguments and Dialogues The three goals of critical argumentation are to identify, analyze, and evaluate arguments. The term argument is used in a special sense, referring to the giving of reasons

More information

Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments

Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments PAAS, RENKL, INTRODUCTION SWELLER EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 38(1), 1 4 Copyright 2003, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments Fred Paas Educational

More information

Problem of the Month: Cutting a Cube

Problem of the Month: Cutting a Cube Problem of the Month: The Problems of the Month (POM) are used in a variety of ways to promote problem solving and to foster the first standard of mathematical practice from the Common Core State Standards:

More information

Collated Food Requirements. Received orders. Resolved orders. 4 Check for discrepancies * Unmatched orders

Collated Food Requirements. Received orders. Resolved orders. 4 Check for discrepancies * Unmatched orders Introduction to Data Flow Diagrams What are Data Flow Diagrams? Data Flow Diagrams (DFDs) model that perspective of the system that is most readily understood by users the flow of information around the

More information

Discourse Markers in English Writing

Discourse Markers in English Writing Discourse Markers in English Writing Li FENG Abstract Many devices, such as reference, substitution, ellipsis, and discourse marker, contribute to a discourse s cohesion and coherence. This paper focuses

More information

1/9. Locke 1: Critique of Innate Ideas

1/9. Locke 1: Critique of Innate Ideas 1/9 Locke 1: Critique of Innate Ideas This week we are going to begin looking at a new area by turning our attention to the work of John Locke, who is probably the most famous English philosopher of all

More information

TEACHER IDENTITY AND DIALOGUE: A COMMENT ON VAN RIJSWIJK, AKKERMAN & KOSTER. Willem Wardekker VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands

TEACHER IDENTITY AND DIALOGUE: A COMMENT ON VAN RIJSWIJK, AKKERMAN & KOSTER. Willem Wardekker VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands International Journal for Dialogical Science Spring 2013. Vol. 7, No. 1, 61-65 Copyright 2013 by Willem Wardekker TEACHER IDENTITY AND DIALOGUE: A COMMENT ON VAN RIJSWIJK, AKKERMAN & KOSTER Willem Wardekker

More information

2 P age. www.deafeducation.vic.edu.au

2 P age. www.deafeducation.vic.edu.au Building Connections Between the Signed and Written Language of Signing Deaf Children Michelle Baker & Michelle Stark In research relating to writing and deaf students there is a larger body of work that

More information

Cohesive writing 1. Conjunction: linking words What is cohesive writing?

Cohesive writing 1. Conjunction: linking words What is cohesive writing? Cohesive writing 1. Conjunction: linking words What is cohesive writing? Cohesive writing is writing which holds together well. It is easy to follow because it uses language effectively to guide the reader.

More information

Language Development and Learning: Strategies for supporting pupils learning through English as an additional language (Secondary)

Language Development and Learning: Strategies for supporting pupils learning through English as an additional language (Secondary) 12 CHECKLIST FOR PLANNING 1. Do the bilingual pupils understand the learning intention? 2. Do I know their prior knowledge of this topic? 3. Do I know what English language skills they have and need to

More information

Writing learning objectives

Writing learning objectives Writing learning objectives This material was excerpted and adapted from the following web site: http://www.utexas.edu/academic/diia/assessment/iar/students/plan/objectives/ What is a learning objective?

More information

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages Syuzanna Mejlumyan Yerevan State Linguistic University Abstract It has been five years since the Korean language has been taught at Yerevan State

More information

Keywords academic writing phraseology dissertations online support international students

Keywords academic writing phraseology dissertations online support international students Phrasebank: a University-wide Online Writing Resource John Morley, Director of Academic Support Programmes, School of Languages, Linguistics and Cultures, The University of Manchester Summary A salient

More information

The Double Lives of Objects: An Essay in the Metaphysics of the Ordinary World

The Double Lives of Objects: An Essay in the Metaphysics of the Ordinary World 252 The Double Lives of Objects: An Essay in the Metaphysics of the Ordinary World, by Thomas Sattig. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2015, 288 pages, ISBN 9780199683017 (hbk). In The Double Lives of

More information

Noam Chomsky: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax notes

Noam Chomsky: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax notes Noam Chomsky: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax notes Julia Krysztofiak May 16, 2006 1 Methodological preliminaries 1.1 Generative grammars as theories of linguistic competence The study is concerned with

More information

A GUIDE TO LABORATORY REPORT WRITING ILLINOIS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY THE COLLEGE WRITING PROGRAM

A GUIDE TO LABORATORY REPORT WRITING ILLINOIS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY THE COLLEGE WRITING PROGRAM AT THE ILLINOIS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY THE COLLEGE WRITING PROGRAM www.iit.edu/~writer writer@charlie.cns.iit.edu FALL 1999 Table of Contents Table of Contents... 2 Introduction... 3 Need for Report Writing...

More information

Historical Linguistics. Diachronic Analysis. Two Approaches to the Study of Language. Kinds of Language Change. What is Historical Linguistics?

Historical Linguistics. Diachronic Analysis. Two Approaches to the Study of Language. Kinds of Language Change. What is Historical Linguistics? Historical Linguistics Diachronic Analysis What is Historical Linguistics? Historical linguistics is the study of how languages change over time and of their relationships with other languages. All languages

More information

the primary emphasis on explanation in terms of factors outside the formal structure of language.

the primary emphasis on explanation in terms of factors outside the formal structure of language. Grammar: Functional Approaches William Croft MSC03 2130, Linguistics 1 University of New Mexico Albuquerque NM 87131-0001 USA Abstract The term functional or functionalist has been applied to any approach

More information

Intending, Intention, Intent, Intentional Action, and Acting Intentionally: Comments on Knobe and Burra

Intending, Intention, Intent, Intentional Action, and Acting Intentionally: Comments on Knobe and Burra Intending, Intention, Intent, Intentional Action, and Acting Intentionally: Comments on Knobe and Burra Gilbert Harman Department of Philosophy Princeton University November 30, 2005 It is tempting to

More information

The College at Brockport: State University of New York 2013-2014

The College at Brockport: State University of New York 2013-2014 The College at Brockport: State University of New York 2013-2014 Compiled by: This interactive guide provides information and resources important to the academic language component of the edtpa. Dr. Sandra

More information

Gesture and ASL L2 Acquisition 1

Gesture and ASL L2 Acquisition 1 Sign Languages: spinning and unraveling the past, present and future. TISLR9, forty five papers and three posters from the 9th. Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research Conference, Florianopolis, Brazil,

More information

164 music theory spectrum 30 (2008)

164 music theory spectrum 30 (2008) 164 music theory spectrum 30 (2008) Lewin, Intervals, and Transformations: a Comment on Hook dmitri tymoczko Keywords: David Lewin, Julian Hook, interval, pitch class, group theory, distance This paper

More information

French Language and Culture. Curriculum Framework 2011 2012

French Language and Culture. Curriculum Framework 2011 2012 AP French Language and Culture Curriculum Framework 2011 2012 Contents (click on a topic to jump to that page) Introduction... 3 Structure of the Curriculum Framework...4 Learning Objectives and Achievement

More information

Active Listening. Learning Objectives. By the end of this module, the learner will have

Active Listening. Learning Objectives. By the end of this module, the learner will have 1 Active Listening Learning Objectives By the end of this module, the learner will have An understanding of what is meant by active listening Gained insight into your natural listening strengths and areas

More information

Inglés IV (B-2008) Prof. Argenis A. Zapata

Inglés IV (B-2008) Prof. Argenis A. Zapata Inglés IV (B-2008) FORMAL ENGLISH - It is used in academic writing (e.g., essays, reports, resumes, theses, and the like), and formal social events such as public speeches, graduation ceremonies, and assemblies

More information

Ohio Early Learning and Development Standards Domain: Language and Literacy Development

Ohio Early Learning and Development Standards Domain: Language and Literacy Development Ohio Early Learning and Development Standards Domain: Language and Literacy Development Strand: Listening and Speaking Topic: Receptive Language and Comprehension Infants Young Toddlers (Birth - 8 months)

More information

Forward exchange rates

Forward exchange rates Forward exchange rates The forex market consists of two distinct markets - the spot foreign exchange market (in which currencies are bought and sold for delivery within two working days) and the forward

More information

Understanding sign language classifiers through a polycomponential approach

Understanding sign language classifiers through a polycomponential approach Lingua 117 (2007) 1159 1168 www.elsevier.com/locate/lingua Introduction Understanding sign language classifiers through a polycomponential approach Gary Morgan a, *, Bencie Woll b a Department of Language

More information

CODING THE NATURE OF THINKING DISPLAYED IN RESPONSES ON NETS OF SOLIDS

CODING THE NATURE OF THINKING DISPLAYED IN RESPONSES ON NETS OF SOLIDS CODING THE NATURE OF THINKING DISPLAYED IN RESPONSES ON NETS OF SOLIDS Christine Lawrie: Centre for Cognition Research in Learning and Teaching, UNE, Australia John Pegg: Centre for Cognition Research

More information

TRU Math Conversation Guide

TRU Math Conversation Guide Release Version Alpha TRU Math Conversation Guide Module A: Contextual Algebraic Tasks This TRU Math Conversation Guide, Module A: Contextual Algebraic Tasks is a product of The Algebra Teaching Study

More information

Critical Analysis So what does that REALLY mean?

Critical Analysis So what does that REALLY mean? Critical Analysis So what does that REALLY mean? 1 The words critically analyse can cause panic in students when they first turn over their examination paper or are handed their assignment questions. Why?

More information

Syntactic Theory. Background and Transformational Grammar. Dr. Dan Flickinger & PD Dr. Valia Kordoni

Syntactic Theory. Background and Transformational Grammar. Dr. Dan Flickinger & PD Dr. Valia Kordoni Syntactic Theory Background and Transformational Grammar Dr. Dan Flickinger & PD Dr. Valia Kordoni Department of Computational Linguistics Saarland University October 28, 2011 Early work on grammar There

More information

An Investigation through Different Types of Bilinguals and Bilingualism Hamzeh Moradi Abstract Keywords:

An Investigation through Different Types of Bilinguals and Bilingualism Hamzeh Moradi Abstract Keywords: International Journal of Humanities & Social Science Studies (IJHSSS) A Peer-Reviewed Bi-monthly Bi-lingual Research Journal ISSN: 2349-6959 (Online), ISSN: 2349-6711 (Print) Volume-I, Issue-II, September

More information

Chapter 3. Data Flow Diagrams

Chapter 3. Data Flow Diagrams Chapter 3. Data Flow Diagrams Table of Contents Objectives... 1 Introduction to Data Flow Diagrams... 2 What are Data Flow Diagrams?... 2 An example Data Flow Diagram... 2 The benefits of Data Flow Diagrams...

More information

Teaching Vocabulary to Young Learners (Linse, 2005, pp. 120-134)

Teaching Vocabulary to Young Learners (Linse, 2005, pp. 120-134) Teaching Vocabulary to Young Learners (Linse, 2005, pp. 120-134) Very young children learn vocabulary items related to the different concepts they are learning. When children learn numbers or colors in

More information

Reading Competencies

Reading Competencies Reading Competencies The Third Grade Reading Guarantee legislation within Senate Bill 21 requires reading competencies to be adopted by the State Board no later than January 31, 2014. Reading competencies

More information

Quine on truth by convention

Quine on truth by convention Quine on truth by convention March 8, 2005 1 Linguistic explanations of necessity and the a priori.............. 1 2 Relative and absolute truth by definition.................... 2 3 Is logic true by convention?...........................

More information

Things to remember when transcribing speech

Things to remember when transcribing speech Notes and discussion Things to remember when transcribing speech David Crystal University of Reading Until the day comes when this journal is available in an audio or video format, we shall have to rely

More information

University of Massachusetts Boston Applied Linguistics Graduate Program. APLING 601 Introduction to Linguistics. Syllabus

University of Massachusetts Boston Applied Linguistics Graduate Program. APLING 601 Introduction to Linguistics. Syllabus University of Massachusetts Boston Applied Linguistics Graduate Program APLING 601 Introduction to Linguistics Syllabus Course Description: This course examines the nature and origin of language, the history

More information

Register Differences between Prefabs in Native and EFL English

Register Differences between Prefabs in Native and EFL English Register Differences between Prefabs in Native and EFL English MARIA WIKTORSSON 1 Introduction In the later stages of EFL (English as a Foreign Language) learning, and foreign language learning in general,

More information

Overview of the TACITUS Project

Overview of the TACITUS Project Overview of the TACITUS Project Jerry R. Hobbs Artificial Intelligence Center SRI International 1 Aims of the Project The specific aim of the TACITUS project is to develop interpretation processes for

More information

Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials

Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials Sample Size and Power in Clinical Trials Version 1.0 May 011 1. Power of a Test. Factors affecting Power 3. Required Sample Size RELATED ISSUES 1. Effect Size. Test Statistics 3. Variation 4. Significance

More information

Business School Writing an Essay

Business School Writing an Essay Business School Writing an Essay What is an essay? An essay is usually written in response to a question or series of questions. The writer's purpose is to convince the reader that his or her way of analysing,

More information

A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English. with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs

A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English. with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs A Comparative Analysis of Standard American English and British English with respect to the Auxiliary Verbs Andrea Muru Texas Tech University 1. Introduction Within any given language variations exist

More information

CAMBRIDGE FIRST CERTIFICATE Listening and Speaking NEW EDITION. Sue O Connell with Louise Hashemi

CAMBRIDGE FIRST CERTIFICATE Listening and Speaking NEW EDITION. Sue O Connell with Louise Hashemi CAMBRIDGE FIRST CERTIFICATE SKILLS Series Editor: Sue O Connell CAMBRIDGE FIRST CERTIFICATE Listening and Speaking NEW EDITION Sue O Connell with Louise Hashemi PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE

More information

Problem of the Month: Fair Games

Problem of the Month: Fair Games Problem of the Month: The Problems of the Month (POM) are used in a variety of ways to promote problem solving and to foster the first standard of mathematical practice from the Common Core State Standards:

More information

Strand: Reading Literature Topics Standard I can statements Vocabulary Key Ideas and Details

Strand: Reading Literature Topics Standard I can statements Vocabulary Key Ideas and Details Strand: Reading Literature Key Ideas and Craft and Structure Integration of Knowledge and Ideas RL.K.1. With prompting and support, ask and answer questions about key details in a text RL.K.2. With prompting

More information

Massachusetts Tests for Educator Licensure

Massachusetts Tests for Educator Licensure Massachusetts Tests for Educator Licensure FIELD 90: FOUNDATIONS OF READING TEST OBJECTIVES Subarea Multiple-Choice Range of Objectives Approximate Test Weighting I. Foundations of Reading Development

More information

Elasticity. I. What is Elasticity?

Elasticity. I. What is Elasticity? Elasticity I. What is Elasticity? The purpose of this section is to develop some general rules about elasticity, which may them be applied to the four different specific types of elasticity discussed in

More information

Reading Instruction and Reading Achievement Among ELL Students

Reading Instruction and Reading Achievement Among ELL Students Research Into Practice READING Reading Instruction and Reading Achievement Among ELL Students Principles of ELL Reading Instruction Some very straightforward principles, directly supported by research,

More information

Why use a visual schedule?

Why use a visual schedule? Why use a visual schedule? As teachers or parents working with children with disabilities, we often assume that the child knows the daily schedule. Do they really know? Think how lost you would be if someone

More information

Comparison of the Cambridge Exams main suite, IELTS and TOEFL

Comparison of the Cambridge Exams main suite, IELTS and TOEFL Comparison of the Cambridge Exams main suite, IELTS and TOEFL This guide is intended to help teachers and consultants advise students on which exam to take by making a side-by-side comparison. Before getting

More information

Revisiting Foreign Language Teaching and Testing in the Light of Cognitive Linguistics

Revisiting Foreign Language Teaching and Testing in the Light of Cognitive Linguistics International Journal of Educational Investigations Available online @ www.ijeionline.com Vol.2, No.3: 42-46, 2015 (March) ISSN: 2410-3446 Revisiting Foreign Language Teaching and Testing in the Light

More information

Perspective taking strategies in Turkish Sign Language and Croatian Sign Language

Perspective taking strategies in Turkish Sign Language and Croatian Sign Language Perspective taking strategies in Turkish Sign Language and Croatian Sign Language Engin Arik Purdue University Marina Milković University of Zagreb 1 Introduction Space is one of the basic domains of human

More information

Strand: Reading Literature Topics Standard I can statements Vocabulary Key Ideas and Details

Strand: Reading Literature Topics Standard I can statements Vocabulary Key Ideas and Details Strand: Reading Literature Key Ideas and Details Craft and Structure RL.3.1 Ask and answer questions to demonstrate understanding of a text, referring explicitly to the text as the basis for the answers.

More information

Introduction to Semantics. A Case Study in Semantic Fieldwork: Modality in Tlingit

Introduction to Semantics. A Case Study in Semantic Fieldwork: Modality in Tlingit A Case Study in Semantic Fieldwork: Modality in Tlingit In this handout, I ll walk you step-by-step through one small part of a semantic fieldwork project on an understudied language: Tlingit, a Na-Dene

More information

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations September, 2002 (Revised January, 2006) Icek Ajzen Brief Description of the Theory of Planned Behavior According to the theory

More information

7.5 Emphatic Verb Tense

7.5 Emphatic Verb Tense Chapter 7 Verb Usage 211 7.5 Emphatic Verb Tense The emphatic tenses of a verb are used to add emphasis. In addition, the emphatic tense can be used with the word not in negative sentences and to form

More information

How To Teach English To Other People

How To Teach English To Other People TESOL / NCATE Program Standards STANDARDS FOR THE ACCREDIATION OF INITIAL PROGRAMS IN P 12 ESL TEACHER EDUCATION Prepared and Developed by the TESOL Task Force on ESL Standards for P 12 Teacher Education

More information

Planning and Writing Essays

Planning and Writing Essays Planning and Writing Essays Many of your coursework assignments will take the form of an essay. This leaflet will give you an overview of the basic stages of planning and writing an academic essay but

More information

A Beautiful Four Days in Berlin Takafumi Maekawa (Ryukoku University) maekawa@soc.ryukoku.ac.jp

A Beautiful Four Days in Berlin Takafumi Maekawa (Ryukoku University) maekawa@soc.ryukoku.ac.jp A Beautiful Four Days in Berlin Takafumi Maekawa (Ryukoku University) maekawa@soc.ryukoku.ac.jp 1. The Data This paper presents an analysis of such noun phrases as in (1) within the framework of Head-driven

More information

To download the script for the listening go to: http://www.teachingenglish.org.uk/sites/teacheng/files/learning-stylesaudioscript.

To download the script for the listening go to: http://www.teachingenglish.org.uk/sites/teacheng/files/learning-stylesaudioscript. Learning styles Topic: Idioms Aims: - To apply listening skills to an audio extract of non-native speakers - To raise awareness of personal learning styles - To provide concrete learning aids to enable

More information

The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Significance of the Lexicon. Australian? The Significance of the Lexicon 澳 大 利 亚 奥 地 利

The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Lexicon. The Significance of the Lexicon. Australian? The Significance of the Lexicon 澳 大 利 亚 奥 地 利 The significance of the lexicon Lexical knowledge Lexical skills 2 The significance of the lexicon Lexical knowledge The Significance of the Lexicon Lexical errors lead to misunderstanding. There s that

More information

Developing Academic Language Skills to Support Reading and Writing. Kenna Rodgers February, 2015 IVC Series

Developing Academic Language Skills to Support Reading and Writing. Kenna Rodgers February, 2015 IVC Series Developing Academic Language Skills to Support Reading and Writing Kenna Rodgers February, 2015 IVC Series Academic Language: What is it? the language of school. -Jim Cummins the hidden curriculum of school.

More information

FROM NUMERICAL EQUIVALENCE TO ALGEBRAIC EQUIVALENCE 1. Rolene Liebenberg, Marlene Sasman and Alwyn Olivier

FROM NUMERICAL EQUIVALENCE TO ALGEBRAIC EQUIVALENCE 1. Rolene Liebenberg, Marlene Sasman and Alwyn Olivier FROM NUMERICAL EQUIVALENCE TO ALGEBRAIC EQUIVALENCE 1 Rolene Liebenberg, Marlene Sasman and Alwyn Olivier Mathematics Learning and Teaching Initiative (MALATI) In this paper we describe Malati s approach

More information

ELPS TELPAS. Proficiency Level Descriptors

ELPS TELPAS. Proficiency Level Descriptors ELPS TELPAS Proficiency Level Descriptors Permission to copy the ELPS TELPAS Proficiency Level Descriptors is hereby extended to Texas school officials and their agents for their exclusive use in determining

More information

Comparing Methods to Identify Defect Reports in a Change Management Database

Comparing Methods to Identify Defect Reports in a Change Management Database Comparing Methods to Identify Defect Reports in a Change Management Database Elaine J. Weyuker, Thomas J. Ostrand AT&T Labs - Research 180 Park Avenue Florham Park, NJ 07932 (weyuker,ostrand)@research.att.com

More information

Likewise, we have contradictions: formulas that can only be false, e.g. (p p).

Likewise, we have contradictions: formulas that can only be false, e.g. (p p). CHAPTER 4. STATEMENT LOGIC 59 The rightmost column of this truth table contains instances of T and instances of F. Notice that there are no degrees of contingency. If both values are possible, the formula

More information

1 Introduction to morphology and syntax

1 Introduction to morphology and syntax 1 Introduction to morphology and syntax If you were to ask anyone the question What is language? you would probably receive an answer that includes the word communication. Most of us, if we think about

More information

Representing Geography

Representing Geography 3 Representing Geography OVERVIEW This chapter introduces the concept of representation, or the construction of a digital model of some aspect of the Earth s surface. The geographic world is extremely

More information

A. Schedule: Reading, problem set #2, midterm. B. Problem set #1: Aim to have this for you by Thursday (but it could be Tuesday)

A. Schedule: Reading, problem set #2, midterm. B. Problem set #1: Aim to have this for you by Thursday (but it could be Tuesday) Lecture 5: Fallacies of Clarity Vagueness and Ambiguity Philosophy 130 September 23, 25 & 30, 2014 O Rourke I. Administrative A. Schedule: Reading, problem set #2, midterm B. Problem set #1: Aim to have

More information

Three domains of learning

Three domains of learning Three domains of learning Leslie Owen Wilson all rights reserved Three domains of learning What are the differences between the cognitive, affective, and psychomotor taxonomies? There are three main domains

More information

A LEVEL ECONOMICS. ECON1/Unit 1 Markets and Market Failure Mark scheme. 2140 June 2014. Version 0.1 Final

A LEVEL ECONOMICS. ECON1/Unit 1 Markets and Market Failure Mark scheme. 2140 June 2014. Version 0.1 Final A LEVEL ECONOMICS ECON1/Unit 1 Markets and Market Failure Mark scheme 2140 June 2014 Version 0.1 Final Mark schemes are prepared by the Lead Assessment Writer and considered, together with the relevant

More information

Managing Variability in Software Architectures 1 Felix Bachmann*

Managing Variability in Software Architectures 1 Felix Bachmann* Managing Variability in Software Architectures Felix Bachmann* Carnegie Bosch Institute Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, Pa 523, USA fb@sei.cmu.edu Len Bass Software Engineering Institute Carnegie

More information

Unit: Fever, Fire and Fashion Term: Spring 1 Year: 5

Unit: Fever, Fire and Fashion Term: Spring 1 Year: 5 Unit: Fever, Fire and Fashion Term: Spring 1 Year: 5 English Fever, Fire and Fashion Unit Summary In this historical Unit pupils learn about everyday life in London during the 17 th Century. Frost fairs,

More information

Welcome to the topic on creating key performance indicators in SAP Business One, release 9.1 version for SAP HANA.

Welcome to the topic on creating key performance indicators in SAP Business One, release 9.1 version for SAP HANA. Welcome to the topic on creating key performance indicators in SAP Business One, release 9.1 version for SAP HANA. 1 In this topic, you will learn how to: Use Key Performance Indicators (also known as

More information

COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH. FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr

COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH. FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr COMPARATIVES WITHOUT DEGREES: A NEW APPROACH FRIEDERIKE MOLTMANN IHPST, Paris fmoltmann@univ-paris1.fr It has become common to analyse comparatives by using degrees, so that John is happier than Mary would

More information

Alignment of the National Standards for Learning Languages with the Common Core State Standards

Alignment of the National Standards for Learning Languages with the Common Core State Standards Alignment of the National with the Common Core State Standards Performance Expectations The Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts (ELA) and Literacy in History/Social Studies, Science,

More information

American Sign Language From a Psycholinguistic and Cultural Perspective Spring 2012 Syllabus Linguistics 242

American Sign Language From a Psycholinguistic and Cultural Perspective Spring 2012 Syllabus Linguistics 242 American Sign Language From a Psycholinguistic and Cultural Perspective Spring 2012 Syllabus Linguistics 242 (This syllabus is a guide that may be modified by the instructor as the semester progresses,

More information

In Defense of Kantian Moral Theory Nader Shoaibi University of California, Berkeley

In Defense of Kantian Moral Theory Nader Shoaibi University of California, Berkeley In Defense of Kantian Moral Theory University of California, Berkeley In this paper, I will argue that Kant provides us with a plausible account of morality. To show that, I will first offer a major criticism

More information

(Refer Slide Time: 2:03)

(Refer Slide Time: 2:03) Control Engineering Prof. Madan Gopal Department of Electrical Engineering Indian Institute of Technology, Delhi Lecture - 11 Models of Industrial Control Devices and Systems (Contd.) Last time we were

More information

JOINT ATTENTION. Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010

JOINT ATTENTION. Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010 JOINT ATTENTION Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010 1 1 1.Outline 2.Joint attention - an informal approximation 3.Motivation of the paper 4.Formalization of

More information

6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence

6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence 222 CHAPTER 6. PROBABILITY 6.3 Conditional Probability and Independence Conditional Probability Two cubical dice each have a triangle painted on one side, a circle painted on two sides and a square painted

More information

Chapter 5. The Sensual and Perceptual Theories of Visual Communication

Chapter 5. The Sensual and Perceptual Theories of Visual Communication Chapter 5. The Sensual and Perceptual Theories of Visual Communication Sensual Theories of Visual Communication Gestalt & Constructivism Gestalt=form or shape Max Wertheimer (1910) the whole is different

More information

SYNTHETIC SIGNING FOR THE DEAF: esign

SYNTHETIC SIGNING FOR THE DEAF: esign SYNTHETIC SIGNING FOR THE DEAF: esign Inge Zwitserlood, Margriet Verlinden, Johan Ros, Sanny van der Schoot Department of Research and Development, Viataal, Theerestraat 42, NL-5272 GD Sint-Michielsgestel,

More information

Grade 4 Writing Curriculum Map

Grade 4 Writing Curriculum Map Curriculum Map BOE Approval November 2013 CURRICULUM MAP WRITING GRADE 4 SEPTEMBER OCTOBER NOVEMBER DECEMBER JANUARY Creating a Buzz About : It s All About Me! Tell Me A Story Realistic Fiction Tell Me

More information

DWG 001. Blueprint Reading. Line Standards Drawing Symbols. Instructor Guide

DWG 001. Blueprint Reading. Line Standards Drawing Symbols. Instructor Guide DWG 001 Blueprint Reading Line Standards Drawing Symbols Instructor Guide Module Purpose Introduction The purpose of the Blueprint Reading modules is to introduce students to production drawings and blueprint

More information

Francis Y. LIN Alex X. PENG (School of Foreign Languages and Literatures / Beijing Normal University)

Francis Y. LIN Alex X. PENG (School of Foreign Languages and Literatures / Beijing Normal University) 331 SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR AND CONSTRUCTION GRAMMAR Francis Y. LIN Alex X. PENG (School of Foreign Languages and Literatures / Beijing Normal University) ABSTRACT: Construction Grammar (CG) as developed

More information

Pre-Sales Requirements Engineering based on Miller Heiman s Sales Approach

Pre-Sales Requirements Engineering based on Miller Heiman s Sales Approach Pre-Sales Requirements Engineering based on Miller Heiman s Sales Approach Christoph Oemig Wincor Nixdorf International GmbH, Germany christoph.oemig@wincor-nixdorf.com Abstract. Requirements engineering

More information

Indiana Department of Education

Indiana Department of Education GRADE 1 READING Guiding Principle: Students read a wide range of fiction, nonfiction, classic, and contemporary works, to build an understanding of texts, of themselves, and of the cultures of the United

More information