Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) 2796 2804. Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia"

Transcription

1 Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: The role of saccade preparation in lateralized word recognition: Evidence for the attentional bias theory Dorine Vergilino Perez a,c,n, Christelle Lemoine a, Eric Siéroff b,c, Anne-Marie Ergis b, Redha Bouhired a,c, Emilie Rigault a,c, Karine Doré-Mazars a,c a Laboratoire Vision Action Cognition, IUPDP, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Institut de Psychologie, 71 Avenue Edouard Vaillant, Boulogne Billancourt, Cedex, France b Neuropsychologie du vieillissement (EA 4468), IUPDP, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Institut de Psychologie, Boulogne Billancourt, France c Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France article info Article history: Received 24 February 2012 Received in revised form 17 July 2012 Accepted 30 July 2012 Available online 7 August 2012 Keywords: Hemispheric asymmetry Right visual field advantage Saccadic eye movements Language Attention abstract Words presented to the right visual field (RVF) are recognized more readily than those presented to the left visual field (LVF). Whereas the attentional bias theory proposes an explanation in terms of attentional imbalance between visual fields, the attentional advantage theory assumes that words presented to the RVF are processed automatically while LVF words need attention. In this study, we exploited coupling between attention and saccadic eye movements to orient spatial attention to one or the other visual field. The first experiment compared conditions wherein participants had to remain fixated centrally or had to make a saccade to the visual field in which subsequent verbal stimuli were displayed. The orienting of attention by saccade preparation improved performance in a lexical decision task in both the LVF and the RVF. In the second experiment, participants had to make a saccade either to the visual field where verbal stimuli were presented subsequently or to the opposite side. For RVF as well as for LVF presentation, saccade preparation toward the opposite side decreased performance compared to the same side condition. These results are better explained by the attentional bias theory, and are discussed in the light of a new attentional theory dissociating two major components of attention, namely preparation and selection. & 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction The human visual system is organized so that stimuli presented to the right visual field (RVF) are initially projected to the left primary visual cortex whereas stimuli presented to the left visual field (LVF) are projected to the right primary visual cortex. Many studies have examined whether this neuroanatomical contralateral organization is responsible for perceptual asymmetries observed on behavioral measures. One of the most studied asymmetries is the right visual field superiority (RVFS) for words, which accounts for the fact that words presented to the right visual field are recognized more readily than those presented to the left visual field (Mishkin & Forgays, 1952). Whatever the verbal task identification, lexical decision response time and accuracy measures improve when verbal stimuli are presented to the right of the fixation point rather than to the left (e.g., Nicholls & Clode, 1996; Faust, Babkoff, & Kravetz, 1995). n Correspondence author. Tel.: þ ; fax: þ address: dorine.vergilino-perez@parisdescartes.fr (D. Vergilino Perez). The RVFS may be explained by the structural model of cerebral asymmetry, assuming that the left hemisphere is solely responsible for verbal processing, or is at least the most efficient (Kimura, 1966; Allen, 1983), as contralateral organization of the human visual system implies a direct access to the language centers located in the left hemisphere for words presented to the RVF. LVF word presentation may require an inter-hemispheric transfer via the callosum corpus that explains longer reaction times in verbal tasks. Arguments for the structural hypothesis come from the work of Cohen, Dehaene and colleagues, who have shown that a portion of the left mid fusiform gyrus called the visual word form area (VWFA) is particularly responsive to visual words (McCandliss, Cohen, & Dehaene, 2003). The VWFA was initially considered as being invariant for spatial position, as it was activated for words regardless of their position of presentation in the visual field (Cohenet al., 2000; Cohen et al., 2002). However, a recent magnetoencephalography study found stronger VWFA responses for words presented to the RVF than to the LVF (Barca et al., 2011). Moreover, a direct relationship between individual language lateralization and visual half field advantages measured by behavioral studies has been reported recently (Hunter & Brysbaert, 2008; Van der Haegen, Cai, Seurinck, & /$ - see front matter & 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

2 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Brsybaert, 2011). Left-handers with left hemisphere language dominance assessed by fmri responses during a word naming task showed an RVFS whereas left-handers with right hemisphere language dominance showed an LVF superiority. However, other explanations of the RVFS have been proposed based on a different allocation of spatial attention across the visual field that could depend on the hemisphere involved in the task processing or on how written language is processed by each hemisphere. In that, attentional hypotheses are not incompatible with the structural model. The attentional bias theory proposed by Kinsbourne (1970) suggests that visual field asymmetries are the result of an attentional imbalance between the hemispheres. The theory suggests that at rest, the two hemispheres are in a state of mutual inhibition resulting in distribution of attention over the two visual fields. However, preferential activation of one hemisphere by a task leads to deployment of attentional resources to the contralateral visual field. Thus, presentation of verbal material in a reading task, resulting in an activation of the left hemisphere, causes a bias of attentional resources to the right side of space. For left-to-right readers, this bias may be strengthened by reading habits, as attention constantly shifts in the direction of most eye movements. Considering spatial attention to be a component of visual asymmetries allows the attentional effects reported in numerous studies to be explained. A first attentional effect refers to the fact that when a word and a distractor are simultaneously presented in opposite visual fields, word identification is more impaired by presentation of the distractor to the RVF (i.e., the visual field in which the attention is mainly allocated) than to the LVF (Siéroff & Urbanski, 2002; Siéroff & Riva, 2011). Interestingly, such distractor effect is reversed when both the word and the distractor were presented in foveal vision, attached to each other in such a way that the word was presented to the left or to the right of the fixation position (Van der Haegen & Brysbaert, 2011). In such case, word presented to the LVF was better recognized than word presented in the RVF. Such a left visual field advantage has been explained in the context of the SERIOL model (Whitney, 2001) by considering that in this situation, both word and nonword were considered as a single unit that has to be processed serially. This model assumes that the letters of a word have to be processed serially with the help of an activation gradient that is based on the decreasing visual acuity from the fovea to the periphery. Whereas such gradient is adapted to RVF presentation, it has to be reversed for LVF information processing. Moreover in case of a single foveal word, information about RVF letter has to be inhibited until LVF letter information is transmitted to the left hemisphere. Such inversion gradient as well as inhibition processes require attention to focus on the initial word letters. A second attentional effect occurs in classical cueing paradigms in which the RVF Superiority is reduced when parafoveal word location is previously cued. This indicates that the LVF benefits more from the valid cue than the RVF (e.g., Mondor & Bryden, 1992; Ducrot & Grainger, 2007; Nicholls & Wood, 1998; Gatheron & Siéroff, 1999). This suggests that the LVF valid cue may counteract the RVF attentional bias by increasing attentional resources in the LVF whereas the RVF valid cue does not further increase the amount of attentional resources in the RVF. Whereas the attentional bias theory assumes that more attention is allocated to the RVF than to the LVF, the attentional advantage theory (Mondor & Bryden, 1992) proposes that the processing of verbal material in the RVF does not require as much attention as in the LVF. According to this model, the word is processed as a whole by the left hemisphere and letter-by-letter by the right hemisphere. Word-level processing and letter-level processing make different demands on attentional processing, the former requiring little or no attentional resources, the latter requiring important attentional resources (Lindell & Nicholls, 2003). Such automatic processing of verbal stimuli in the RVF also explains why an RVF valid cue does not help word recognition any further and why a distractor presented in the RVF decreases recognition of the word in the LVF. A recent neuroimaging study has provided arguments for the attentional advantage theory by showing that the fast and parallel RVF word recognition is underpinned by a network involving the left ventral occipitotemporal pathway including the VWFA, whereas serial letter-byletter scanning involved in LVF word processing is underpinned by the dorsal parietal attention system (Cohen, Dehaene, Vinckier, Jobert, & Montavont, 2008). Whether the RVF Superiority may be explained by the attentional bias theory or by the attentional advantage theory is still under debate. Siéroff and Riva (2011) provide an argument in favor of the attentional bias theory, as they show the same larger distractor effect on LVF words than on RVF words in young children and adults, although the effect of the distractor is globally much stronger in children indicating less automatic reading. If the asymmetric distractor effect was explained by automatic word processing of the left hemisphere, then one would expect a greater asymmetric distractor effect in adults for whom reading is more automatic. On the other hand, the greater word length effect for words as well as the larger effects of letter distortion such as case alternation for LVF words than for RVF words argue in favor of whole-word processing as assumed by the attentional advantage theory (see Ellis, 2004 and Lindell, 2006 for a review). Recently, Siéroff et al., 2012 proposed an attentional theory that could reconcile the two attentional hypotheses by taking into consideration two attentional components, namely preparation and selection (LaBerge, 1995). Divided visual field studies generally involve a first phase in which a warning signal allows the subjects to get ready to answer to the subsequent verbal task by allocating processing resources in the location where words are likely to occur. During this preparatory phase, distribution of attentional resources could be influenced by hemispheric activation as proposed by Kinsbourne (1970), as well as by reading habits. In the second phase, word presentation implies a deployment of selective attention that helps its identification. Different modes of selective attention may exist depending on visual field and reading direction. For left-to-right language, RVF word presentation implies that selective attention initially oriented to the first letters of the word could smoothly move to the other letters resulting in a very fast word processing. On the contrary, LVF word presentation implies that selective attention be oriented to the left which contradicts the natural tendency to read from left to right and takes more time, explaining the poorer performance in word recognition. Such mechanism may be compared to the cost of acuity gradient inversion proposed in the SERIOL model (Whitney, 2001). Thus, the attentional theory distinguishing preparatory and selective aspects of attention may help to understand why the distractor effect is reversed in foveal and attached presentation of the word and distractor, compared to parafoveal and separated presentation (Siéroff et al., 2012). It should be noted that in previously cited studies, covert attention is generally oriented by instructions or by a cueing paradigm with the gaze having to be fixated to the center of the screen. Lateralized verbal stimuli were presented briefly to avoid any eye movement in their direction, even if only recording of eye movement can really ensure that the eyes remain fixated on the central fixation point (see Bourne, 2006 for other methodological constraints in divided visual field studies). Some authors monitored eye movement but simply to discard trials with eye movement (e.g., Nicholls & Wood, 1998; Lindell & Nicholls, 2003; Calvo & Nummenmaa, 2009). It is quite surprising that

3 2798 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) none of these studies exploited coupling between attention and saccade to orient attention. Indeed, it is now well known that preparation of a saccade to a spatial location induces a concomitant deployment of spatial attention at this location. This leads, in a detection task, to better performances at the saccade target location than at any other location of the visual field (Hoffman & Subramaniam, 1995; Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser, 1995; Deubel & Schneider, 1996, Doré-Mazars, Pouget, & Beauvillain, 2004). To our knowledge, only one study has examined the effect of orienting attention by saccade preparation on the RVF superiority (Hyönä & Koivisto, 2006). Participants had to perform a lexical decision task on verbal stimuli presented 100 ms to the RVF or in the LVF. The fixation condition in which the participants had to remain fixated on the screen center did not involve any manipulation of attention as verbal stimuli were not preceded by any cue or any instructions. The move condition, in which the participants had to execute a saccade to the verbal stimuli before making their lexical decision, involved an overt attentional engagement. As expected, a RVFS was found in the fixation condition. However, in the move condition, saccade preparation toward verbal stimuli improved performance only in the LVF, leading to a cancellation of the RVFS. The authors interpreted their results in the context of the attentional advantage theory, suggesting that orientation of attention by saccade preparation allowed to cancel out the initial poorer performance in the LVF but did not increase the RVF performance due to automatic word processing. However, we trust that the results of Hyönä and Koivisto were not incompatible with the attentional bias theory. Indeed, if we consider that there is a maximal amount of attention that could be allocated to a region of visual space, then attention allocated to the RVF by saccade preparation may not be cumulated with the attentional resources due to the preexisting bias. Moreover, the fact that the deployment of attention linked to the saccade preparation compensated for the RVFS is surprising as most of the studies looking at the cueing effect have found a reduction rather than a cancellation of the RVFS (e.g., Ducrot & Grainger, 2007). The main objective of the two experiments presented here was to disentangle the attentional bias and the attentional advantage theories by building on coupling between spatial attention and saccade preparation. Moreover, we also tried to evaluate the respective contributions of covert and overt attention in the RVF Superiority. Indeed, it was previously shown that covert attention is sufficient to identify words in a divided visual field study, with better performance in the RVF than in the LVF (Calvo & Nummenmaa, 2009) and Hyönä and Koivisto s study suggests that the overt attention linked to saccade preparation increases performance in the LVF, but not in the RVF. In our study, spatial attention was oriented by both a cueing procedure and preparation of a saccade. The first experiment was inspired from Hyönä and Koivisto s as we asked participants to do a lexical decision task while remaining fixated on the screen centre during all the trial or while executing a saccade to the verbal stimuli. However, our study differs as we used a central cue, always valid, to indicate spatial location of the subsequent verbal stimuli as well as saccade direction. In the second experiment, the participants had to make a saccade in each trial. However, the central cue that indicated saccade direction was valid or invalid relative to the hemifield of presentation for verbal stimuli. We were particularly interested in the condition in which stimuli were presented to the RVF and a saccade executed on the opposite side, as this was the critical condition to disambiguate between the two attentional hypotheses. Indeed, since the attentional advantage theory assumes an automatic processing of verbal stimuli in the RVF, RVF performance should not be deteriorated by deployment of attention linked to saccade preparation to the LVF. Conversely, since the attention bias theory assumes that more attention is allocated to the RVF, RVF performance should decrease when attention is oriented to the LVF by saccade preparation. 2. Experiment Method Participants Sixteen French students (8 men) from the University of Paris Descartes took part in the experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision, were between 19 and 32 (mean age: 23) and were right-handed (mean laterality score: 91%, from 81% to 100%). Their hand preference was determined by using the Humphrey Laterality Questionnaire modified by (Hécaen and Ajuriaguerra (1963), 22 items testing hand, eye and foot preferences). The study adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki Apparatus Stimuli were presented on an Iiyama HM240DT monitor with a refresh rate of 170 Hz and a resolution of pixels. The experimental sessions took place in a dimly lit room. Subjects were seated 57 cm away from the screen and their head kept stable with a chin and forehead rest. Eye movements were recorded with an Eyelink 1000 s (SR Research, Ontario, Canada), with a temporal resolution of 1000 Hz, and a spatial resolution of Viewing was binocular but only movements of the right eye were monitored. Each session began with a 9 point calibration over the entire screen. Before each trial, central fixation was checked and compared to the calibration. If the distance between the fixation check and the calibration was greater than 0.751, fixation was refused and a new calibration was initiated. When successful calibration was detected, the trial began. Online saccade detection corresponded to above-threshold velocity (301/s) and acceleration (80001/s 2 ) Stimuli The experimental material comprised 120 French four-letter nouns and 120 French four-letter pseudo-words, each divided in 4 lists of 30 items. The word frequency computed from the Lexique 3.71 database (New, Pallier, Ferrand, & Matos, 2001; New, Pallier, Brysbaert, & Ferrand, 2004) did not vary across word lists (mean: 47.8 occurrences per million, all t-tests non significant). The pseudo-words were constructed from real words by changing only one letter and conformed to the phonotactic rules of French. The stimuli were displayed in lowercase black letters on a white background in 14-point Bold Courier New Font. Each letter covered a visual angle of 0.51 horizontally and 0.51 vertically Procedure and design The participants performed the fixation and the saccade tasks in separate sessions. Each trial began with the presentation of a black double arrow for 500 ms at the screen s center (see Fig. 1). Four dashes were presented at 21 degrees of eccentricity (from the arrow center to the closest edge of the dashes) on both sides, at the same position of the stimuli when they were presented to the right or to the left of the fixation point. The double arrow was then replaced by a single arrow pointing to the right or to the left for 500 ms. In the Fixation task, the single arrow indicated the visual field in which the stimulus would appear. In the Saccade task, it also indicated the direction of the saccade as all the trials were valid. A trial was cancelled and replaced later if the participant s gaze deviated by more than

4 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) from the arrow. Simultaneously to the extinction of the arrow (go-signal in the saccade task), a letter string was displayed for a duration adapted for each participant on a pre-experiment (see description below). For each participant, the stimulus duration was the same in the fixation and the saccade conditions, and should be shorter than the mean latency of a saccade. Indeed, in both fixation and saccade tasks, the stimulus should be processed only in parafoveal vision when the participants fixated the centre of the screen. In other words, in the saccade condition, the stimulus was expected to have disappeared at the time the eye movement started. Otherwise, the data were excluded (see Section 2.2). The participant had to decide whether or not the stimulus was a French word. A manual response was given at the end of the trial, after the stimulus extinction, by pressing one of the two designated buttons in the gamepad. Responses were always given with one hand, with the index finger for the yes response and the middle finger for the no. The hand of response was counterbalanced across participants. For each participant, a pre-experiment was conducted before the experimental sessions in which we adapted the time of presentation of the verbal stimuli in order to avoid chance performance and ceiling effects. Four blocks of 16 trials were presented, using the same procedure as the Fixation task. The stimuli differed from the experimental sessions. The duration of stimulus presentation was fixed at 80 ms for the first block. At the end of the block, the duration was increased or decreased depending on participants performance. The procedure was repeated for following blocks until a percentage of correct answers of approximately 80% was obtained. The mean duration was 76 ms (standard deviation: 16.3, range from 50 to 100). Each participant had to do the two tasks in two sessions of 120 trials each. The tasks order was maintained constant across subjects. They began by the fixation task in which they were asked to keep fixating the screen centre until responding. In the Fig. 1. Procedure used in the first experiment. After the verification of the calibration, a double arrow was displayed during 500 ms in the center of the screen. Four dashes were presented on either side, at the same position of the stimuli when they were presented to the right or to the left of the fixation point. The double arrow was then replaced by a single arrow pointing to the right or to the left for 500 ms and indicating the hemifield of the stimulus presentation in the fixation task, and the saccade direction. The verbal stimulus was displayed during a time adapted for each participant. saccade task, they were instructed to execute a saccade towards the visual field indicated by a cue at the moment of its disappearance. In each session, two lists of words and two lists of pseudo-words were presented. The presentation of the lists was counterbalanced between participants so that each list was presented in each of the 4 experimental conditions resulting from the crossing of the task instructions (Fixation vs. Saccade) and the visual field of presentation (Left or Right). Each item was seen only once by each participant but appeared in each of the four experimental conditions across them all. In each session, trials were randomized, so that participants could not anticipate on which side the stimulus would be presented Results Trials in which blinks occurred (0.1%) were eliminated from further analyses. In the Fixation task, we also eliminated trials in which a saccade was executed (7.4%). In the Saccade task, we eliminated trials in which a participant did not make an eye movement (5.75%), made it in the opposite direction relative to the arrow (0.7%), made a saccade with a latency shorter than the stimulus duration implying that the letter string was still on the screen when the saccade landed (3.78%), or made a saccade with short (o80 ms) or long (4800 ms) saccade latencies (2.96%). We first report data on saccadic latency and amplitude obtained with the saccade task. We ran a 2 2 Anova including visual field of presentation (Left or Right) and stimulus type (words vs. pseudo-words). For data focused on the RVF superiority (percentage of correct responses, sensitivity index d 0 and reaction time), we first verified that the responding hand did not modulate the results pattern by adding it to the analyses as a between-subject factor. As we did not find any effect of the responding hand, as well as no interaction with the withinsubject factors, we collapsed the data across the two modalities. So for the percentage of correct responses and the mean reaction time for correct responses, we ran a Anova including stimulus type (words vs. pseudo-words), visual field of presentation (Left or Right) and task instructions (Fixation vs. Saccade). As noted by Hyönä and Koivisto (2006), data on reaction time should be interpreted with caution as in the saccade task, the psychological refractory period could induce delayed secondary manual response due to the preceding saccadic eye movement (Wolf, Deubel, & Hauske, 1984). However, we have chosen to report these data as we did not find significant differences between mean reaction times obtained in fixation and saccade tasks. The means for correct responses and reaction times are shown in Table 1. As Hyönä and Koivisto, we computed the parametric sensitivity index of d 0 that measures detection accuracy in the context of the signal detection theory (see formula in Gescheider (1985)) and the index C that reflects a possible response bias (see formula in Snodgrass and Corwin (1988)). For these two measures, we ran a 2 2 Anova including visual field of presentation (Left or Right) and task instructions (Fixation vs. Saccade). Table 1 Percentage of correct response and reaction time (in ms) relative to the task instructions (fixation vs. saccade), hemifield of stimulus presentation (LVF vs. RVF) and stimulus type (word vs. pseudo-words). Fixation Saccade LVF RVF LVF RVF Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word % of correct response 69.7 (17.8) 79.9 (11.3) 84.7 (9.2) 83.7 (8.6) 72.5 (12.8) 82.7 (12.8) 88.9 (8.3) 86.4 (11.2) Reaction time (ms) 885 (285) 1163 (493) 765 (236) 945 (309) 880 (288) 1112 (418) 738 (194) 942 (290)

5 2800 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Saccadic latency and amplitude The mean latency was of ms739 ms with no significant differences between words and pseudo-words (177 ms742 ms, and 174 ms738 ms, Fo1) and between LVF and RVF (174 ms740 ms, and 177 ms739 ms, Fo1). The interaction between both factors failed to reach significance (F(1,15)¼3.83, po06). The mean saccade amplitude was of However, the analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of the visual field of presentation (F(1,15)¼5.04, po04), indicating that saccades directed to the LVF had larger amplitudes ( ) than saccades to the RVF ( ). Such an effect interacted with the type of stimulus (F(1,15)¼6.31, po03) as it was only marginal for words ( vs , F(1,15)¼2.88, po10) and significant for pseudo-words ( vs , F(1,15)¼6.88, po01). Note, however, that the effect of 0.21 was below the spatial accuracy of eye movement. and Koivisto, as the RVFS was maintained in the saccade condition. We estimated a possible bias in performance by computing the index C. A completely neutral bias has a C value of 0 whereas a conservative bias induces a positive value and a liberal bias a negative value. As shown in Fig. 2, we obtained C values relatively close to 0 in each experimental condition. However, the analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of the visual field of presentation (F(1,15)¼19.10, po0006), indicating that the participants presented a conservative bias when they responded to stimuli presented to the LVF and a tendency to be liberal for stimuli to the RVF. The greater bias to respond word with RVF presentation compared to that of the LVF has already been reported in other studies (e.g., Babkoff, Faust, & Lavidor, 1997). There was no task effect and no interaction between the two factors (Fso1) Probability of correct response The Anova ran on the probability of correct responses revealed significant effects of the visual field of presentation (F(1,15)¼ 22.02, po0003) and the task instructions (F(1,15)¼4.35, po05). We found a right visual field superiority as the percentage of correct response was significantly better for stimuli presented in the RVF than in the LVF. The percentage of correct response was also greater in the saccade than in the fixation task. The interaction between the two factors was not significant (Fo1), indicating that a right visual field superiority was found for the fixation task but also for the saccade task. This differs from Hyönä and Koivisto (2006) who found a disappearance of right visual field superiority in the saccade condition, with equally good performance in both hemifields. The main effect of the stimulus type was not significant (F(1,15)¼1.94, ns). However, the type of stimulus interacted with the visual field of presentation (F(1,15)¼19.35, po0005), indicating an effect of the visual field presentation for words (F(1,15)¼28.91, po0005) that was only marginal for pseudowords (F(1,15)¼3.75, po07). Moreover, performance was better for words than for pseudo-words in the LVF (F(1,15)¼7.03, po02) but not in the RVF (Fo1). Finally, there was no interaction between the three factors (Fo1) Sensitivity index d 0 and response bias index C Data presented in Fig. 2 seem to be in accordance with results obtained on the probability of correct responses. A RVFS appears for both fixation and saccade tasks, even if performance is better in the latter. The analysis of variance confirmed these descriptive results: the main effects of visual field presentation and of task instructions were significant (F(1,15)¼19.41, po0005 and F(1,15)¼4.70, po04, respectively), with no interaction between the two (Fo1). Again, we failed to replicate the results of Hyönä Fig. 2. Sensitivity index d 0 (left panel) and response bias index C (right panel) as a function of the task instructions (fixation vs. saccade) and the hemifield of stimulus presentation (LVF vs. RVF) Reaction time Reaction times for correct responses yielded a main effect of the stimulus type (F(1,15)¼19.37, po0005), with longer reaction times for pseudo-words than for words and as expected a main effect of the visual field presentation (F(1,15)¼16.87, po0009), with longer reaction times for stimuli presented in the LVF than in the RVF. There was no effect of the task instructions (Fo1) as well as no interactions (Fo1) Discussion In this experiment, we used fixation and saccade conditions similar to those of Hyönä and Koivisto (2006), except that a central cue, always valid, indicated the visual field in which verbal stimuli appeared. We did not replicate their results. Indeed, they found equally good performance in both visual fields when the participants had to make a saccade to the LVF or RVF and performance in the RVF identical in the fixation and saccade conditions. Using the context of the attentional advantage theory, they concluded that saccade preparation allowed deployment of attentional resources to the LVF, overcoming its disadvantage found in a fixation condition but did not benefit the RVF in which word processing required less attention. Our results differed as we found a RVFS in the fixation as well as in the saccade conditions, with better performance in both LVF and RVF when participants had to make a saccade. This suggests that if attentional resources due to saccade planning help performance in both visual fields, this does not cancel out the RVFS. This is in agreement with previous studies showing that cue or instructions reduce RVFS but do not cancel it (e.g., Ducrot & Grainger, 2007). Increased performance for words presented to the RVF in the saccade condition compared to the fixation condition are better explained by the attentional bias theory, assuming that deployment of attention due to saccade preparation leads to better performance in the LVF, that has initially limited attentional resources, but also in the RVF by cumulating with the existing attentional bias due to hemispheric activation and reading habits (Kinsbourne, 1970). However, a critical condition to disambiguate between the two attentional theories would consist in using invalid cues to direct the saccade to the opposite visual field from the one in which verbal stimuli will appear. Whereas the attentional advantage theory assumes that performance in the RVF should not be deteriorated by engagement of attentional resources due to saccade preparation in the LVF, the attention bias theory predicts that it should. In order to test these hypotheses, we conducted a second experiment, in which the participants had to make a saccade to the visual field in which verbal stimuli appeared subsequently or to the opposite one.

6 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Experiment Method Participants Thirty-two French students (16 men) from the University of Paris Descartes took part in the experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision, were between 19 and 31 (mean age: 24) and were right-handed (mean laterality score: 94%, from 81, 81% to 100%) Apparatus and stimuli The apparatus and stimuli were the same as in Experiment Procedure and design The procedure was similar to the one used in the first experiment, except that the participants only performed the saccade task (240 trials). As in the first experiment, they had to execute a saccade toward the visual field indicated by the single arrow. However, verbal stimuli could be displayed in the same visual field (valid trials) or in the opposite visual field (invalid trials) with a proportion of 50/50. The presentation of the lists was counterbalanced between participants so that each list was presented in each of the 4 experimental conditions resulting from crossing the visual field of stimulus presentation (Left or Right) and the saccade direction (Same vs. opposite direction relative to the visual field of presentation). Each item was seen only once by each participant but appeared in each of the four experimental conditions across all participants. Experimental trials were randomized. The mean duration of verbal stimuli was 85 ms (standard deviation: 18.9 ms, range from 50 ms to 120 ms) Results Trials in which blinks occurred (0.03%) were eliminated from further analyses. We also eliminated trials in which a participant did not make an eye movement (8.7%), made it in the opposite direction relative to the arrow (10.35%), made a saccade with a latency shorter than the stimulus duration implying that the letter string was still on the screen when the saccade landed (15.18%), or made a saccade with short (o80 ms) or long (4800 ms) saccade latencies (4.03%). As in the first experiment, we first checked a potential effect of the responding hand on the percentage of correct response, the d 0 and the reaction time. Again, we did not find any effect of the responding hand and collapsed the data across the two modalities Saccadic latency and amplitude Saccade latency was longer when the saccade was planned to the opposite side than to the same side of the stimulus presentation (248 ms768 ms, and 161 ms731 ms, respectively). Although the participants had 500 ms to prepare a saccade in the direction indicated by the central arrow, the abrupt onset of verbal stimuli in the opposite visual field automatically triggered the execution of a saccade towards it that had to be inhibited. Such a latency difference between the same side and the opposite side conditions may be related to the classical differences found between pro- and anti-saccades (Munoz & Everling, 2004). Here, the difference of 87 ms between the two saccadic conditions was significant (F(1,31)¼99.93, po0005). Moreover, we also found a smaller but significant difference of 16 ms between the latency of saccades directed to the LVF or RVF, the former being longer than the latter (212 ms754 ms vs. 196 ms745 ms, F(1,31)¼10.85, po002). The interaction between the visual field of stimulus presentation and the saccade direction failed to reach significance (F(1,31)¼3.07, po08). We did not find any other significant effect or interactions (Fso1). Saccade amplitudes were only affected by the saccade direction, the amplitude of saccades directed to the opposite side of visual stimulus presentation being larger than the amplitude of saccades directed to the same side ( vs , respectively, F(1,31)¼7.63, po009).this difference of 0.81 could be understood as an inaccuracy of the saccade directed to the visual field with no stimulation Probability of correct response Table 2 presents the percentage of correct response for words and pseudo-words displayed in the LVF and in the RVF when a saccade is directed to either the same or the opposite visual field. The Anova ran on the data indicated a main effect of the visual field presentation (F(1,31)¼53.41, po0005) as well as a main effect of the saccade direction (F(1,31)¼14.06, po0008) with no interaction between the two factors (Fo1). This showed that although performance is better when the participant planned a saccade to the visual field in which a stimulus subsequently appeared, a RVFS was found in this condition as well as when the saccade was planned in the opposite direction. Note that the type of stimulus interacted with the saccade direction (F(1,31)¼18.39, po0002) as well as with the visual field of presentation (F(1,31)¼11.36, po002), its main effect being not significant (Fo1). The first interaction means that the effect of the saccade direction was shown for words (F(1, 31)¼24.52, po0005) but not for pseudo-words (F(1,31)¼2.03, ns). The second interaction indicated a stronger RVFS for words, although the RVFS was significant for both words and pseudo-words (F(1,31)¼38.65, po0005 and F(1,31)¼11.72, po002, respectively) Sensitivity index d 0 and response bias index C As in the first experiment, we analyzed our data in the context of the signal detection theory by computing the d 0 and the index C. As shown in Fig. 3, performance in terms of perceptual capacity is better for stimuli presented in the RVF than in the LVF in both same- and opposite-side conditions. As for the probability of correct response, the performance increased when the saccade was planned to the visual field in which the stimulus Table 2 Percentage of correct response and reaction time (in ms) relative to the saccade direction (same side vs. opposite side from the stimulus), hemifield of stimulus presentation (LVF vs. RVF) and stimulus type (word vs. pseudo-words). Same side Opposite side LVF RVF LVF RVF Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word Word Pseudo-word % of correct response 74,1 (11.8) 72.7 (14.9) 91.9 (7.3) 78.6 (11.1) 60.1 (23.8) 75.4 (14.8) 78 (21.7) 81.7 (17) Reaction time (ms) 985 (506) 1171 (537) 813 (367) 1033 (548) 1281 (689) 1205 (683) 1156 (654) 1136 (616)

7 2802 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) General discussion Fig. 3. Sensitivity index d 0 (left panel) and response bias index C (right panel) as a function of saccade direction (same side vs. opposite side from the stimulus) and the hemifield of stimulus presentation (LVF vs. RVF). subsequently appeared. The Anova confirmed these results, by showing a main effect of the visual field of presentation (F(1, 31)¼63.75, po0005) and a main effect of the saccade direction (F(1,31)¼14.23, po0007), with no interaction between these factors (Fo1). As in the first experiment, the analysis of the index C showed that the participants presented a conservative bias for stimuli presented in the LVF and a liberal bias for stimuli presented in the RVF, the difference being significant (F(1, 31)¼18.71, po0001). The Anova also revealed an effect of the saccade direction (F(1, 31)¼18.97, po0001), indicating a conservative bias when the saccade was made to the visual field opposite that of the verbal stimulus presentation and a liberal bias when the saccade was made in the same visual field. There was no interaction between the two factors (F(1, 31)¼1.79, ns) Reaction time As expected, the Anova ran on mean reaction times showed a main effect of the visual field of presentation (F(1,31)¼23.47, po0005), with longer times for stimuli presented in the LVF than in the RVF. The saccade direction also affected the mean reaction times (F(1,31)¼11.04, po002) that were longer when the saccade was made to the visual field opposite that of the verbal stimulus presentation. We also found a main effect of the type of stimulus (F(1,31)¼5.51, po02), with longer reaction time for pseudo-words than for words. The interaction between the type of stimulus and the saccade direction was significant (F(1,31)¼32.48, po0005) indicating that the saccade direction effect occurred only for words (F(1,31)¼24.42, po0005) Discussion In the second experiment, we used the central cue to indicate the visual field in which the saccade had to be executed. The cue could be valid when verbal stimuli appeared on the same visual field or invalid when it appeared on the opposite visual field. In valid trials which correspond to the saccade condition of the first experiment, again we did not replicate Hyönä and Koivisto s results: the allocation of attentional resources to the LVF induced by the saccade preparation did not cancel out the RVFS. However, the condition of interest to distinguish between the attentional bias and attentional advantage theories relied on trials in which the central cue oriented attention to the LVF whereas verbal stimuli appeared in the RVF. In this condition, the attentional advantage theory predicts that the performance for words presented in the RVF should not be affected by the LVF cue and so should be equal to valid RVF cue whereas the attentional bias theory predicts a decrease in performance compared to the valid RVF cue. Our results clearly argue in favor of the attentional bias theory as the performance measured by the percentage of correct response and by the d 0 clearly decreased for verbal stimuli presented in the RVF preceded by an invalid cue compared to a valid cue. In this study, we conducted two experiments, inspired from the work of Hyönä and Koivisto (2006), in which we examined the effect of orienting attention by saccade preparation on the RVFS. In the first experiment, we used similar fixation and saccade conditions. In the fixation condition, the participants had to do a lexical decision task on verbal stimuli displayed in the LVF or RVF by remaining fixated on the center of the screen, whereas in the saccade condition, they had to execute a saccade toward the visual field in which verbal stimuli were displayed. Whereas both studies found a RVFS in the fixation condition, they differed in the saccade condition. In Hyönä and Koivisto s work, saccade preparation toward the stimulus improved performance only in the LVF, cancelling the RVFS. In the RVF, performance was equivalent in the two conditions. They concluded, in the context of the attentional advantage theory, that orientation of attention by saccade preparation increased performance in the LVF where little attention was allocated, but did not benefit the RVF, where automatic word processing did not necessitate much attention. Their results are coherent with the work of Mondor & Bryden (1992) showing also a cancellation of the RVFS when a cue oriented attention to the LVF, even in the case of short delays between the apparition of the cue and the verbal stimuli. In Hÿonä and Koivisto s study, attention was simply oriented by saccade preparation. The mean saccade latency, which represented the delay to orient attention, was of 156 ms, and so coherent with Mondor & Bryden s delays. However, it should be noted that many other studies found reduction rather than cancellation of the RVFS (e.g., Nicholls & Wood, 1998; Nicholls, Wood, & Hayes, 2001; Ducrot & Grainger, 2007). Our results are in agreement with these studies as deployment of attention by saccade preparation increased performance in both LVF and RVF, without overcoming the RVFS. The benefit in the RVF as well as in the LVF argues for the attentional bias theory. Moreover, this suggests that overt attention can be cumulated with covert attention to increase performance in both visual fields. This result fits in with a previous study showing that covert attention is sufficient to identify words in a divided visual field study, with better performance in the RVF than in the LVF (Calvo & Nummenmaa, 2009). In the second experiment, we contrasted trials with valid and invalid cue involving the execution of a saccade in the same or in the opposite visual field in which the verbal stimuli were displayed. We were particularly interested in the condition in which the stimuli were presented to the RVF and the saccade executed toward it or on the opposite side. The decreased performance in the latter condition compared to the former argues again for the attentional bias theory. The question may arise of the differences between our works and Hÿonä and Koivisto s? Methodological differences can be put forward. Indeed, in Hÿonä and Koivisto s study, verbal stimuli were briefly presented during 100 ms for all participants. Here, we adapted in a pre-experiment the time of presentation of verbal stimuli that was on average between 75 ms and 85 ms in the first and second experiment, respectively. It is possible that a 100-ms presentation made the lexical decision task too easy in the saccade condition. However, in this case, ceiling effects should have been highlighted on both the percentage of correct responses and d 0. Another methodological difference, and, to our view, the crucial difference, is related to the way attention is directed to one or the other visual field, involving preparatory attention or not, in addition to attentional selection components (Laberge, 1995). In Hÿonä and Koivisto s study, there was no manipulation of spatial attention other than that due to saccade preparation. The authors did not use any cue or instructions to orient covert attention. In other words, their experiment did not

8 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) allow any preparatory phase and attention could only select the word target location when it appeared. As proposed by the new attentional bias theory presented by Siéroff et al. (2012), selective attention could be naturally directed to the right in left-to-right languages, implying that the initial orientation to the first letters could be smoothly moved to the final letter resulting in a very fast word processing in the RVF, in the fixation as in the saccade conditions. On the contrary, our task allows the dissociation of the two components of attention. Indeed, the central cue presented during 500 ms induced a preparatory phase during which our participants could prepare for the subsequent verbal task by allocating processing resources at the location where words are likely to occur. Siéroff et al. (2012) expanded on Kinsbourne, 1970 theory and proposed that distribution of attentional resources during this preparatory phase might be influenced by hemispheric activation. Riva and Siéroff (2010) have provided arguments for that by showing that a distractor deteriorates the performance more significantly when it is presented before the word than simultaneously for RVF as well as LVF words. The RVF attentional bias occurring during the preparatory phase before word presentation is also compatible with the larger visual half field differences observed with bilateral than with unilateral presentation (Boles, 1987; Boles, 1990; Hunter & Brysbaert, 2008). Boles (1990) had suggested that activation of homologous areas in the two hemispheres by a bilateral presentation caused a disruption of the inter-hemispheric transfer, resulting in even worse performance for stimuli presented to the LVF (Boles, 1990). However, one can expect that such disruption of the inter-hemispheric transfer does not deteriorate good performance for RVF stimuli presentation due to distribution of attentional resources. It should be noted that the RVFS, consistently found in our data regardless of whether the experiment was conducted in the fixation or saccade conditions, can also be explained by the structural hypothesis (Kimura, 1966; Allen, 1983), considering that the left hemisphere is solely responsible for verbal processing, or is at least the most efficient. However, as mentioned in the introduction, structural and attentional factors were not incompatible and our results clearly argue for a role of attention in the RVFS, in addition to structural factors. Indeed, we show a modulation of performance by the attentional component due to saccade preparation to the LVF as well as to the RVF. Thus, the performance improves when the subject has to prepare a saccade to the visual field in which verbal stimuli will be displayed and decreases when the subject prepares a saccade to the opposite visual field. Another argument for a role of attention comes from the fact that in the second experiment, saccades directed to the LVF had longer latencies than saccades directed to the RVF. Earlier studies looking at the saccade characteristics have shown such left-right asymmetry for right-handers but not for left-handers (e.g., Pirozzolo & Rayner, 1980; Hutton & Palet, 1986). The leftright asymmetry has been taken as a behavioral indicator of hemispheric specialization for oculomotor control, the left hemisphere being more efficient than the right one at executing visuomotor tasks. This difference in efficiency was hypothetically enhanced by reading habits. However, another study failed to replicate it in data averaged over subjects but found it at the individual level, arguing for a cognitive explanation linked to a visuospatial attention bias specific to individuals (Honda, 2002). Note that we also failed to find the left-right asymmetry on saccade latencies in the first experiment. To orient attention by preparation of an eye movement appears to be an interesting line of research in the study of lateralized word recognition. The eye movement recording is now made easier by the development of new systems and opens new possibilities. By manipulating the mode of triggering of the saccades involving reactive or voluntary saccades or the delay of their preparation, it could be possible to further investigate the two attentional components preparatory attention and selection in line with the recent theory proposed by Siéroff et al. (2012) in order to reconsider the discrepancies found between previous works arguing in favor of the attentional bias and the attentional advantage theories. Acknowledgements We thank Julie Lecas for correcting our English. References Allen, M. (1983). Models of hemispheric specialization. Psychological Bulletin, 93, Babkoff, H., Faust, M., & Lavidor, M. (1997). Lexical decision, visual hemifield and angle of orientation. Neuropsychologia, 35(4), Barca, L., Cornelissen, P., Simpson, M., Urooj, U., Woods, W., & Ellis, A. W. (2011). The neural basis of the right visual field advantage in reading: an MEG analysis using virtual electrodes. Brain and Language, 118, Boles, D. B. (1987). Reaction time asymmetry through bilateral vs. unilateral stimulus presentation. Brain and Cognition, 6, Boles, D. B. (1990). What bilateral displays do. Brain and Cognition, 12, Bourne, V. J. (2006). The divided visual field paradigm: methodological considerations. Laterality, 11, Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2009). Lateralised covert attention in word identification. Laterality, 14(2), Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehéricy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Hénaff, M. (2000). The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain, 123, Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Vinckier, F., Jobert, A., & Montavont, A. (2008). Reading normal and degraded words: contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. NeuroImage, 40, Cohen, L., Lehéricy, S., Cochon, F., Lemer, C., Rivaud, S., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the visual word form area. Brain, 125, Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, Doré-Mazars, K., Pouget, P., & Beauvillain, C. (2004). Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements. Psychological Research, 69, Ducrot, S., & Grainger, J. (2007). Deployment of spatial attention to words in central and peripheral vision. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, Ellis, A. W. (2004). Length, formats, neighbors and the processing of words presented laterally or at fixation. Brain and Language, 88, Faust, M., Babkoff, H., & Kravetz, S. (1995). Linguistic processes in the two cerebral hemispheres: implications for modularity vs. interactionism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, Gatheron, D., & Siéroff, E. (1999). Right hemifield superiority in reading and attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 40, Gescheider, G. A. (1985). Psychophysics: method. Theory, and application (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Hécaen, H., & Ajuriaguerra, J. (1963). Les Gauchers: Prévalence Manuelle et Dominance Cérébrale. Paris: PUF, Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, Honda, H. (2002). Idiosyncratic left-right asymmetries of saccadic latencies: examination in a gap paradigm. Vision Research, 42, Hunter, Z. R., & Brysbaert, M. (2008). Visual half field experiments are a good measure of cerebral language dominance if used properly. Neuropsychologia, 46, Hutton, J. T., & Palet, J. (1986). Lateral saccadic latencies and handedness. Neuropsychologia, 24(3), Hyönä, J., & Koivisto, M. (2006). The role of eye movements in lateralized word recognition. Laterality, 11(2), Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, Kimura, D. (1966). Dual functional asymmetry of the brain in visual perception. Neuropsychologia, 4, Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, LaBerge, D. (1995). Attentional processing: the brain s art of mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lindell, A. K. (2006). In your right mind: right hemisphere contributions to language processing and production. Neuropsychology Review, 16(3), Lindell, A. K., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2003). Attentional deployment in the visual halffield tasks: the effect of cue position on word naming latency. Brain and Cognition, 53,

9 2804 D. Vergilino Perez et al. / Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, Mishkin, M., & Forgays, D. G. (1952). Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, Mondor, T. A., & Bryden, M. P. (1992). On the relation between visual spatial attention and visual field asymmetry. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nature Review Neurosciences, 5, New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du franc-ais contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L Année Psychologique, 101, New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique2: a new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), Nicholls, M. E. R., & Clode, D. (1996). A failure to produce a left hemisphere advantage through verbal priming. Neuropsychologia, 34, Nicholls, M. E. R., & Wood, A. G. (1998). The contribution of attention to the right visual field advantage for word recognition. Brain and Cognition, 38, Nicholls, M. E. R., Wood, A. G., & Hayes, L. (2001). Cerebral asymmetries in the level of attention required for word recognition. Laterality, 6, Pirozzolo, F. J., & Rayner, K. (1980). Handedness, hemispheric specialization and saccadic eye movement latencies. Neuropsychologia, 18, Riva, M., & Siéroff, E. (2010). Right visual field attentional bias in reading in children and adults. Behavioural Neurology, 23, Siéroff, E., Dahman, R., & Fagard, J. (2012). Mechanisms of attention in reading parafoveal words: a cross-linguistic study in children. Neuropsychology, 26(3), Siéroff, E., & Riva, M. (2011). Attention and the identification of parafoveal words in school-age children and adults. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24(4), Siéroff, E., & Urbanski, M. (2002). Conditions of visual verbal extinction: does the ipsilesional stimulus have to be identified?. Brain and Cognition, 48, Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, Van der Haegen, L., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). The mechanisms underlying the interhemispheric integration of information in foveal word recognition: evidence for transcortical inhibition. Brain and Language, 118(3), Van der Haegen, L., Cai, Q., Seurinck, R., & Brsybaert, M. (2011). Further fmri validation of the visual half field technique as an indicator of language laterality: a large-group analysis. Neuropsychologia, 49(10), Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: the SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(2), Wolf, W., Deubel, H., & Hauske, G. (1984). Interaction between saccadic and manual reactions. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, 25, 262.

History of eye-tracking in psychological research

History of eye-tracking in psychological research History of eye-tracking in psychological research In the 1950s, Alfred Yarbus showed the task given to a subject has a very large influence on the subjects eye movements. Yarbus also wrote about the relation

More information

Decision and metrics of refixations in reading isolated words

Decision and metrics of refixations in reading isolated words Vision Research 44 (24) 29 217 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Decision and metrics of refixations in reading isolated words Dorine Vergilino-Perez *,Therese Collins, Karine Dore-Mazars Centre National

More information

Exploring and targeting saccades dissociated by saccadic adaptation

Exploring and targeting saccades dissociated by saccadic adaptation available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Exploring and targeting saccades dissociated by saccadic adaptation Louisa Lavergne a, b, Dorine Vergilino-Perez a, b,

More information

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION Peremen Ziv and Lamy Dominique Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University zivperem@post.tau.ac.il domi@freud.tau.ac.il Abstract Research has demonstrated

More information

Studying the contextual cues associated with fear of crime through eye tracking techniques

Studying the contextual cues associated with fear of crime through eye tracking techniques Studying the contextual cues associated with fear of crime through eye tracking techniques Inês Guedes 1, Pedro Fernandes 2 & Carla Cardoso 1 1 School of Criminology, Faculty of Law, University of Porto

More information

Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation

Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (4), 1032-1044 doi:10.3758/app.72.4.1032 Reduction of the flash-lag effect in terms of active observation MAKOTO ICHIKAWA Chiba University, Chiba, Japan

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

219 2009 The Psychonomic Society, Inc. Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee

219 2009 The Psychonomic Society, Inc. Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee sychonomic Bulletin Review 00, 1 (1), 1- doi:10./br.1.1.1 Both exogenous and endogenous target salience manipulations support resource depletion accounts of the attentional blink: A reply to Olivers, Spalek,

More information

Designing eye tracking experiments to measure human behavior

Designing eye tracking experiments to measure human behavior Designing eye tracking experiments to measure human behavior Eindhoven, The Netherlands August, 2010 Ricardo Matos Tobii Technology Steps involved in measuring behaviour 1. Formulate and initial question

More information

WMS III to WMS IV: Rationale for Change

WMS III to WMS IV: Rationale for Change Pearson Clinical Assessment 19500 Bulverde Rd San Antonio, TX, 28759 Telephone: 800 627 7271 www.pearsonassessments.com WMS III to WMS IV: Rationale for Change Since the publication of the Wechsler Memory

More information

User Eye Fatigue Detection via Eye Movement Behavior

User Eye Fatigue Detection via Eye Movement Behavior This is a pre-print User Eye Fatigue Detection via Eye Movement Behavior Evgeniy Abdulin Department of Computer Science Texas State University San Marcos, TX 78666 e_a146@txstate.edu Oleg Komogortsev Department

More information

An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers

An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers 언 어 치 료 연 구, 제14 권 제4호 Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders 2005, Vol.14, No.4, 29 ~ 36 An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers Hyo-Woon Yoon(Brain Science Research

More information

Brain Function, Spell Reading, and Sweep-Sweep-Spell by Abigail Marshall, March 2005

Brain Function, Spell Reading, and Sweep-Sweep-Spell by Abigail Marshall, March 2005 Brain Function, Spell Reading, and Sweep-Sweep-Spell by Abigail Marshall, March 2005 This is not phonics or a phonetic process; it is simply letter and word recognition. Ronald D. Davis T wo of the most

More information

Video-Based Eye Tracking

Video-Based Eye Tracking Video-Based Eye Tracking Our Experience with Advanced Stimuli Design for Eye Tracking Software A. RUFA, a G.L. MARIOTTINI, b D. PRATTICHIZZO, b D. ALESSANDRINI, b A. VICINO, b AND A. FEDERICO a a Department

More information

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention Logan Trujillo, Ph.D. Post-Doctoral Fellow University of Texas at Austin Cognitive Science Course, Fall 2008 What is Attention? Everyone knows what attention

More information

SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT. A Thesis

SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT. A Thesis SOURCE MEMORY AND THE PICTURE SUPERIORITY EFFECT A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Vestibular Assessment

Vestibular Assessment Oculomotor Examination A. Tests performed in room light Vestibular Assessment 1. Spontaneous nystagmus 2. Gaze holding nystagmus 3. Skew deviation 4. Vergence 5. Decreased vestibular ocular reflex i. Head

More information

GAZE STABILIZATION SYSTEMS Vestibular Ocular Reflex (VOR) Purpose of VOR Chief function is to stabilize gaze during locomotion. Acuity declines if

GAZE STABILIZATION SYSTEMS Vestibular Ocular Reflex (VOR) Purpose of VOR Chief function is to stabilize gaze during locomotion. Acuity declines if GAZE STABILIZATION SYSTEMS Vestibular Ocular Reflex (VOR) Purpose of VOR Chief function is to stabilize gaze during locomotion. Acuity declines if slip exceeds 3-5 deg/sec. Ex: Head bobbing and heel strike

More information

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain).

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). Functional neuroimaging Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). The brain is bloody & electric Blood increase in neuronal activity increase in metabolic demand for glucose

More information

Studying Human Face Recognition with the Gaze-Contingent Window Technique

Studying Human Face Recognition with the Gaze-Contingent Window Technique Studying Human Face Recognition with the Gaze-Contingent Window Technique Naing Naing Maw (nnmaw@cs.umb.edu) University of Massachusetts at Boston, Department of Computer Science 100 Morrissey Boulevard,

More information

BRAIN ASYMMETRY. Outline

BRAIN ASYMMETRY. Outline 2 BRAIN ASYMMETRY 3 Outline Left Brain vs. Right Brain? Anatomy of the Corpus Callosum Split Brain The Left Hemisphere The Right Hemisphere Lateralization of Emotion 1 Questionnaire 4 1. When you work

More information

PERSPECTIVE. How Top-Down is Visual Perception?

PERSPECTIVE. How Top-Down is Visual Perception? PERSPECTIVE How Top-Down is Visual Perception? featuring new data (VSS Poster): Attentional Cycles in Detecting Simple Events within Complex Displays Sunday PM Poster #36.301, VSS 2014 Thomas Sanocki,

More information

Tracking translation process: The impact of experience and training

Tracking translation process: The impact of experience and training Tracking translation process: The impact of experience and training PINAR ARTAR Izmir University, Turkey Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Spain The translation process can be described through eye tracking.

More information

The Effects of Reading Speed on Visual Search Task

The Effects of Reading Speed on Visual Search Task The Effects of Reading Speed on Visual Search Task Masaharu Kato (pieko@abmes.twmu.ac.jp) Tokyo Women s Medical University 8-1, Kawada-cho, Shinjuku-ku, Tokyo162-8666, JAPAN Mitsugu Kuriyama (kuri@cs.c.u-tokyo.ac.jp)

More information

Shifting views on the symbolic cueing effect: Cueing attention through recent prior experience

Shifting views on the symbolic cueing effect: Cueing attention through recent prior experience Psicológica (2008), 29, 97-114. Shifting views on the symbolic cueing effect: Cueing attention through recent prior experience Matthew J. C. Crump * (1), Bruce Milliken (1), & Imran Ansari (2) (1) McMaster

More information

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology 1.Obtaining Knowledge 1. Correlation 2. Causation 2.Hypothesis Generation & Measures 3.Looking into

More information

Tobii AB. Accuracy and precision Test report. Tobii Pro X3-120 fw 1.7.1. Date: 2015-09-14 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7*

Tobii AB. Accuracy and precision Test report. Tobii Pro X3-120 fw 1.7.1. Date: 2015-09-14 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7* Tobii AB Accuracy and precision Test report Tobii Pro X3-120 fw 1.7.1 Date: 2015-09-14 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7* 1. Introduction This document provides an overview of tests in terms of accuracy

More information

Sample Paper for Research Methods. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne

Sample Paper for Research Methods. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne Running head: RESEARCH METHODS PAPER 1 Sample Paper for Research Methods Daren H. Kaiser Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne Running head: RESEARCH METHODS PAPER 2 Abstract First notice that

More information

Age-Related Changes and the Attention Network Task: An Examination of Alerting, Orienting and, Executive Function

Age-Related Changes and the Attention Network Task: An Examination of Alerting, Orienting and, Executive Function LOGO Age-Related Changes and the Attention Network Task: An Examination of Alerting, Orienting and, Executive Function JANINE M. JENINGS, DALE DAGENBACH, CHRISTINE M. ENGLE AND LAURA J. FUNKE Reporter:

More information

Representing Vector Fields Using Field Line Diagrams

Representing Vector Fields Using Field Line Diagrams Minds On Physics Activity FFá2 5 Representing Vector Fields Using Field Line Diagrams Purpose and Expected Outcome One way of representing vector fields is using arrows to indicate the strength and direction

More information

Subjects. Subjects were undergraduates at the University of California, Santa Barbara, with

Subjects. Subjects were undergraduates at the University of California, Santa Barbara, with Category-specific visual attention 1 SI Appendix 1 Method Subjects. Subjects were undergraduates at the University of California, Santa Barbara, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exp 1: n=30;

More information

The Capacity of Visual Short- Term Memory Is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects G.A. Alvarez and P.

The Capacity of Visual Short- Term Memory Is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects G.A. Alvarez and P. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Capacity of Visual Short- Term Memory Is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects G.A. Alvarez and P. Cavanagh Harvard University ABSTRACT

More information

An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks

An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks Psychophysiology, 46 (2009), 771 775. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2009 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00814.x BRIEF REPORT An electrophysiological

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches to Thinking about the Mind Cognitive Neuroscience Evolutionary Approach Sept 20-22, 2004 Interdisciplinary approach Rapidly changing How does the brain enable cognition?

More information

Quantitative Analysis of Reading Comprehension and Reading Speed Based on Serif and Sans-Serif Fonts

Quantitative Analysis of Reading Comprehension and Reading Speed Based on Serif and Sans-Serif Fonts Quantitative Analysis of Reading Comprehension and Reading Speed Based on Serif and Sans-Serif Fonts Abstract Taylor Burke Clemson University tdburke@clemson.edu Our work investigates the effect of using

More information

Picture Memory Improves with Longer On Time and Off Time

Picture Memory Improves with Longer On Time and Off Time Journal ol Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 197S, Vol. 104, No. 2, 114-118 Picture Memory mproves with Longer On Time and Off Time Barbara Tversky and Tracy Sherman The Hebrew University

More information

Fairfield Public Schools

Fairfield Public Schools Mathematics Fairfield Public Schools AP Statistics AP Statistics BOE Approved 04/08/2014 1 AP STATISTICS Critical Areas of Focus AP Statistics is a rigorous course that offers advanced students an opportunity

More information

Time Window from Visual Images to Visual Short-Term Memory: Consolidation or Integration?

Time Window from Visual Images to Visual Short-Term Memory: Consolidation or Integration? Time Window from Visual Images to Visual Short-Term Memory: Consolidation or Integration? Yuhong Jiang Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA Abstract. When two dot arrays are briefly

More information

Filling a gap in the semantic gradient: Color associates and response set effects in the Stroop task

Filling a gap in the semantic gradient: Color associates and response set effects in the Stroop task Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (2),???-??? 310-315 Filling a gap in the semantic gradient: Color associates and response set effects in the Stroop task EVAN F. RISKO University of

More information

THE IMPORTANCE OF STIMULUS-RESPONSE RULES IN SEQUENCE LEARNING. A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty. Hillary Schwarb

THE IMPORTANCE OF STIMULUS-RESPONSE RULES IN SEQUENCE LEARNING. A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty. Hillary Schwarb THE IMPORTANCE OF STIMULUS-RESPONSE RULES IN SEQUENCE LEARNING A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty By Hillary Schwarb In Partial Fulfillment Of Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in Psychology

More information

9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Single Factor design. Single design experiment. Experimental design. Textbook Chapters

9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Single Factor design. Single design experiment. Experimental design. Textbook Chapters 9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition Fall 2009 Single factor design Textbook Chapters Chapter 5: Types of variables Chapter 8: Controls Chapter 7: Validity Chapter 11: Single factor design Single design

More information

Vision: Receptors. Modes of Perception. Vision: Summary 9/28/2012. How do we perceive our environment? Sensation and Perception Terminology

Vision: Receptors. Modes of Perception. Vision: Summary 9/28/2012. How do we perceive our environment? Sensation and Perception Terminology How do we perceive our environment? Complex stimuli are broken into individual features, relayed to the CNS, then reassembled as our perception Sensation and Perception Terminology Stimulus: physical agent

More information

Direct Evidence Delay with A Task Decreases Working Memory Content in Free Recall

Direct Evidence Delay with A Task Decreases Working Memory Content in Free Recall 1 Direct Evidence Delay with A Task Decreases Working Memory Content in Free Recall Eugen Tarnow, Ph.D. 1 18-11 Radburn Road, Fair Lawn, NJ 07410, USA etarnow@avabiz.com 1 The author is an independent

More information

Research Methods & Experimental Design

Research Methods & Experimental Design Research Methods & Experimental Design 16.422 Human Supervisory Control April 2004 Research Methods Qualitative vs. quantitative Understanding the relationship between objectives (research question) and

More information

A Guided User Experience Using Subtle Gaze Direction

A Guided User Experience Using Subtle Gaze Direction A Guided User Experience Using Subtle Gaze Direction Eli Ben-Joseph and Eric Greenstein Stanford University {ebj, ecgreens}@stanford.edu 1 Abstract This paper demonstrates how illumination modulation can

More information

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex 1 Neuroscience 2 Neurons output integration axon cell body, membrane potential Frontal planning control auditory episodes soma motor Temporal Parietal action language objects space vision Occipital inputs

More information

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Interaction: memory experiment

9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science. Interaction: memory experiment 9.63 Laboratory in Cognitive Science Fall 25 Lecture 6 Factorial Design: Complex design Aude Oliva Ben Balas, Charles Kemp Interaction: memory experiment Goal: In an experiment, you compare the explicit

More information

Sample Paper for Learning Research Proposal. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne

Sample Paper for Learning Research Proposal. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne Running head: RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1 Sample Paper for Learning Research Proposal Daren H. Kaiser Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne RESEARCH PROPOSAL 2 Abstract Notice that you do not indent

More information

Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory

Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory Review 119 Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory Edward Awh and John Jonides Spatial selective attention and spatial working memory have largely been studied in isolation. Studies

More information

Tobii Technology AB. Accuracy and precision Test report. X2-60 fw 1.0.5. Date: 2013-08-16 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7

Tobii Technology AB. Accuracy and precision Test report. X2-60 fw 1.0.5. Date: 2013-08-16 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7 Tobii Technology AB Accuracy and precision Test report X2-60 fw 1.0.5 Date: 2013-08-16 Methodology/Software version: 2.1.7 1. Introduction This document provides an overview of tests in terms of accuracy

More information

Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org)

Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) Title The verbal overshadowing effect in memory for pictures Author 伊 東, 裕 司 (Ito, Yuji) 佐 々 木, 璃 恵 (Sasaki, Rie) Hisamatsu, Takeshi(Hisamatsu, Takeshi) 舘, 瑞 恵 (Tachi,

More information

Skill acquisition. Skill acquisition: Closed loop theory Feedback guides learning a motor skill. Problems. Motor learning practice

Skill acquisition. Skill acquisition: Closed loop theory Feedback guides learning a motor skill. Problems. Motor learning practice Motor learning theories closed loop theory schema theory hierarchical theory Skill acquisition Motor learning practice Fitt s three stages motor imagery physical changes Skill acquisition: Closed loop

More information

Factorial Design. A factorial design. 9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Effect of Attraction x Emotion

Factorial Design. A factorial design. 9.63 Laboratory in Visual Cognition. Effect of Attraction x Emotion 9.63 aboratory in Visual Cognition Fall 29 Factorial Design & Interaction Factorial Design Two or more independent variables Simplest case: a 2 x 2 design (2 factors and 2 conditions per factor) A factorial

More information

Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus

Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus Our brains are maps. This mapping results from the way connections in the brain are ordered and arranged. The ordering of neural pathways between different parts of the

More information

Doppler. Doppler. Doppler shift. Doppler Frequency. Doppler shift. Doppler shift. Chapter 19

Doppler. Doppler. Doppler shift. Doppler Frequency. Doppler shift. Doppler shift. Chapter 19 Doppler Doppler Chapter 19 A moving train with a trumpet player holding the same tone for a very long time travels from your left to your right. The tone changes relative the motion of you (receiver) and

More information

Fixation Biases towards the Index Finger in Almost-Natural Grasping

Fixation Biases towards the Index Finger in Almost-Natural Grasping RESEARCH ARTICLE Fixation Biases towards the Index Finger in Almost-Natural Grasping Dimitris Voudouris 1,2 *, Jeroen B. J. Smeets 1, Eli Brenner 1 1 Department of Human Movement Sciences, VU University

More information

Long-term Memory for 400 Pictures on a CommonTheme

Long-term Memory for 400 Pictures on a CommonTheme Long-term Memory for 400 Pictures on a CommonTheme Stine Vogt a* and Svein Magnussen, a,b a Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway b Centre for Advanced Study, The Norwegian Academy of Science

More information

Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition

Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 2057 2073 Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition Stefan R. Schweinberger, Esther C. Pickering, A. Mike Burton, Jürgen M. Kaufmann

More information

FREE FALL. Introduction. Reference Young and Freedman, University Physics, 12 th Edition: Chapter 2, section 2.5

FREE FALL. Introduction. Reference Young and Freedman, University Physics, 12 th Edition: Chapter 2, section 2.5 Physics 161 FREE FALL Introduction This experiment is designed to study the motion of an object that is accelerated by the force of gravity. It also serves as an introduction to the data analysis capabilities

More information

Motion processing: the most sensitive detectors differ in temporally localized and extended noise

Motion processing: the most sensitive detectors differ in temporally localized and extended noise ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 15 May 2014 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00426 Motion processing: the most sensitive detectors differ in temporally localized and extended noise Rémy Allard 1,2,3 * and

More information

ACTUARIAL CONSIDERATIONS IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGENT CONTINGENT COMPENSATION PROGRAMS

ACTUARIAL CONSIDERATIONS IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGENT CONTINGENT COMPENSATION PROGRAMS ACTUARIAL CONSIDERATIONS IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF AGENT CONTINGENT COMPENSATION PROGRAMS Contingent compensation plans are developed by insurers as a tool to provide incentives to agents to obtain certain

More information

JOINT ATTENTION. Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010

JOINT ATTENTION. Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010 JOINT ATTENTION Kaplan and Hafner (2006) Florian Niefind Coli, Universität des Saarlandes SS 2010 1 1 1.Outline 2.Joint attention - an informal approximation 3.Motivation of the paper 4.Formalization of

More information

Adult cognition of large-scale geometric facts

Adult cognition of large-scale geometric facts Roberto Casati, David Mark, Ira Noveck Adult cognition of large-scale geometric facts Draft 4, April 1999 Project description Objectives 1. Adult cognition of large-scale geometric facts The primary objective

More information

Blinded Abstract Body

Blinded Abstract Body Blinded Abstract Body Title: Eye-movements in Oral and Silent Reading and Reading Proficiency for Beginning Readers Purpose Silent reading is the primary mode of reading for proficient readers, and proficient

More information

Does the language we speak affect our perception of the

Does the language we speak affect our perception of the Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left Aubrey L. Gilbert*, Terry Regier, Paul Kay **, and Richard B. Ivry* *Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute and Departments of Psychology

More information

CELL PHONE INDUCED PERCEPTUAL IMPAIRMENTS DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

CELL PHONE INDUCED PERCEPTUAL IMPAIRMENTS DURING SIMULATED DRIVING CELL PHONE INDUCED PERCEPTUAL IMPAIRMENTS DURING SIMULATED DRIVING David L. Strayer, Frank A. Drews, Robert W. Albert, and William A. Johnston Department of Psychology University of Utah Salt Lake City,

More information

6 Space Perception and Binocular Vision

6 Space Perception and Binocular Vision Space Perception and Binocular Vision Space Perception and Binocular Vision space perception monocular cues to 3D space binocular vision and stereopsis combining depth cues monocular/pictorial cues cues

More information

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing Birgit Quante and Lothar Mühlbach Heinrich-Hertz-Institut für Nachrichtentechnik Berlin GmbH (HHI) Einsteinufer 37, D-15087 Berlin, Germany, http://www.hhi.de/

More information

Effects of CEO turnover on company performance

Effects of CEO turnover on company performance Headlight International Effects of CEO turnover on company performance CEO turnover in listed companies has increased over the past decades. This paper explores whether or not changing CEO has a significant

More information

Effects of Negation, Truth Value, and Delay on Picture Recognition after Reading Affirmative and Negative Sentences

Effects of Negation, Truth Value, and Delay on Picture Recognition after Reading Affirmative and Negative Sentences Effects of Negation, Truth Value, and Delay on Picture Recognition after Reading Affirmative and Negative Sentences Barbara Kaup (barbara.kaup@tu-berlin.de) Jana Lüdtke (janaluedtke@gp.tu-berlin.de) Department

More information

Eye Movement Control During Reading: Fixation Measures Reflect Foveal but Not Parafoveal Processing Difficulty

Eye Movement Control During Reading: Fixation Measures Reflect Foveal but Not Parafoveal Processing Difficulty Eye Movement Control During Reading: Fixation Measures Reflect Foveal but Not Parafoveal Processing Difficulty JOHN M. HENDERSON and FERNANDA FERREIRA University of Alberta Abstract The main purpose of

More information

A Review of the Hosman and Van der Vaart Tracking Experiment. Delft University of Technology, P.O. Box 5058, 2600 GB Delft, The Netherlands

A Review of the Hosman and Van der Vaart Tracking Experiment. Delft University of Technology, P.O. Box 5058, 2600 GB Delft, The Netherlands AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference and Exhibit 2-23 August 27, Hilton Head, South Carolina AIAA 27-6896 A Review of the Hosman and Van der Vaart Tracking Experiment D.M. Pool, M. Mulder,

More information

Eye Movement Assessment of Selective Attentional Capture by Emotional Pictures

Eye Movement Assessment of Selective Attentional Capture by Emotional Pictures Emotion Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 6, No. 2, 257 268 1528-3542/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.257 Eye Movement Assessment of Selective Attentional Capture

More information

Oculomotor Exploration of Impossible Figures in Early Infancy

Oculomotor Exploration of Impossible Figures in Early Infancy Infancy, 18(2), 221 232, 2013 Copyright Ó International Society on Infant Studies (ISIS) ISSN: 1525-0008 print / 1532-7078 online DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00115.x Oculomotor Exploration of Impossible

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Skilled Deaf Readers have an Enhanced Perceptual Span in Reading

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Skilled Deaf Readers have an Enhanced Perceptual Span in Reading Skilled Deaf Readers have an Enhanced Perceptual Span in Reading Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI--0.R Manuscript Type: Research article Date Submitted by the Author: n/a Complete List

More information

Working Memory Capacity and Attention Network Test Performance

Working Memory Capacity and Attention Network Test Performance APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 20: 713 721 (2006) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/acp.1224 Working Memory Capacity and Attention Network

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

CATALYST REPORT DebateHub and Collective Intelligence Dashboard Testing University of Naples Federico II

CATALYST REPORT DebateHub and Collective Intelligence Dashboard Testing University of Naples Federico II CATALYST REPORT DebateHub and Collective Intelligence Dashboard Testing University of Naples Federico II 1. Overview: Information about Community Partner Involved 1.1. Resumé This test was carried out

More information

Problem-Based Group Activities for a Sensation & Perception Course. David S. Kreiner. University of Central Missouri

Problem-Based Group Activities for a Sensation & Perception Course. David S. Kreiner. University of Central Missouri -Based Group Activities for a Course David S. Kreiner University of Central Missouri Author contact information: David Kreiner Professor of Psychology University of Central Missouri Lovinger 1111 Warrensburg

More information

Convention Paper Presented at the 118th Convention 2005 May 28 31 Barcelona, Spain

Convention Paper Presented at the 118th Convention 2005 May 28 31 Barcelona, Spain Audio Engineering Society Convention Paper Presented at the 118th Convention 25 May 28 31 Barcelona, Spain 6431 This convention paper has been reproduced from the author s advance manuscript, without editing,

More information

Statistics Review PSY379

Statistics Review PSY379 Statistics Review PSY379 Basic concepts Measurement scales Populations vs. samples Continuous vs. discrete variable Independent vs. dependent variable Descriptive vs. inferential stats Common analyses

More information

Continuous Performance Test 3 rd Edition. C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Continuous Performance Test 3 rd Edition. C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Continuous Performance Test 3 rd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Assessment Report Name/ID: Alexandra Sample Age: 16 Gender: Female Birth Date: February 16, 1998 Grade: 11 Administration Date: February

More information

Fixplot Instruction Manual. (data plotting program)

Fixplot Instruction Manual. (data plotting program) Fixplot Instruction Manual (data plotting program) MANUAL VERSION2 2004 1 1. Introduction The Fixplot program is a component program of Eyenal that allows the user to plot eye position data collected with

More information

Module 13 : Measurements on Fiber Optic Systems

Module 13 : Measurements on Fiber Optic Systems Module 13 : Measurements on Fiber Optic Systems Lecture : Measurements on Fiber Optic Systems Objectives In this lecture you will learn the following Measurements on Fiber Optic Systems Attenuation (Loss)

More information

Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding?

Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding? Memory & Cognition 1977, Vol. 5 (2),252-256 Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding? PAUL R. D'AGOSTINO Gettysburg CoUege, Gettysburg, Pennsylvania 17925

More information

GCSE COMBINED SCIENCE: TRILOGY

GCSE COMBINED SCIENCE: TRILOGY SPECIMEN MATERIAL GCSE COMBINED SCIENCE: TRILOGY PAPER 6: PHYSICS 2H Mark scheme Specimen 208 Version.0 Mark schemes are prepared by the Lead Assessment Writer and considered, together with the relevant

More information

Chapter 8: Perceiving Depth and Size

Chapter 8: Perceiving Depth and Size Chapter 8: Perceiving Depth and Size Cues to Depth Perception Oculomotor - cues based on sensing the position of the eyes and muscle tension 1. Convergence knowing the inward movement of the eyes when

More information

EEG COHERENCE AND PHASE DELAYS: COMPARISONS BETWEEN SINGLE REFERENCE, AVERAGE REFERENCE AND CURRENT SOURCE DENSITY

EEG COHERENCE AND PHASE DELAYS: COMPARISONS BETWEEN SINGLE REFERENCE, AVERAGE REFERENCE AND CURRENT SOURCE DENSITY Version 1, June 13, 2004 Rough Draft form We apologize while we prepare the manuscript for publication but the data are valid and the conclusions are fundamental EEG COHERENCE AND PHASE DELAYS: COMPARISONS

More information

Improvement of Visual Attention and Working Memory through a Web-based Cognitive Training Program

Improvement of Visual Attention and Working Memory through a Web-based Cognitive Training Program . Improvement of Visual Attention and Working Memory through a Web-based Cognitive Training Program Michael Scanlon David Drescher Kunal Sarkar Context: Prior work has revealed that cognitive ability is

More information

Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults

Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults Implications for cochlear implantation outcome D Lazard, HJ Lee, E Truy, AL Giraud Ecole Normale Supérieure, Inserm U960,

More information

SR2000 FREQUENCY MONITOR

SR2000 FREQUENCY MONITOR SR2000 FREQUENCY MONITOR THE FFT SEARCH FUNCTION IN DETAILS FFT Search is a signal search using FFT (Fast Fourier Transform) technology. The FFT search function first appeared with the SR2000 Frequency

More information

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT

HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT HOW TO WRITE A LABORATORY REPORT Pete Bibby Dept of Psychology 1 About Laboratory Reports The writing of laboratory reports is an essential part of the practical course One function of this course is to

More information

PERCEPTUAL RECOGNITION AS A FUNCTION OF MEANINGFULNESS OF STIMULUS MATERIAL '

PERCEPTUAL RECOGNITION AS A FUNCTION OF MEANINGFULNESS OF STIMULUS MATERIAL ' Journal of Experimental Psychology 1969, Vol. 81, No. 2, 275-280 PERCEPTUAL RECOGNITION AS A FUNCTION OF MEANINGFULNESS OF STIMULUS MATERIAL ' GERALD M. REICHER 8 University of Michigan The present study

More information

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 29 (2008) Indiana University

RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 29 (2008) Indiana University RESEARCH ON SPOKEN LANGUAGE PROCESSING Progress Report No. 29 (2008) Indiana University A Software-Based System for Synchronizing and Preprocessing Eye Movement Data in Preparation for Analysis 1 Mohammad

More information

Package gazepath. April 1, 2015

Package gazepath. April 1, 2015 Type Package Package gazepath April 1, 2015 Title Gazepath Transforms Eye-Tracking Data into Fixations and Saccades Version 1.0 Date 2015-03-03 Author Daan van Renswoude Maintainer Daan van Renswoude

More information

Lecture 2, Human cognition

Lecture 2, Human cognition Human Cognition An important foundation for the design of interfaces is a basic theory of human cognition The information processing paradigm (in its most simple form). Human Information Processing The

More information

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics Claire Moore-Cantwell, Joe Pater, Robert Staubs, Benjamin Zobel and Lisa Sanders RUMMIT UMass Amherst, April 6th 2013 Introduction: learning phonology in the lab

More information

Brain Training Influence. Cognitive Function Effectiveness. Boiron Labs

Brain Training Influence. Cognitive Function Effectiveness. Boiron Labs Brain Training Influence on Cognitive Function Effectiveness at Boiron Labs By Bernard Croisile (1), Gilles Reilhac (2), Sandrine Bélier (2), Franck Tarpin-Bernard (2) & Michel Noir (2) (1) Centre Mémoire

More information

research/scientific includes the following: statistical hypotheses: you have a null and alternative you accept one and reject the other

research/scientific includes the following: statistical hypotheses: you have a null and alternative you accept one and reject the other 1 Hypothesis Testing Richard S. Balkin, Ph.D., LPC-S, NCC 2 Overview When we have questions about the effect of a treatment or intervention or wish to compare groups, we use hypothesis testing Parametric

More information

Correlation and response relevance in sequence learning

Correlation and response relevance in sequence learning DOI 10.1007/s00426-012-0444-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Correlation and response relevance in sequence learning Josephine Cock Beat Meier Received: 1 February 2012 / Accepted: 12 June 2012 Ó Springer-Verlag 2012

More information