Romantic involvement, self-discrepancy, and psychological well-being: A preliminary investigation



Similar documents
SATISFACTION WITH LIFE SCALE

INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY TRAINING ON INCREASED HARDINESS AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING

1998, Vol. 74, No. 2, /98/$3.00. Andrew J. Elliot University of Rochester

The Three Faces of Self-Esteem. Jonathon D. Brown. University of Washington. and. Margaret A. Marshall. Seattle Pacific University

Mental Health Professionals Attitudes Towards People Who Are Deaf

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan

Expectancy Value Theory: Motivating Healthcare Workers

Educational Psychology Specializations in Counseling Psychology and Program Evaluation

Factors of Adolescent Self-Concept: Mass Mediated, Peer, and Family Communication. David J. Feliciano. University of West Florida

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data

Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships

BRIAN KEITH PAYNE Curriculum Vitae EDUCATION. Ph.D., Psychology. Major area: Social Psychology, Washington University, 2002.

Better Than Better-Than-Average (or Not): Elevated and Depressed Self-evaluations Following Unfavorable Social Comparisons

ORIGINAL ATTACHMENT THREE-CATEGORY MEASURE

A Study on Customer Orientation as Mediator between Emotional Intelligence and Service Performance in Banks

Introduction to Social Psychology: Psychology 260, Section 005 SAMPLE SYLLABUS

Guide to Outcome Measures

Organizational Change Management: A Test of the Effectiveness of a Communication Plan

Spirituality and Moral Development Among Students at a Christian College Krista M. Hernandez

Running head: BODY ART AND ACADEMIC SUCCESS 1

Studying Gender and Ethnic Differences in Participation in Math, Physical Science, and Information Technology

Psychology 596a Graduate Seminar In Social Psychology Topic: Attitudes And Persuasion

Levi R. Baker. Mobile: Web:

EXAMINING HEALTHCARE PROFESSIONALS ACCEPTANCE OF ELECTRONIC MEDICAL RECORDS USING UTAUT

PSYCHOLOGY PROGRAM LEARNING GOALS, LEARNING OUTCOMES AND COURSE ALLIGNMENT MATRIX. 8 Oct. 2010

Maximizing and customer loyalty: Are maximizers less loyal?

Self-Discrepancies and Vulnerability to Body Dissatisfaction and Disordered Eating

Gender Differences in Employed Job Search Lindsey Bowen and Jennifer Doyle, Furman University

ABSTRACT OF THE DOCTORAL THESIS BY Cătălin Ovidiu Obuf Buhăianu

The Relationship between Ethnicity and Academic Success in Online Education Courses

Overview of Factor Analysis

The relationship between goals, metacognition, and academic success

The Influence of Parent-Child Attachment on Romantic Relationships

Usefulness of expected values in liability valuation: the role of portfolio size

Undergraduate Psychology Major Learning Goals and Outcomes i

The Effect of Online Social Networking on Facilitating Sense of Belonging among University Students Living Off Campus

Procrastination in Online Courses: Performance and Attitudinal Differences

Melissa Gutworth's Search for a PhD and the IAT

An Examination of the Association Between Parental Abuse History and Subsequent Parent-Child Relationships

THE ROCHESTER INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION. Brand Loyalty: College Student Loyalty to Brands with Social Media Outlets

Edward Burkley present Associate Professor, Oklahoma State University Psychology Department

Impact of attendance policies on course attendance among college students

M.A. PSYCHOLOGY FIRST YEAR COURSES (MAPC)

STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK COLLEGE OF TECHNOLOGY CANTON, NEW YORK COURSE OUTLINE PSYC 340 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 1, 2009, pp. 1-8

Leadership Frames and Perceptions of Effectiveness among Health Information Management Program Directors

Body Image, Eating Disorders and Psychiatric Comorbidity:

Principal s Personal Characteristics and Conflict Management in Kwara State Secondary Schools, Nigeria

I have no desire at all to leave psychology hanging in the air with no organic basis. But, beyond a feeling of conviction [that there must be such a

Impact of Self Concept on Scholastic Achievement of 9th Class Students in Physical Sciences

Assessment Plan Henderson State University

PSYCHOLOGY PROGRAM LEARNING GOALS AND OUTCOMES BY COURSE LISTING

Rachel A. Klein, Psy.D Licensed Clinical Psychologist (610)

The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys

1/27/2013. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2

The Effects of Parent Trust on Perceived Influence and School Involvement

Applied Psychology. Dr. Marya Howell-Carter, Acting Chair Psychology Dept. Bachelor of Science Degree

Ideals, Perceptions, and Evaluations in Early Relationship Development

The Inventory of Male Friendliness in Nursing Programs (IMFNP)

Judith Ann Perry. San Antonio, Texas (210)

Rede ning medical students' disease to reduce morbidity

Course Descriptions Psychology

Running head: SAMPLE FOR STUDENTS 1. Sample APA Paper for Students Interested in Learning APA Style 6th Edition. Jeffrey H. Kahn

Belief Formation in the Returns to Schooling

Self-efficacy, self-determination and victim blaming as predictors of adolescent sexual victimization

High procedural fairness heightens the effect of outcome favorability on self-evaluations: An attributional analysis

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Psychology. The Effects of Facebook on Jealousy in Relationships Jennifer L. Eby. This paper was written for Dr. Kraemer s Survey Research course.

Under the Start Your Search Now box, you may search by author, title and key words.

Model of Learning Organizational Development of Primary School Network under the Office of Basic Education Commission

1998, Vol. 74, No. 5, /98/$3.00

Validation of the Core Self-Evaluations Scale research instrument in the conditions of Slovak Republic

Correlational Research

Use of social media by college students: Relationship to communication and self-concept

Jessica Miller and Brandi Tedder. Hanover College. PSY 401: Advanced Research. Fall 2011

Research Basis for Catchup Math

In the past two decades, the federal government has dramatically

LOVE ATTITUDES SCALE

Introducing Social Psychology

PSYCHOLOGY DEPARTMENT GOALS, OBJECTIVES, AND MEASURES

The Relationship between Low Self-Esteem and Alcoholism. by Mindy Checkon

Best friends forever?: High school best friendships and the transition to college

World Scientific News

Effects of Divorce on Theories of Relationships. Derik Orschell. Hanover College

International Tourism Market Segmentation Based on Consumer Behavior

Running head: THE EFFECTS OF EXTRA-CURRICULAR ACTIVITIES

This historical document is derived from a 1990 APA presidential task force (revised in 1997).

Self-Other Agreement on a 360-Degree Leadership Evaluation. Stefanie K. Halverson Rice University. Scott Tonidandel Rice University

Current Problems and Resolutions. The Relative Effects of Competence and Likability on Interpersonal Attraction

Effectiveness of positive psychology training in the increase of hardiness of female headed households

STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK College at Cortland Department of Psychology

SEM Analysis of the Impact of Knowledge Management, Total Quality Management and Innovation on Organizational Performance

THE LONG WEEKEND: TRANSITION AND GROWTH IN RETIREMENT. M Cecil Smith

THE EFFECT OF SPIRITUALITY ON WORKPLACE ETHICS

Open and Distance Learning Student Retention: A Case Study of the University of Papua New Guinea Open College

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life

High School Counselors Influence

Report on the Ontario Principals Council Leadership Study

Anxiety, Self-Efficacy, and College Exam Grades

PSY 013: Social Psychology

Transcription:

Personal Relationships, 1 (1994), 399-404. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright 1994 Cambridge University Press. 1350 4126/94 $5.00 +.00 BRIEF REPORT Romantic involvement, self-discrepancy, and psychological well-being: A preliminary investigation W. KEITH CAMPBELL," CONSTANTINE SEDIKIDES, a AND JENNIFER BOSSON b "University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; and b University of Texas at Austin Abstract In this study, we explored the relation between romantic involvement status, actual/ideal self-discrepancy, and psychological well-being. We hypothesized that romantically involved individuals would report being closer to their ideal selves than would romantically uninvolved individuals. We also hypothesized that the reduced self-discrepancy reported by romantically involved individuals would be related to their experiencing higher levels of psychological well-being in comparison to romantically uninvolved individuals. Romantically involved and romantically uninvolved subjects rated their actual selves, closeness to their ideal selves, and psychological well-being. Results were consistent with both hypotheses. Although romantically involved and romantically uninvolved subjects did not differ in ratings of their actual selves, romantically involved subjects reported being significantly closer to their ideal selves. Furthermore, romantically involved subjects tended to report higher psychological wellbeing. Although a great deal of research has examined the precursors, correlates, and consequences of romantic involvement (Brehm, 1992), very little research has focused on the relation between romantic involvement and the self-concept, hereafter referred to as the self (i.e., the individual's perception of his or her attributes). The objective of the present study is to explore whether romantic involvement is associated with changes in the self. More specifically, the present study at-tempts to provide preliminary answers to two questions: (1) Is romantic involvement This article is partially based on data presented at the 1993 Conference of the International Network of Personal Relationships. Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to W. Keith Campbell, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, CB 3270, Davie Hall, Chapel Hill, NC, 27599-3270. associated with the magnitude of actual/ideal selfdiscrepancy? (2) Is the magnitude of actual/ideal selfdiscrepancy linked to overall psychological wellbeing? The actual/ideal self-discrepancy (here-after referred to as "self-discrepancy") has been defined as the measurable difference between an individual's beliefs about who he actually thinks he is (actual self) and his image of the person he would ideally like to be (ideal self) (Higgins, 1987). For example, an individual who earns $30,000 a year and ideally sees herself as making $100,000 a year has a rather large self-discrepancy. Conversely, an individual who makes $30,000 a year and ideally sees herself as making $30,000 a year has no self-discrepancy. Furthermore, self-discrepancy, be-cause it consists of the difference between the actual self and the ideal self, can be 399

400 W.K. Campbell, C. Sedikides, and J. Bosson changed by altering at least one of the two selves: that is, a decrease in self-discrepancy can result from an increase in the actual self or a decrease in the ideal self. Although selfdiscrepancy has been a prolific topic in psychological research (for reviews, see Higgins, 1987, 1989), the relation between romantic involvement and self-discrepancy has not yet been empirically explored. In the present study we set forth two hypotheses. First, romantically involved individuals will report smaller self-discrepancies than will romantically uninvolved individuals. Second, the lower self-discrepancy experienced by romantically involved individuals will be associated with greater psychological well-being. These hypotheses reflect the exploratory nature of this investigation in that, instead of being derived from one predominant theoretical position, they reflect insights gleaned from a range of theories, as well as speculation. Consistent with our first hypothesis are six formulations, which can be divided into two general classes: one class that predicts a decrease in self-discrepancy via an increase in ratings of the actual self (formulations 1 and 2) and another class that predicts a de-crease in self-discrepancy via a decrease in the ideal self (formulations 3-6). 1 A description of the six formulations follows. 1. Self-expansion theory (Aron & Aron, 1986; Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991) focuses on the expansion of the self that accompanies involvement in close relationships. Although self-expansion theory does not make any concrete predictions regarding self-discrepancy, it is possible that the self, as it expands, becomes closer to the ideal self with a consequent reduction in self-discrepancy. 2. One suggested extension of interdependence theory, promotive interdependence, is defined as the process by which one relationship partner facilitates, through expectancy processes and behavioral interaction, the other partner becoming his or her ideal self (Rusbult & Buunk, 1993). This analysis suggests that romantically involved individuals who are engaged in promotive relationships will experience a reduction in selfdiscrepancy over the course of the relationship as their actual self-concept becomes closer to their ideal self. This process, although theoretically relevant, is not directly examined in the current study because there is no measure of the degree to which the romantically involved individuals studied are engaged in promotively interdependent relationships. 3. The psychodynamic formulation set forth by Freud (1922/1959) and Reik (1944) states that, when an individual falls in love, his or her egoideal is projected onto the partner, and thus the discrepancy between the ego and ego-ideal is reduced via a perceived reduction in the ego-ideal. This formulation, how-ever, is concerned (a) with love rather than romantic involvement, and (b) with the ego-ideal, a construct that does not directly correspond to the ideal self. 4. Romantic involvement can be seen as a process of relinquishing individual level ideals, goals, and desires to obtain and maintain a state of relatedness, security, and intimacy with another individual (Campbell, Elliott, Reeder, & Sedikides, 1994; Guisinger & Blatt, 1994; Sedikides, Oliver, & Campbell, 1994; Sedikides & Strube, 1994; see also Sedikides, 1993). Romantically in-volved individuals, then, should be expected to place less importance on 1. It is possible that self-discrepancy in romantic relationships may be reduced by other, more complex processes involving a simultaneous change in the actual and ideal self. If, for example, the actual self is decreased only moderately and the ideal self is de - creased to a larger extent, the result would be an overall decrease in self-discrepancy. Simultaneous change in the actual and ideal self is especially likely if the two selves are nonindependent. That is, a change in one self would sometimes bring about a change in the other self.

Self-discrepancy 3 individual centered ideals, which may result in a compromise (i.e., diminishing) of the ideal self and therefore a reduction in reported self-discrepancy. 5. Romantically involved individuals may believe that their partners are involved with them because of who they are and not because of who they may be-come. Thus, these individuals may not feel as strongly the need to attain the ideals they once held. This process would lead to a reduction in the ideal self and a corresponding reduction in selfdiscrepancy. 6. Romantically involved individuals may become disillusioned after getting to know their partners and thus reduce to more realistic levels their goals and ideals in all other aspects of their lives. Thus, self-discrepancy would be reduced as a result of a reduction in the ideal self. Consistent with our second hypothesis namely, that a decrease in self-discrepancy leads to increased psychological well-being is the work of Higgins and colleagues (Higgins, 1987, 1989; Higgins, Bond, Klein, & Strauman, 1986; Higgins, Vookles, & Tyko - cinski,1992). These researchers have shown that self-discrepancy is linked to emotional vulnerability; that is, the greater the discrepancy between actual and ideal selves, the greater the individual's risk of experiencing dejection-related emotions such as disappointment, dissatisfaction, and frustration. Emotional vulnerability and psychological well-being are not isomorphic constructs, the former referring to the likelihood of experiencing certain negative emotional states (Higgins, 1987) and the latter referring to a more global, positive or negative experience of life (Ryff,1989). Arguably, however, those individuals who experience feelings of dis - appointment, dissatisfaction, and frustration repeatedly in their lives will also report lower psychological well-being in comparison to individ uals who do not experience these negative emotional states. Methods Subjects were 128 (60 men and 68 women, Mage = 19.8) romantically involved (i.e., dating) and 54 (30 men and 24 women, Mage = 19.6) romantically uninvolved, predomi nantly (97%) white, heterosexual, second-and third-year undergraduate students at the University of Wisconsin Madison. The roughly 2% of subjects who reported being homosexual were not included in the study because they constituted a small sample size. Romantically involved subjects had been involved with their present partner an average of 8 months and reported an aver-age relationship satisfaction score of 28.5 out of 35 on the Relationship Assessment Scale 2 (Hendrick, 1988). Subjects were not preselected on the basis of romantic involvement status; this was assessed at the end of the study. Subjects completed two questionnaires. The first of these was Pelham and Swann's (1989) Self-Attributes Questionnaire, which asks subjects to rate both actual self and closeness to ideal self on the following 10 attributes: intellectual ability, social competence, artistic ability, athletic ability, physical attractiveness, leadership ability, common sense, emotional stability, sense of humor, and discipline. Subjects indicated, on a 10-point scale ranging from "bottom 5%" to "top 5%," what percentile they would fall into along each of these 10 attributes compared to their peers (actual self-rating). Subjects further indicated, on a 10-point scale ranging from "very short of my ideal self" to "very much like my ideal self," the degree to which they differed from their ideal self along each of the 10 attributes (selfdiscrepancy rating). Both the reliability and the validity of this ques tionnaire have been demonstrated in college-age samples (Pelham & Swann, 1989). 2. We conducted ancillary analyses to determine if scores on the Relationship Assessment Scale predicted reported self-discrepancy in romantically in-volved subjects. No statistically significant predictive relation was found.

W.K. Campbell, C. Sedikides, and J. Bosson The second questionnaire was the Scales of Psychological Well-Being (Ryff, 1989) in which subjects rated their level of well-being along 6 domains: positive relationships with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. Subjects responded to a total of 84 statements on a scale ranging from (1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree. The reliability and validity of this questionnaire have been demonstrated in college-age and older samples (Ryff,1989). Results We used multiple regression analyses to test both hypotheses. We effects -coded all dichotomous variables (i.e., 1, - 1) and centered all predictor variables (i.e., for each variable we subtracted the mean from each value, thus creating a new variable with a mean of zero) (Aiken & West, 1991). We tested the first hypothesis (i.e., romantically involved individuals have smaller self-dis - crepancies than do romantically uninvolved individuals) by using two dichotomous variables, romantic involvement status (romantically involved, romantically uninvolved) and sex (female, male), as well as the inter-action term, as predictors. Selfdiscrepancy served as the criterion variable. The predicted main effect of romantic involvement status on selfdiscrepancy was obtained: romantically involved subjects (M = 73.82) rated themselves as being significantly closer to their ideal selves than romantically uninvolved subjects (M = 67.43), b = 3.20, p =.009. Neither the sex main effect nor the interaction was significant. We wondered whether the smaller selfdiscrepancy reported by romantically in - volved individuals was simply the result of romantically involved subjects feeling closer to their ideal s elves on the attributes of "social competence" and/or "physical attractiveness," given that these two attributes are associated with success in interpersonal relationships (Reis, Wheeler, Spiegel, Kernis, Nezlek, & Perry, 1982). This possibility was not supported. When we replaced overall self-discrepancy with each of the 10 attributes as criterion variables in the above model, we found that romantic involvement status significantly (or marginally significantly) predicted not only physical attractiveness, b =.44, p =.026, but also leader-ship ability, b =.38,p =.051, discipline, b =.32,p =.092, and athletic ability b =.58,p =.008. Neither social competence nor any of the remaining 5 attributes were significantly predicted in this equation. A reduction in self-discrepancy can be the result of at least two processes: an in-crease in the actual self or a decrease in the ideal self. To gain some insight as to which of these two processes was at work, we examined the difference in actual self-ratings reported by romantically involved and romantically uninvolved subjects. The logic behind this analysis was as follows. If there is a difference in the reported self-discrepancy of romantically involved and romantically uninvolved subjects, but no difference in these two groups' actual self-ratings, then the difference in selfdiscrepancy can be attributed to a difference in the ideal self. We conducted a multiple regression analysis with romantic involvement status, sex, and the interaction term as predictor variables and actual self-ratings as the criterion variable. We found no difference in the actual selves of romantically involved (M = 69.98) and romantically uninvolved (M = 67.85) subjects, b = 1.08, p =.156. We therefore tentatively concluded that it was the ideal selves of romantically involved and romantically uninvolved subjects that differed. Compared to romantically uninvolved persons, romantically involved persons appeared to have lower ideal selves. To test our second hypothesis (i.e., reduced self-discrepancy in romantically in -volved subjects is associated with higher psychological well-being), we first tested the link between romantic involvement status, sex, and the interaction term, on the one hand, and the six domains of psycho-logical well-being as well as overall psycho- logical well-being, on the other. If a signifi-

Self-discrepancy 5 cant relation was found between romantic involvement status and psychological wellbeing, then self-discrepancy could be examined as a potential mediator of this relation (Baron & Kenny, 1986). We found that romantic involvement status was significantly related to only the "positive relatedness" domain of psychological well-being, b =- 2.54, p =.005, thus essentially ruling out self-discrepancy as a mediator of the rela - tion between romantic involvement status and psychological well-being. Next, we examined the relation between self-discrepancy, sex, and the interaction term (with romantic involvement status controlled for by being placed in the equation first) and overall psychological well-being, as well as the six domains of psychological well-being. We found that self-discrepancy had a marginally significant relation to overall psychological wellbeing, b =.40, p =.081. Furthermore, selfdiscrepancy was significantly related to three domains of psychological well-being (environmental mastery, b =.14, p =.001, purpose in life, b =.16,p =.001, and selfacceptance, b =.17,p =.003) and marginally significantly related to another (personal growth, b =.07,p =.069). In all cases, the closer subjects felt to their ideal selves, the more likely they were to report higher psychological well-being. Discussion The results were generally consistent with our hypotheses. Romantically involved individuals felt closer to their ideal selves than did romantically uninvolved individuals, with the two groups not differing in ratings of their actual selves. Furthermore, feeling closer to one's ideal self was associated with a higher reported level of psycho-logical well-being in several domains, al-though self-discrepancy did not mediate the relation between romantic involvement and psychological well-being. As mentioned in the introduction, there are at least four possible processes that may lead to romantically involved individuals' experiencing lower self-discrepancy associated with a reduction in ideal self, but no change in actual self. These processes in clude: (a) the projection of the ego-ideal onto the romantic partner (Freud, 1922/1959; Reik, 1944), (b) a change in the focus of romantically involved persons from one of individual achievement and autonomy to one of "relatedness," (c) a be-lief on the part of romantically involved persons that they do not need to attain an ideal self because their actual self is accepted by their partner, and (d) the experience of disillusionment by romantically in-volved persons after getting to know their partners. Clearly, future research is needed to determine which process underlies the lower self-discrepancy reported by romantically involved vis-a-vis romantically uninvolved individuals. Future research can also improve upon the present study in several ways. First, our data are correlational; therefore, directionality cannot be determined and causal inferences cannot be made. For instance, it is possible that people who engage in relationships have a lower ideal self to begin with. The use of longitudinal designs could lead to a better understanding of directionality, and the use of experimental designs, such as manipulating the accessibility in memory of existing romantic relationships (Sedikides & Skowronski,1991) or manipulating relationship closeness with self-discrepancy treated as a dependent variable, would enable causal conclusions to be drawn. Second, lower selfdiscrepancy may be a phenomenon that occurs mainly in the context of young adults' romantic relation-ships. For example, the self-discrepancies of older individuals may be more resistant to change. The study of individuals in other age brackets would help to clarify this issue. Overall, our results provide a preliminary demonstration of one of the many ways in which romantic involvement is likely to be related to the self. The results are provocative in that they open up new and exciting research vistas. It remains for future research to take on the challenge.

References Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. London: Sage. Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love as the expansion of the self': Understanding attraction and satisfaction. New York: Hemisphere. Aron, A., Aron, E. A., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 241-253. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderatormediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Brehm, S. S. (1992). Intimate relationships (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Campbell, W. K., Elliott, A. T., Reeder, G., & Sedikides, C. (1994, May). Attenuation of the self-serving bias in close relationships. Paper presented at the Midwestern Psychological Association convention, Chicago, IL. Freud, S. (1959). Group psychology and the analysis of the ego (Trans. J. Strachey). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1922). Guisinger, S., & Blatt, S. J. (1994). Individuality and relatedness: Evolution of a fundamental dialectic. American Psychologist, 49, 104-111. Hendrick, S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 93-98. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319-340. Higgins, E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people to suffer? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 93-136). New York: Academic Press. Higgins, E. T., Bond, R., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional vulner- ability: How magnitude, type, and accessibility of discrepancy influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 5-15. Higgins, E. T., Vookles, J., & Tykocinski, O. (1992). Self and health: How "patterns" of self-beliefs pre-dict types of emotional and physical problems. So cial Cognition, 10, 125-150. Pelham, B. W., & Swann, W. B. (1989). From self-conceptions to self-worth: On the sources and structures of global self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 672-680. Reik, T. (1944). A psychologist looks at love. New York: Ferrar and Rinehart. Reis, H. T., Wheeler, L., Spiegel, N., Kernis, M. H., Nezlek, J., & Perry, M. (1982). Physical attractiveness in social interaction: II. Why does appearance affect social experience? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 979-996. Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P. (1993). Commitment processes in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 175-204. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069-1081. Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 317-338. Sedikides, C., Oliver, M. B., & Campbell, W. K. (1994). Perceived benefits and costs of romantic relation-ships for women and men: Implications for ex-change theory. Personal Relationships, 1, 5-21. Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1991). The law of cognitive structure activation. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 169-184. Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1994). Motivated selfevaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be sure. Manuscript submitted for publication.