The Impact of Teaching Algebra with a Focus on Procedural Understanding



Similar documents
Performance Assessment Task Which Shape? Grade 3. Common Core State Standards Math - Content Standards

1. I have 4 sides. My opposite sides are equal. I have 4 right angles. Which shape am I?

JERILYNN LEPAK EDUCATION

Teaching and Encouraging Meaningful Reading of Mathematics Texts

Consumer s Guide to Research on STEM Education. March Iris R. Weiss

Introduction. Two vastly different online experiences were presented in an earlier column. An

EDF 6211 Educational Psychology

PROSPECTIVE MIDDLE SCHOOL TEACHERS KNOWLEDGE IN MATHEMATICS AND PEDAGOGY FOR TEACHING - THE CASE OF FRACTION DIVISION

A STATISTICS COURSE FOR ELEMENTARY AND MIDDLE SCHOOL TEACHERS. Gary Kader and Mike Perry Appalachian State University USA

THE EFFECT OF MATHMAGIC ON THE ALGEBRAIC KNOWLEDGE AND SKILLS OF LOW-PERFORMING HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS

Comments About the Programs Reviewed

GRADE 8 MATH: TALK AND TEXT PLANS

Designing Scoring Rubrics for Your Classroom

Masters of Education Degree with a specialization in Elementary Mathematics

How To Be A Mathematically Proficient Person

Executive Summary Principles and Standards for School Mathematics

MED RESEARCH IN MATHEMATICS EDUCATION SPRING, 2006

How Old Are They? This problem gives you the chance to: form expressions form and solve an equation to solve an age problem. Will is w years old.

High School Functions Interpreting Functions Understand the concept of a function and use function notation.

Mathematics Education Master of Education Degree Program PROFESSIONAL PORTFOLIO RUBRIC Candidate: ID#: Date: Evaluator:

Designing a Discussion: Teacher as Designer

Essays on Teaching Excellence. The Challenge of Teaching the Introductory-level Course

Mathematics Content Courses for Elementary Teachers

LU-EdS Handbook. Educational Specialist Degree Handbook

Grade Level Year Total Points Core Points % At Standard %

An Analysis of how Proctoring Exams in Online Mathematics Offerings Affects Student Learning and Course Integrity

Exploring Philosophy During a Time of Reform in Mathematics Education

THE EQUIVALENCE AND ORDERING OF FRACTIONS IN PART- WHOLE AND QUOTIENT SITUATIONS

High School Algebra Reasoning with Equations and Inequalities Solve systems of equations.

COURSE SEQUENCING AND PERFORMANCE: AN INVESTIGATION OF BUSINESS COLLEGE REQUIREMENTS IN RELATION TO AN INFORMATION SYSTEMS CORE COURSE

% ! 3 40% % Percent Cards. This problem gives you the chance to: relate fractions, decimals and percents

Active Learning in Accounting Classes: Three Literacy Strategies

Writing Better Objective Tests Bill Cerbin UW La Crosse, Center for Advancing Teaching & Learning

Mapping the AP / College Calculus Transition IM&E Workshop, March 27 29, 2010

PoW-TER Problem Packet A Phone-y Deal? (Author: Peggy McCloskey)

High School Algebra Reasoning with Equations and Inequalities Solve equations and inequalities in one variable.

FOREIGN LANGUAGE TEACHING AN INTERVIEW WITH NINA SPADA

Principles to Guide the Design and Implementation of Doctoral Programs in Mathematics Education

Background Knowledge

AERA American Educational Research Association 2000, New Orleans: Roundtable

Critical Thinking Paper: Technology in the Classroom. There have been predictions that by the year 2013, a supercomputer that is more

Geometry Solve real life and mathematical problems involving angle measure, area, surface area and volume.

N Q.3 Choose a level of accuracy appropriate to limitations on measurement when reporting quantities.

Calculator Use on Stanford Series Mathematics Tests

Revisioning Graduate Teacher Education in North Carolina Master of Arts in Elementary Education Appalachian State University

Our research-based knowledge about good math. What Does Good Math Instruction Look Like?

Balanced Assessment Test Algebra 2008

Student Preferences for Learning College Algebra in a Web Enhanced Environment

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

Research Basis for Catchup Math

The Preparation of Elementary Teachers at Austin Peay State University. Dorothy Ann Assad. Austin Peay State University

Assessments in Alabama

Articulating Instructional Goals

SCAFFOLDING SPECIAL NEEDS STUDENTS LEARNING OF FRACTION EQUIVALENCE USING VIRTUAL MANIPULATIVES

Probability Using Dice

MATH 0110 Developmental Math Skills Review, 1 Credit, 3 hours lab

Secondary Mathematics Teacher Preparation Programs in the US. Jon R. Star Harvard University

Methodology Research San Ysidro Schools California

Dislodging students misconceptions through the use of worked examples and self-explanation

Demonstrating Understanding Rubrics and Scoring Guides

How Students Interpret Literal Symbols in Algebra: A Conceptual Change Approach

Proposal for New and Revised Graduate Mathematics Education Programs

I n t r o d u c t i o n

Mathematics Curriculum Evaluation Framework

Assessment of Critical Thinking Institutional Learning Outcome (ILO)

ACT National Curriculum Survey Policy Implications on Preparing for Higher Standards. improve yourself

Supporting the General Education Program for Distance and Adult Students

Which Shape? Student Task Core Idea 4 Geometry and Measurement

Competencies There are three units in this course. At the completion of these units, students will be able to:

ANALYZING THE BENEFITS OF USING TABLET PC-BASED FLASH CARDS APPLICATION IN A COLLABORATIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENT A Preliminary Study

Algorithms in Everyday Mathematics

Colorado Learning Assistant Program

Informed Design: A Contemporary Approach to Design Pedagogy Design as The Core Process in Technology Design as an Instructional Strategy

Biography-Driven Culturally Responsive Teaching

Department of Political Science. College of Social Science. Undergraduate Bachelor s Degree in Political Science

Unpacking Division to Build Teachers Mathematical Knowledge

Family Studies and Human Development Student Learning Assessment FSHD 117 Introduction to Lifespan Development

THE STRUCTURE OF ELEMENTARY STUDENTS ABILITY IN GEOMETRIC TRANSFORMATIONS: THE CASE OF TRANSLATION

Instructional Scaffolding to Improve Learning

UNEXPECTED ADVICE FOR BEGINNING GRADUATE STUDENTS IN ASTROPHYSICS

LAKE ELSINORE UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT

Seeing Math Elementary. Final Evaluation Report. September 2005

EXPLORING INFORMAL INFERENCE WITH INTERACTIVE VISUALIZATION SOFTWARE

GOGOT SUHARWOTO AND MAGGIE NIESS Oregon State University Corvallis, OR USA and

Report to the Academic Senate General Education Area B1 - Pilot Assessment Plan

Walden University Catalog

Virginia Tech Department of Accounting and Information Systems Ph.D. Program GENERAL INFORMATION

History of Development of CCSS

GRADE 6 MATH: SHARE MY CANDY

Center for Effective Organizations

ALGEBRA 1 ~ Cell Phone Task Group: Kimberly Allen, Matt Blundin, Nancy Bowen, Anna Green, Lee Hale, Katie Owens

Table 1A-Business Management A.A.S. Student Learning Results

An Analysis of College Mathematics Departments Credit Granting Policies for Students with High School Calculus Experience

Relevant Concreteness and its Effects on Learning and Transfer

Brunswick High School has reinstated a summer math curriculum for students Algebra 1, Geometry, and Algebra 2 for the school year.

Integrating Technology in Teaching and Teacher Education: Implications for Policy and Curriculum Reform Dr. Charalambos Vrasidas Dr. Marina S.

Student Intelligence and Academic Achievement in Albanian Universities. Case of Vlora University

STUDENTS DIFFICULTIES WITH APPLYING A FAMILIAR FORMULA IN AN UNFAMILIAR CONTEXT

Critical Thinking Competency Standards

Transcription:

The Impact of Teaching Algebra with a Focus on Procedural Understanding Andy Belter Faculty Sponsors: Jon Hasenbank and Jennifer Kosiak, Department of Mathematics ABSTRACT Present research indicates that teaching math for understanding may yield positive gains in student learning and retention of knowledge. In the 2007-2008 school year, nine classes used a studentcentered framework for teaching math with an emphasis on understanding. Five comparison classes were selected as a baseline. Students in each of these 14 classes took three algebra tests throughout the year. We developed task-specific rubrics for scoring all three tests, blinded ourselves to the treatment condition by assigning random codes to all tests, and proceeded to score the 830 tests. Statistical analysis revealed that over the course of one-year study period the treatment students made significantly greater gains than comparison students on the skill questions; there were no differences in gains on the understanding questions. Many of the students, regardless of treatment, demonstrated deficiencies in basic algebraic proficiencies, including predicting, interpreting, and evaluating the correctness of their answers. These results warrant further investigation into the effectiveness of teaching for understanding. INTRODUCTION Mathematics is a multidimensional discipline. It is about procedures, concepts, and number facts. It is about predicting, estimating, and verifying. It can be algorithmic, and yet it is also robust and permits flexibility and seeks efficiency. It is grounded in reality and yet it defines a reality of its own. Too often, school mathematics overlooks these intriguing dimensions and focuses on training students to become proficient only at following prescribed recipes in the context in which they were learned. Yet such a limited view of proficiency produces students who have difficulty explaining how they know their work is legitimate, what alternate methods could have been used, what approach would be the most efficient, or how to communicate their answer and verify that it is reasonable. Efforts aimed at teaching for procedural understanding seek to redirect students to delve deeper into the subject matter where they can begin to develop a multidimensional understanding of the mathematical procedures they study. This deeper knowledge of procedures has been characterized over the years as connected knowledge, metacognition, mathematical literacy, and as algebraic habits of mind (Brozo & Simpson, 1999; Burke et al., 2001; Cuoco et al., 1996; Goldenberg, 1996; Hiebert & LeFevre, 1986; Schoenfeld, 1992). We have adopted the term that has become popular in recent literature on mathematics education: procedural understanding (Baroody et al., 2007; Hasenbank, 2006; Star, 2005, 2007). By any name, this deeper procedural understanding is directly contrasted with shallow, disconnected, and simply memorized knowledge. Research from both cognitive psychology and mathematics education suggests that such shallow knowledge is short-lived and does not transfer well to contexts different from the learning environment (Bower, 2000; Greeno, 1978; Hiebert & Carpenter, 1992), while knowledge that is understood lasts longer and can be used flexibly (Star & Siefert, 2002). In 2001, the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) published its Navigating Through Algebra in Grades 9-12. In its pages was a concise framework for what the algebraically literate student would know and be able to do. Dr. Hasenbank and his colleagues modified the NCTM framework to make it more student centered for use in a teaching experiment in college algebra (Hasenbank, 2006); the resulting student-centered framework for teaching mathematics for understanding consisted of the following eight questions: 1a) What is the goal of the procedure? 1b) What sort of answer should I expect? 2a) How do I carry out the procedure? 2b) What other procedures could I use? 3) Why does the procedure work? 1

4) How can I verify my answer? 5) When is this the best procedure to use? 6) What else can I use this procedure to do? This procedural understanding framework helps illustrate what students should understand about algebra procedures by the end of their coursework, and it provides specific questions for use in both instruction and assessment. Teaching for understanding in the mathematics classroom involves building up such understandings of certain mathematical procedures. Examples of procedures include factoring by grouping, graphing in slope intercept form, solving an algebraic equation for a variable, and so on. For instance, if a student was asked to solve 3x+4 = 7, they would be expected to find an answer of x = 1. A series of possible understanding follow-up questions include: Discuss as many ways as possible for checking to see if your solution is correct, (Possible answers: Graph the lines y = 3x+4 and y = 7 and confirm they intersect when x = 1; Evaluate the left hand side of the equation at x = 1 and confirm it is equal to 7). Can you solve for x in a different way? (Possible alternate methods: educated guess and check, graphing, or making a table of values) How could you use estimation to decide whether the answer should be positive or negative? (Possible answer: It should be positive because we need to add to the 4 to get up to 7) How might you convince a skeptic that the steps you used are legitimate? (Possible answers: We solve by applying the additive and multiplicative properties of equality, or we could make and use a physical model to demonstrate the steps). The research so far is fairly limited in measuring the short- and long-term impact of teaching for understanding. One reason for this limitation has been a historical linking of the descriptor understanding with knowledge that is conceptual (but not procedural) in nature (Star, 2005). It is becoming clear that both procedures and concepts can be understood along a continuum ranging from shallow to deep; indeed, Hasenbank s adaptation of the NCTM framework defines a series of questions that can be used to assess the depth of a student s procedural understanding. The recharacterization of procedural knowledge (Hiebert & Handa, 2004; Star, 2005) has begun to influence new curricula and has informed the debate on what effective teaching can and should look like. Yet teaching for understanding can be challenging to accomplish in a classroom for a number of reasons: the time & training needed for developing materials, student resistance to new ways of teaching, and the development of new models for assessing for understanding. But despite the reasons against explicitly teaching for understanding, there is a growing body of evidence that suggests explicitly teaching for understanding has benefits for students. According to Hiebert & Carpenter (1992), if existing knowledge is understood, new knowledge is easier to learn and understand. Students can learn new content more quickly once previous procedures and concepts are understood. In addition to this, knowledge that is understood lasts longer and can be applied in a variety of situations (Carpenter & Lehrer, 1999; Hiebert & Carpenter, 1992; Van Hiele, 1986). With understanding, students are gaining a more diverse education that can serve them in a variety of ways. Furthermore, understanding and skill support one another, and these ties lead to more robust, longer lasting knowledge (Rittle-Johnson, et al., 2001). Lastly, research shows that with understanding procedures are executed intelligently and with fewer errors (Star & Siefert, 2002). The existing literature on teaching for understanding supports it as a worthwhile pursuit in mathematics education. METHOD In the summer of 2007, 13 teachers participated in a summer institute on how to teach for understanding in their classrooms. During this course, the teachers learned the theory behind teaching for understanding, they learned questioning strategies for engaging students in key algebraic habits of mind as defined by Hasenbank s modified NCTM framework, and they learned to assess understanding to reinforce its value and measure its growth. Followup training days during the academic year focused on the classroom implementation of the strategies learned during the summer institute. After the summer institute, participating teachers each selected one or two focus classes they would teach using the framework for understanding. Five comparison teachers were also recruited to act as a control group. The students in each focus class took three algebra tests throughout their academic year to assess their mathematical skill and understanding. The tests each consisted of approximately 17 questions and a mix of both skill questions and understanding questions. The treatment student sample consisted of five algebra classes, two geometry classes, one 2

physical science class, and one AP calculus class. The comparison sample consisted of four algebra classes and one geometry class. Data analysis began with creating task-specific rubrics for each question on the three tests. The rubrics were all based on the following 0 to 3 scale: 0: No degree of understanding 1: Low degree of understanding 2: Moderate degree of understanding 3: High degree of understanding We then reviewed a subset of the 830 tests and began to create the task-specific rubrics. In order to develop a reliable rubric for scoring the diverse set of responses, we generated a series of preliminary rubrics and revised them based on a pilot scoring of student responses. The final rounds of pilot grading served as a reliability check between graders (discussed in further detail below). Eventually we agreed upon a set of task-specific rubrics that we felt would allow us to consistently code the wide variety of answers in a way that would accurately assess students understanding. To illustrate the assessment format and the typical rubric design, we have included in Figure 1 a sample skill question and subsequent understanding question, together with their associated rubrics: Skill Question: Evaluate (2/3)x when x = (9/4). Simplify your answer if possible. Correct Solution is 3/2, or 1½, or 1.5. (Note: 18/12 is incorrect because it has not been simplified.) Rubric: Possible errors: Finding a common denominator Computational error Simplifying incorrectly 0: Blank or no honest effort (e.g. "I don't know (IDK.)") 1: Two or more errors, or used cross multiplication to solve, or an incomplete attempt, or a solution that is way off track. 2: Only one error is present as listed above. 3: Correct answer is given in lowest terms. Understanding Question: Explain how you could use estimation to see if your previous answer was wrong. Rubric: Sample responses: I can use decimals to estimate the problem as.7 x 2 = 1.4. I can estimate the problem as 2/3 of 2¼, so it should be between 1 and 2. The problem is a fraction (less than one) times a number a little greater than 2, so it should be less than 2. I can estimate the problem as a little less than 1 times a little more than 2, so the answer should be close to 1 x 2 = 2. 0: Blank or no honest effort (e.g. "I don't know (IDK.)") 1: Response is vague, confusing, contains little insight (e.g. Use my calculator ), OR uses estimation after performing the multiplication (e.g. I got 1 and 6/12, and 6 is about half of 12, so 1½ is right). 2: Response uses the relative magnitudes of the fractions (e.g., use decimal approximations, or rounds 9/4 to 2) but omits details or contains minor mistakes or oversights that detract from the overall argument. 3: Response is similar to one of the sample responses, or uses other techniques that produce a reasonable estimate. Figure 1. Sample Rubrics 3

Before we scored the tests, we blinded ourselves to the treatment condition to ensure we were not unintentionally entering bias into our coding. We stripped identifying information from the tests and assigned a random code to each student. Because we needed to divide up the workload between three graders, we also needed to make sure we were scoring items consistently. To test this we conducted a pilot grading of all tests from students whose four-digit random code began with 0 (that is, approximately 1/10 of the tests). We found Cohen's kappa for inter-rater agreement on the three tests to be 0.867, 0.843, 0.881, respectively. A kappa statistic above 0.8 is considered Almost Perfect agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977). Since this was satisfactory, we continued to score the remaining tests. To better illustrate the grading process, we have included two student responses to the sample questions we included in Figure 1. These are presented in Figure 2 and 3, below. The assigned score for each response is also shown. Skill Question: Evaluate (2/3)x when x = (9/4). Simplify your answer if possible. Correct Solution is 3/2, or 1½, or 1.5. (Note: 18/12 is incorrect because it has not been simplified.) Figure 2. Sample Skill Question with Student Responses Understanding Question: Explain how you could use estimation to see if your previous answer was wrong. Figure 3. Sample Understanding Question with Student Responses RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Our analysis consisted of comparing gain scores from test 1 to test 3 for both the skill and understanding subscores. The data presented here represent only the algebra classes to permit comparison between treatment and control groups. The skill questions in algebra illustrate the attributes that are often associated with the subject, and these are certainly an important component of algebraic fluency. A repeated measures ANOVA indicated significant 4

differences between groups. Follow-up t-tests revealed a significantly greater gain in skill from test one to test three (p =.003) in favor of the treatment group. The treatment group started with an average skill score below the control group but finished with a higher average skill score (see Figure 4). Figure 4. Skill and Understanding Subscore Results We next looked at the understanding results for the students in algebra. The t-test of the gain score in understanding from test 1 to test 3 revealed no significant difference in gain scores between groups (p =.413). This result shows that the treatment group did not make improvements in their understanding scores over the comparison group. Figure 5 displays this result above. It is worth noting that, as the figures suggest, the overall understanding scores were statistically well below the overall skill scores (p <.001), indicating that regardless of treatment condition the students scored significantly lower on our understanding questions than they did on our skill questions. Apparently additional work is needed to develop methods that raise students ability to answer deep questions about the procedures they are learning. The fact that we observed greater gains in skill for the treatment group but no differences on the understanding questions led us to wonder what the relationship between skill and understanding questions was for our students, particularly since many of the understanding questions produced very poor results. To explore this discrepancy between skill and understanding, we produced crosstabs of skill scores vs. understanding scores for our students, regardless of treatment condition. The skill and understanding questions included in the crosstab are shown for reference in Figure 5 (below); these results were taken from the year-end test. Skill question: If 3x + 4y = 14, and y = -(5/2)x, what is the value of x? Answer: x = -2 Understanding question: What does the answer to the previous problem tell you about the graphs of the two equations? Answer: The graphs of the two lines will intersect at the point (-2,5). Figure 5. Skill and Understanding Questions used on Crosstabs 5

Table 1 (below) shows a crosstab of skill score vs. understanding score for all students (including algebra, geometry, physical science, and AP calculus) who took the three tests. Table 1. Understanding vs. Skill Crosstab Results Understanding 0 1 2 3 Total 0 40 (15.6%) 5 (1.9%) 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%) 45 (17.5%) 1 35 (13.6%) 73 (28.4%) 11 (4.3%) 1 (0.4%) 120 (46.7%) Skill 2 5 (1.9%) 36 (14.0%) 3 (1.2%) 0 (0.0%) 44 (17.1%) 3 8 (3.1%) 32 (12.5%) 5 (1.9%) 3 (1.2%) 48 (18.7%) Total 88 (34.2%) 146 (56.8%) 19 (7.4%) 4 (1.6%) 257 (100%) The highlighted cells in the crosstab reveal that, even at the end of year, most students demonstrated a low level of understanding on this algebra task. The crosstab shows that only 4 students could correctly answer the understanding question, while 48 students could correctly answer the related skill question. Other crosstab results revealed similar results. We interpret this as evidence that many students had a very limited understanding of the processes of algebra, especially in their ability to predict, estimate, and make sense of their answers. Moreover, students had difficulty connecting the problems they solve to the underlying meaning or alternate representations. Such limited understanding is characteristic of the fragile learning we noted in our introduction, and it suggests a greater emphasis is needed on procedural understanding in the K-12 curriculum. In addition to this, the total understanding average score from all algebra students on test three was only 1.44. This means that after a full year of algebra, most students were at either a low or moderate degree of understanding. Several understanding questions in test 3 produced average scores around 0.7, indicating a very low degree of understanding (it also indicates that many students left these questions blank). Our research shows that certain algebra procedures are simply not being understood, which further supports the need for more effective student-centered implementations of the principles of teaching for understanding. REFERENCES Baroody, A. J., Feil, Y., & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualization of rocedural and conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 115 131. Bower, G. H. (2000). A brief history of memory research. In E. Tulving & F. Craik (Eds.), Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 3 32). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Brozo, W. G., & Simpson, M. L. (1999). Readers, teachers, learners: Expanding literacy across the content areas (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc. Burke, M., Erickson, D., Lott, J. W., & Obert, M. (2001). Navigating through algebra in grades 9-12. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Carpenter, T. P., & Lehrer, R. (1999). Teaching and learning mathematics with understanding. In E. Fennema & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Mathematics classrooms that promote understanding (pp. 19 32). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cuoco, A., Goldenberg, E. P., & Mark, J. (1996). Habits of mind: An organizing principle for mathematics curricula. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 15, 375 402. Goldenberg, E. P. (1996). "habits of mind" as an organizer for the curriculum. Boston University Journal of Education, 178(1), 13 34. Greeno, J. G. (1978). Understanding and procedural knowledge in mathematics instruction. Educational Psychologist, 12(3), 262 283. Hasenbank, J. F. (2006). The effects of a framework for procedural understanding on college algebra students procedural skill and understanding. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Montana State University, Bozeman. Hiebert, J., & Carpenter, T. P. (1992). Learning and teaching with understanding. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 65 97). New York: Macmillan. Hiebert, J., & Handa, Y. (2004). A modest proposal for reconceptualizing the activity of learning mathematical procedures, American Educational Research Association. San Diego. 6

Hiebert, J., & LeFevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 1 27). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159 174. Rittle Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 346 362. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334 370): Macmillan. Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 36(5), 404 411. Star, J. R. (2007). Research commentary: A rejoinder foregrounding procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 132 135. Star, J. R., & Siefert, C. (2002). Re conceptualizing procedural knowledge: Flexibility and innovation in equation solving, Annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association. New Orleans, Louisiana. Van Hiele, P. M. (1986). Structure and insight: A theory of mathematics education. London: Academic Press, Inc. 7