Aggressive Behavior in Adopted Dogs (Canis Familiaris) that Passed a Temperament Test

Similar documents
DOG BITE INVESTIGATE WORKSHEET CITY OF WAYCROSS ANIMAL CONTROL

Humane Society International. Creating Animal Shelter Guidelines: Selecting Animals for Euthanasia

The Fiscal Impact of Breed Discriminatory Legislation in the United States. Prepared for Best Friends Animal Society

A note on the influence of visual conspecific contact on the behaviour of sheltered dogs

Kennel Syndrome Kennel Syndrome

Options for dog population management: Where do you start?

Animal Ordinances for the City of Columbus, Georgia

Distance Education Program

Contract for Pet Care

THAT S NOT MY DOG An overview of dog liability in British Columbia

Dog Bites and Dangerous Dogs Managing Dog Bites while Protecting People and Dogs

Chapter No. 765] PUBLIC ACTS, CHAPTER NO. 765 HOUSE BILL NO By Representatives West, Marrero. Substituted for: Senate Bill No.

DECISION ON APPEAL OF VICIOUS DOG DETERMINATION. This is an appeal from a determination by the City of Rutland Board of Alderman

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

Laws and Regulations Relating to RABIES. Excerpts from the. California Health and Safety Code. and the. California Code of Regulations

For the Provision of animal collection and impound services.

Using C.L.A.S.S. to Encourage Training and Increase Adoptions in Your Local Shelter, Rescue Group, or Municipal Animal Control

IACUC POLICIES, PROCEDURES, and GUIDELINES ADOPTION OF ANIMALS DESIGNATED FOR RESEARCH

+ Big Problem 1/6/14. + Action Needed. Saving Lives Nationwide. In 2002, 33,847 animals entered area shelters in Duval County and 23,104 died

FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT: HYPOTHESIZING PREDICTORS AND PURPOSES OF PROBLEM BEHAVIOR TO IMPROVE BEHAVIOR-CHANGE PLANS

Crate Training Your Dog

Solving the Problem of Dog Bites

HOMEWARD BOUND ANIMAL PLACEMENT POLICY

DogNostics Career College Certificate & Diploma Program Tool-Kits -

Idaho Law For Dogs - A Section of Design

Neutering family planning for felines

CITY OF TOPEKA City Attorney s Office

Healthy puppies come from breeders who:

ACMCAS410A Conduct companion animal training classes

Frequently Asked Questions: Live baiting in greyhound racing

Non-Accidental Injury in Dogs and Cats in Colorado Final Report to the Animal Assistance Foundation

Using Proxy Measures of the Survey Variables in Post-Survey Adjustments in a Transportation Survey

Dog Behavior Problems Aggression Diagnosis and Overview

Many dogs love to play and to be petted. But not all dogs are friendly or want to play all the time.

Planning for Your Pets Care When You Can No Longer Care for Them. Major and Planned Gifts Fact Sheets

Chapter 6 Proposed Ordinance Changes

Relationship. Initial:

Financial Assistance for Veterinary Bills

2012 Thundershirt Cat Anxiety Survey The Cat Anxiety Problem: Size, Scope, and Solutions

Behavior Analysis and Strategy Application after Brain Injury: Addressing the long-term behavioral outcomes of brain injury

INDICATORS OF STRESS AND STRESS ASSESSMENT IN DOGS

MANAGEMENT AND REHABILITATION OF INTER-DOG AGGRESSION IN ANIMAL SHELTERS. Jane S. Orihel. B.A. (Psychology), The University of Winnipeg, 2002

CRISTA L. COPPOLA Ph.D. Certified Applied Animal Behaviorist

The Insider's Guide To The West Highland Terrier - The Dog Barking Helper HOW TO MANAGE DOGGY PROBLEMS. Dog Barking Help

(b) Safeguard and protect property of Taylor County citizens,

Ramona Humane Society Animal Transfer Program

POSITION GUIDE. ( Atlanta, GA MISSION

NE Ohio Greyhound Rescue, Inc. Adoption Application PLEASE ANSWER EVERY QUESTION THOROUGHLY

Published on: 07/04/2015 Page 1 of 5

Understanding Dogs Temperament in Dogs Its Role in Decision Making

Zero Euthanasia - Optimising The Foster Care Program Presentation Paper 2011

Volunteer Department 169 West Grand Avenue Chicago, IL ext.

DOG ADOPTION CONTRACT/AGREEMENT

Customer Information Sheet

Managing dog bite incidences through public education

Emergency Contact Information

PUBLIC SAFETY COMMITTEE ORDINANCE NO.

THE CORPORATION OF THE CITY OF SAULT STE. MARIE BY-LAW

PAWS Humane. Puppy Foster Guide. Everything you need to know about fostering puppies!

UCC2: Course Change Transmittal Form

Upgrade Request for Provisional Wildlife Rehabilitator

Breed differences in canine aggression

St. Catherine University Support Animal Policy

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

Additional sources Compilation of sources:

DOG SURRENDER PROFILE COCHECO VALLEY HUMANE SOCIETY

Train Walk Poop Client Contract Training and Daycare Program

Association Between Variables

Time Management Does Not Matter For Academic Achievement Unless You Can Cope

Violence in the Workplace Procedures Manual 417-A

Reprinted From the November 2001 Issue of New Jersey Municipalities Magazine

Application Checklist for Dog Therapy Team

UC HASTINGS POLICY # : Dogs on Campus

So you are thinking of working with dogs?

Application Process for Approval of Animal Control Officer Training Courses Texas Department of State Health Services, Zoonosis Control Branch

Job Announcement Registered Veterinary Technician- Shelter Medicine

Effects of Dog Breed Labeling on Potential Adopter Perceptions & Shelter Length of Stay

Dealing with the aftermath, & a plan for the future

Alzheimer s and other related diseases: coping with behavioural disorders in the patient s home

Ark-Valley Humane Society Volunteer Handbook

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS

You and Your New Dog: Navigating The First Weeks After Adopting a Dog

Incidence of Dog Bite-Associated Emergency Department Visits in Maryland,

Feline Coronavirus (FCoV) and Feline Infectious Peritonitis (FIP)

Hobart and William Smith Colleges

Structured Interviewing:

Research into Issues Surrounding Human Bones in Museums Prepared for

Updated on-line 7/26/07

Between Orest Strynatka, claimant, and Alison Forbes, defendant. [2002] B.C.J. No BCPC 481. North Vancouver Registry No.

Gold Coast City Council

A Guide To Create A Successful Adoption Experience

Dog Law Northern Ireland

Applicant: I am interested in the following animal (s): 1: Dog

EXCLUDED HEREDITARY CONDITIONS BY BREED *

Temperament and Character Inventory R (TCI R) and Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ): convergence and divergence 1

WOOD COUNTY HEALTH DEPARTMENT

Guidelines for caring for injured turtles

BT-3. Bark Control Collar. Please Read All Instructions Carefully.

Pet Insurance Fact Sheet

ASSISTANCE & SERVICE ANIMALS FOR TENANTS WHO ARE PERSONS WITH DISABILITIES

Transcription:

Aggressive Behavior in Adopted Dogs (Canis Familiaris) that Passed a Temperament Test E Christensen *, J Scarlett 1, M Campagna 2, K Houpt 3 *,3 Animal Behavior Clinic, Department of Clinical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853 USA 1 Department of Population Medicine and Diagnostic Science, College of Veterinary Medicine, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 14853 USA 2 1150 N. Triphammer Rd #T8C, Ithaca, New York, 14850 USA * Corresponding author: emcdvm@ yahoo.com Introduction In an effort to find an objective way to measure and predict an animal s behavioral adoptability and meet the demands of stakeholders, many shelters have adopted temperament testing. This is especially important because many dogs are surrendered to shelters because of aggressive behavior (Salman et al 2000; Wells & Hepper 2000). There are several tests to choose from, some published and some disseminated via conferences and personal communications. They vary in length and complexity and may be administered by highly trained personnel or people newly entering the shelter field (van der Borg et al 1991; Netto & Planta 1997; Sternberg 2003). Thousands of dog bites occur in the United States each year and in addition to the injury to people, the rising frequency of litigation makes protecting the public from aggressive animals increasingly important (Weiss et al 1998). Although temperament testing is a controversial method of assessing the behavioral traits of dogs, it is one tool that can potentially be helpful in screening for aggression, especially in conjunction with relinquishment interviews and shelter staff observations (van der Borg et al 1991; Segurson et al 2005). However, when used to screen for aggression, temperament testing is not perfect. Animals in a shelter environment, despite shelters best efforts to maintain good animal welfare, may be suffering from occult disease, sleep deprivation, noise pollution, social stress (either from isolation or competition), and unknown emotional distress. These additional stressors inherent in shelter life may inhibit some dogs with aggressive tendencies from exhibiting them while in the shelter and even for a variable period after adoption. Furthermore, the situations simulated within any given temperament test may not stimulate a dog s triggers for aggressive behavior. 25

The goal of this project was to evaluate the proportion of dogs passing a temperament test that exhibited behaviors associated with aggression in their adoptive home. Our hypotheses were that some dogs with aggressive tendencies would pass the temperament test and that aggressive behavior requiring a social bond (owner-directed aggression) or a defendable space (territorial or stranger-directed aggression), or stimulation of a chase sequence (predatory aggression) would not be reliably found on this temperament test. Materials and Methods In order to test these hypotheses, all dogs (279) were identified that were adopted between 3/5/04 and 1/6/05 from a local animal shelter. They had been tested with a modified version of Sternberg s (2003) standardized temperament testing protocol. These temperament tests were performed by one of two people - the first author or one well-trained shelter staff member with whom the senior author worked frequently. This staff member had tested numerous dogs before the study in conjunction with the Animal Behavior Clinic. The test was always administered in the same room in an area of the shelter where none of the dogs had been. It was performed according to a list including each dog s response to various physical manipulations (opening the mouth, stroking, and hugging), environmental manipulations (loud noises, presence of cats in cages), social manipulations (removal of food/toys/rawhide, approached by friendly dog), and novel stimulus presentation (toddler-sized doll). In order to be available for adoption each segment of the test had to be passed without snarling, growling, lunging, snapping, or biting. Any dog that snarled, growled, lunged, snapped, or bit in any situation (other than resource guarding) during the temperament test was not considered adoptable and was euthanized according to shelter policy. A telephone interview was conducted within 13 months of adoption by a person who had not interacted with the dogs or the adopters prior to the telephone call. The interviewer called the owners of all dogs adopted during the testing period who had telephone access and had legible contact information. Approximately 60 per cent of the adoption sheets had this information. If the adopters were unavailable during the first telephone call, two more attempts were made to contact them during the study period. Using this method, approximately 24 per cent of the adopters were reached. During this interview, the adopters answered questions about house-soiling, jumping up, and separation-related behavior. They were also asked a list of questions from a standardized aggression screen used routinely for behavioral consultations 26

at the Animal Behavior Clinic at Cornell University. This questionnaire addresses behaviors during various physical manipulations (push on dog, physical punishment, attach leash, etc.), social situations (approached when eating or chewing on toys/food/bones, push/pull off furniture, approached by other dogs on/off leash, around children/toddlers, etc.), and situations involving territorial behavior (strangers/familiar people in the yard, behavior on walks/at gas stations/toll booths, etc.). For those dogs displaying aggressive or potentially aggression-motivated behaviors, the aggression type (territorial, resource guarding (to humans or animals), intra-specific, predatory, and owner-directed) was classified based on the situations that stimulated the behavior (functional definitions) or the target(s) of the behavior (context-specific definitions). Territorial, predatory, and intra-specific aggression were classified according to Landsberg et al (2003 Ch 19). Resource guarding was used as a category in order to combine food-related aggression and possessive aggression as defined by Overall (1997 p 513-516). Owner-directed aggression was used as a category to include aggression to the owner possibly due to a variety of motivations (including social status/dominance, conflict, fear, etc.) as described by Reisner (2002). Resource guarding was assessed separately from other situations stimulating owner-directed aggression. Overall aggression level was evaluated by the owners responses to the questionnaire for each dog and was classified into one of four groups: no aggression, low (barking only), moderate (growling or lunging, +/- barking), and high (biting/snapping, +/- [growling, lunging, or barking]). Data Analysis The percentages of dogs displaying various types of behaviors and levels of aggression were calculated. Comparisons of the frequency of these aggressive behaviors were performed among age groups and among sexes of dogs and owners using the chi square test of independence. In order to evaluate whether the proportion of dogs observed with various types of aggression (with the exception of resource guarding) differed significantly from zero (on the assumption that they had been screened out) a one sample proportion test was used comparing the observed percentages to 1% (since the statistical program would not accept zero). For resource guarding, we compared the observed frequency after adoption to that observed among dogs on the temperament test. P values of 0.01 were considered significant after adjusting the alpha error rate for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction. 27

Results Of the tested dogs, 67 dogs were eligible and their owners responded to a standardized telephone interview completed within 13 months of adoption. At the time of the telephone interview, 89.2 per cent of the dogs were still in their homes. One owner did not complete the entire questionnaire. This left 66 owner/dog pairs in the final analysis for the aggression screening questions. Based on these interviews, types of aggressive behavior were estimated in the population. Situations that could stimulate territorial behavior elicited aggression (barking, growling, lunging, and biting) in over half of the dogs. Of dogs with territorial behaviors, the majority were in the low aggression group. In situations related to predatory behavior, approximately 14 per cent of the dogs exhibited behaviors associated with aggression (barking, growling, lunging, and biting). Most of the dogs with predatory behavior were in the moderate aggression group. Aggression to the owner was rarely reported. Among dogs with owner-directed aggression, all had moderate aggression. Resource guarding to people also occurred rarely, as did aggression over food/rawhides/toys to other dogs or cats. Only one dog was reported to be aggressive to the owner in social situations and over resources. Intra-specific aggression was reported in less than 17 per cent of the dogs. The overall aggression level was assessed for each dog as described previously. In this assessment, the majority of the dogs were reported to exhibit behaviors that could be consistent with aggression; however, most of these dogs were in the low or moderate category. Dogs were reported only rarely to bite or snap on one or more of the aggression screen questions. The proportions of dogs displaying territorial, intra-specific, and/or predatory aggression were all significantly higher than the expectation of no aggression (p < 0.01). Ideally, all aggression (except resource guarding to people) would be screened and eliminated from the adopted population by the temperament test. Discussion: Our results strongly suggest that there are certain types of aggressive tendencies that are not exhibited reliably during temperament testing using the techniques described above. A significant number of dogs passing this temperament test exhibited aggressive tendencies in situations suggesting motivations such as territorial, predatory, and intra-specific aggression. 28

Assigning dogs exhibiting barking behavior without concurrent lunging, growling, snapping, or biting to the low aggression group could be controversial since one cannot tell reliably from owner descriptions whether the dog is being aggressive as opposed to attention-seeking, etc. However, since some of the barking dogs could be barking as part of an aggressive behavioral pattern they were included in an attempt to account for as many potentially aggressive dogs as possible. Although it was assessed separately from growling, baring teeth, snapping, biting, and piloerection, including barking as a possibly aggressive behavior was also done in the van der Borg et al (1991) study following temperament tested dogs after adoption. Spain et al (2004) also included barking in their assessment of aggression in dogs when evaluating the behavioral and medical effects of early-age gonadectomy. Dogs in the low aggression category must have been barking in response to an item on the aggression screen (i.e. strangers in the yard, other dogs, at toll booths, etc.). Dogs barking when left alone or in situations not addressed on the aggression screen were not included in the overall aggression levels. When compared to other studies investigating temperament, the adopted dogs in our study were more likely to have behaviors consistent with aggression to strangers, but less likely to have intra-specific aggression than the dogs in a study by Hsu and Serpell (2003). Compared to Wells and Hepper s (2000) study which was composed of a larger population of mostly intact animals, it is interesting that the percentage of dogs in our study exhibiting potentially aggressive behaviors (barking, snarling, growling, snapping, lunging, or biting) is much higher than the combined percentages of aggression to dogs and humans in their study. The high percentage of dogs exhibiting aggressive behaviors of any level in our study population is similar to the findings of Spain et al (2004). It was hypothesized that post-adoption, owner-directed aggression would be relatively common in the population of dogs passing the temperament test. Many situations that could stimulate owner-directed aggression are evaluated in this temperament test, but the dogs presumably do not have a relationship with the temperament tester prior to testing and the evaluation quickly advances to physical manipulation of the dog. It is of interest that few dogs were reported to have owner-directed aggression during the post-adoption interview, even with these inherent limitations in the temperament test. This may be because the post-adoption questions that related to owner-directed aggression (such as petting the dog, staring at the dog, hugging the dog, etc.) were sufficiently similar to the elements of this temperament test (stranger petting the 29

dog, staring, veterinary technician hug, etc.). Dogs that reacted aggressively on the test would have failed and, therefore, not been available for adoption. Alternatively, the adopters that were contacted may not have reported accurately due to recall bias, fear of repercussion for the dog, and/or concern about whether they would be blamed for the behavior. Alternatively, the adopters that were contacted may not have reported accurately due to recall bias, fear of repercussion for the dog, and/or concern about whether they would be blamed for the behavior. These psychological influences on the reporting adopter could lead to data bias in this study. For the reasons listed above, it is likely that owners did not report some aggressive behaviors. In addition, owners of some dogs that were temperament tested during this time could not be contacted for the telephone interview. There may be a variety of logistical and emotional reasons that adopters could not be contacted. One of these is that the owners did not want to discuss the dog s behavior or disposition. Conclusion Despite the limitations, this study strongly suggests that significant numbers of dogs with certain types of aggression have the potential for escaping the notice of shelter workers even when employing a standardized temperament test and combining its results with shelter observation, histories, and strict euthanasia policies. The majority of dogs with aggression-associated behaviors were in the low aggression category (barking only); however, many of the dogs were in the moderate category (growling and/or lunging) and some did exhibit high levels of aggression (biting/snapping). These findings are important as we continue to refine evaluation of adoption suitability in order to protect public health and the public image of shelters and shelter dog adoptions, to improve adoption stability and retention rates, and to decrease the number of adoptable dogs euthanized each year. Key words Aggression, bark, dog, shelters, temperament testing Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the Tompkins County Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, the adopters that participated in this study, and Donna Davies for their assistance with this research. 30

References Salman MD, Hutchison J, Ruch-Gallie R, Kogan L, New JC, Kass PH and Scarlett JM 2000 Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12 shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1: 207-226 Wells D and Hepper PG 2000. Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69: 55-65 van der Borg JAM, Netto WJ and Planta DJU 1991 Behavioural testing of dogs in an animal shelter to predict problem behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32: 237-251 Netto WJ and Planta DJU 1997 Behavioural testing for aggression in the domestic dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 52: 243-263 Sternberg S 2003 Successful Dog Adoption. Wiley Publishing Inc: Indianapolis, USA Weiss HB, Friedman DI and Coben, JH 1998 Incidence of dog bite injuries treated in emergency departments. The Journal of the American Medical Association 279: 51-53 Segurson S, Serpell JA and Hart BL 2005 Evaluation of a behavioral assessment questionnaire for use in the characterization of behavioral problems of dogs relinquished to animal shelters. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 227: 1755-1761 Landsberg G, Hunthausen W and Ackerman L 2003 Handbook of Behavior Problems of the Dog and Cat. Saunders: Edinburgh, UK Overall KL 1997 Clinical Behavioral Medicine for Small Animals. Mosby: St. Louis, USA Reisner I 2002 An overview of aggression. In: Horwitz DF, Mills DS and Heath S (Eds), BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine pp 181-194. British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Waterwells, UK Spain CV, Scarlett JM and Houpt KA 2004 Long term risks and benefits of early-age gonadectomy in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 224: 380-387 Hsu Y and Serpell JA 2003 Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 223: 1293-1300 31