The impact of television viewing on language development from birth to 30 months: A Critical Review of the Literature

Similar documents
Critical Review: What are the effects of adding music to the treatment of speech and language disorders in pre-school and school aged children?

Do Babies Learn From Baby Media?

Critical Review: Does music therapy have a positive impact on language functioning in adults with dementia?

November 2008 / Common Sense Media. Media + Child and Adolescent Health: A Systematic Review

What the Research Tells Us About The Impact of TV/Video Viewing on Children Under Three

Evaluating the Benefits of Early Intervention via Internet-based Two-way Videoconferencing A Multi-site Study funded by the Oberkotter Foundation

Guided Reading 9 th Edition. informed consent, protection from harm, deception, confidentiality, and anonymity.

University Child Care Centre EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

National Academy of Sciences Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism

Reading to Young Children: A Head-Start in Life

Reading Assessment BTSD. Topic: Reading Assessment Teaching Skill: Understanding formal and informal assessment

Chapter Eight: Quantitative Methods

How Does Memory Change With Age? Class Objectives. Think about the importance of your memory 3/22/2011. The retention of information over time

III. FREE APPROPRIATE PUBLIC EDUCATION (FAPE)

Critical Review: Is the integration of mobile device apps into speech and language therapy effective clinical practice?

The importance of using marketing information systems in five stars hotels working in Jordan: An empirical study

Evaluation and Assessment and Eligibility Regulations 2011

Marcia E. Humpal, M.Ed., MT-BC Ronna S. Kaplan, M.A. MT-BC. Journal of Music Therapy

WHAT IS A JOURNAL CLUB?

National Early Literacy Panel: Questions and Answers

DR. PAT MOSSMAN Tutoring

Early Childhood Measurement and Evaluation Tool Review

A Guide to Understanding and Using Data for Effective Advocacy

Mode and Patient-mix Adjustment of the CAHPS Hospital Survey (HCAHPS)

Zero to Eight. Children s Media Use in America

Childhood Vaccinations and Health Risks. Sarah Mullins. Research in Allied Health

The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment. Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households

INFANT TODDLER ASSESSMENT

Adult Child Ratio in Child Care

How do families find and use child care? What are child care challenges and needs?

classroom Tool Part 3 of a 5 Part Series: How to Select The Right

TEXAS RISING STAR WEBINAR SERIES: CURRICULUM AND EARLY LEARNING GUIDELINES RECORDED OCTOBER 29, 2015 NOTES

8 th European Conference on Psychological Assessment

Descriptive Methods Ch. 6 and 7

INCREASE YOUR PRODUCTIVITY WITH CELF 4 SOFTWARE! SAMPLE REPORTS. To order, call , or visit our Web site at

A PUBLICATION OF THE NATIONAL COUNCIL FOR ADOPTION ADOPTION USA: SUMMARY AND HIGHLIGHTS OF A CHARTBOOK ON THE NATIONAL SURVEY OF ADOPTIVE PARENTS

How do we know what we know?

Course Syllabus College of Education Winona State University. Department: Special Education Date: August 2011

Discipline and Intergenerational Transmission

GEORGIA DIVISION OF FAMILY AND CHILDREN SERVICES CHILD WELFARE POLICY MANUAL

Stories To Inspire Very Young Engineers: Can Robots Burp? Allison Lague; Stephanie McCartan; AnnMarie Thomas, University of St.

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

References to Play in NAEYC Position Statements

Adventure Games as Deaf Education Tools: Action Research Results. Scott Whitney Stephen F. Austin State University. Gabriel A. Martin Lamar University

Evaluation of four autism early childhood intervention programs Final Evaluation Report Executive Summary

DETECTION AND NONOPERATIVE MANAGEMENT OF PEDIATRIC DEVELOPMENTAL DYSPLASIA OF THE HIP IN INFANTS UP TO SIX MONTHS OF AGE SUMMARY

In-service training available through Netflix

Developmental Assessment. School of Child & Youth Care University of Victoria

Sherry Peter M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Technical Report. Overview. Revisions in this Edition. Four-Level Assessment Process

Social Media and Digital Marketing Analytics ( INFO-UB ) Professor Anindya Ghose Monday Friday 6-9:10 pm from 7/15/13 to 7/30/13

Correlational Research. Correlational Research. Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP EDS 250. Descriptive Research 1. Correlational Research: Scatter Plots

Child Care Center and Preschool Quality Framework. November 2013

Comparison of Research Designs Template

Marie Evans Schmidt. Ph.D., Psychology, 2003, Area of Specialization: Developmental Psychology M.S., Psychology, 2001

Critical Review: Does print referencing during shared storybook reading improve pre-literacy skills in preschoolers?

MICHIGAN TEST FOR TEACHER CERTIFICATION (MTTC) TEST OBJECTIVES FIELD 062: HEARING IMPAIRED

Placement Stability and Number of Children in a Foster Home. Mark F. Testa. Martin Nieto. Tamara L. Fuller

Integration of Children with Developmental Disabilities in Social Activities. Abstract

Written Example for Research Question: How is caffeine consumption associated with memory?

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

MAY Legal Risks of Applicant Selection and Assessment

Childcare and early years survey of parents 2014 to 2015

Each reform was almost the exact opposite of the New Labour education reforms that were taking place at the same time in the UK.

Online Consumer Herding Behaviors in the Hotel Industry

Wake County SmartStart ACTIVITY DESCRIPTION DEFINITIONS

DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS

Abstract Title: Identifying and measuring factors related to student learning: the promise and pitfalls of teacher instructional logs

Pilot Testing and Sampling. An important component in the data collection process is that of the pilot study, which

Lecture 13/Chapter 10 Relationships between Measurement (Quantitative) Variables

Executive Summary. 1. What is the temporal relationship between problem gambling and other co-occurring disorders?

2015 Christmas Post-Campaign Tracking Research

Nursing Journal Toolkit: Critiquing a Quantitative Research Article

Overview of Intervention Points, Barriers and Gaps. North Carolina Institute of Medicine February 17, Pat Hansen Shape NC Project Manager

Digging Deeper into Safety and Injury Prevention Data

MISSION STATEMENT PHILOSOPHY

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

Research Methods. Types of Research. Descriptive Research. Example: Types of research. Developmental research designs. Ethics

Early Bird Catches the Worm: The Causal Impact of Pre-school Participation and Teacher Qualifications on Year 3 NAPLAN Outcomes

Harrison, P.L., & Oakland, T. (2003), Adaptive Behavior Assessment System Second Edition, San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

1.0 Abstract. Title: Real Life Evaluation of Rheumatoid Arthritis in Canadians taking HUMIRA. Keywords. Rationale and Background:

Del Mar College Child Development / Early Childhood. Course Syllabus TECA 1354 (online)

Evaluation: Designs and Approaches

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness

The Special Education Referral & Identification Process. Menlo Park City School District Source: US Department of Education and

Training Agency/Online Company Approval Packet

Evidence translation for effective early childhood intervention

The Early Catastrophe: The 30 Million Word Gap by Age 3 Betty Hart and Todd R. Risley

SPEECH AND LANGUAGE EVALUATION CLIENT : RESP. PARTY : ADDRESS : INFORMANT : REFERRAL SOURCE : BIRTH DATE : EVALUATION DATE : PHONE : REPORT DATE :

Overview. Benefits and Features

Critical Review: Sarah Rentz M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Ten Strategies to Encourage Academic Integrity in Large Lecture Classes

Krystal Revai, MD, FAAP. Written Testimony. Breastfeeding as Primary Obesity Prevention. Obesity Prevention Initiative Act Public Hearings

Indiana Association for Infant and Toddler Mental Health DC 0-3 R Crosswalk to DSM and ICD Systems

2014 New Brunswick Gambling Prevalence Study. Department of Health and Department of Finance

Strategic Plan Roadmap

FACE-TO-FACE AND ONLINE PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT FOR MATHEMATICS TEACHERS: A COMPARATIVE STUDY

Experimental methods. Elisabeth Ahlsén Linguistic Methods Course

A Self-Reflection Framework for Technology Use by Classroom Teachers of Young Learners

IMPACT OF TRUST, PRIVACY AND SECURITY IN FACEBOOK INFORMATION SHARING

Transcription:

The impact of television viewing on language development from birth to 30 months: A Critical Review of the Literature Angela Kaufman M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review examines the impact of television viewing on language development from birth to 30 months. Study designs include: non-experimental survey research (1), retrospective case-control (1), longitudinal process-product design (1) and a systematic review (1). Overall, the literature reviewed provides variable evidence regarding the topic of interest. It is thought that viewing television can have both positive and negative effects on language development, depending on frequency of exposure and content of the material being viewed. This relationship is worthy of further, more rigorous experimental investigation. Introduction Infants and toddlers in today s society are living in environments that are saturated with television and electronic media. Recently, there has been an explosion of baby DVDs and television channels specifically marketed towards these very young children. Despite this increase, little research has investigated the impact television viewing has on this very young population (Anderson & Pempek, 2005). The American Academy of Pediatrics (1999) recommends that children two years and under not be exposed to television, and children older than two years be limited to one to two hours of educational television per day. Regardless of these recommendations, a national survey revealed that 43 percent of all children under the age of two watch television every day, averaging two hours and five minutes each day (Rideout, Vandewater & Wartella, 2003). Furthermore, parents of these children conveyed that they believe television to be an important educational tool that is valuable to their children s development (Rideout et al., 2003). This finding is not surprising, since 76 of the 100 best-selling videos for infants at Amazon.com in 2005, claimed to have educational benefits (Garrison & Christakis, 2005). Given the number of infants and toddlers who are exposed to television on a daily basis, it is essential that parents, educators, researchers and health professionals pay immediate attention to the potential effects television exposure can have on the development of these very young children. One particular area of interest that requires further exploration is the impact television viewing has on language development in infants and toddlers. Reviewing the research in this area will enable Speech-Language Pathologists (SLP) to advocate and bring awareness to parents, educators and other professionals regarding the risks and benefits of television related to language development. Additionally, this information will help parents to make informed decisions based on evidence from research, for the betterment of their children. Objectives The primary objective of this paper is to provide a critical evaluation of existing literature on the impact of television viewing on language development in children from birth to 30 months of age. A secondary objective is to offer evidence-based recommendations regarding the use of television in this population and areas for future research. Methods Search Strategy Computerized databases, including PsycINFO, PubMed, SCOPUS and CINAHL were searched using the following search strategy: ((television) OR (media)) AND (language) AND ((infants) OR (toddlers)). The search was limited to articles written in English. Selection Criteria Studies selected for inclusion in this critical review paper were required to investigate the impact of television viewing on language development in children from birth to 30 months of age. With the exception of age (birth-30 months), no limits were set on the demographics of research participants or outcome measures. Data Collection Results of the literature search yielded the following types of articles congruent with the aforementioned selection criteria: non-experimental survey research (1), retrospective case-control (1), longitudinal processproduct design (1) and systematic review (1). Articles are discussed in chronological order.

Results Systematic review. Authors of this systematic review, Anderson and Pempek (2005), discuss the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) (1999) recommendation that children under the age of two years not be exposed to television. This recommendation is discussed in comparison with current research available on television exposure, foreground and background television and the impact of television on language, attention and cognitive development. For the purpose of this critical review, the only section focused on is the impact of television on language. Although the research studies discussed in this article are appropriate and relevant, the authors did not provide clear guidelines for article inclusion or exclusion, nor were search terms outlined. In addition, the studies examined by the authors related to language could have been discussed more specifically. In particular, discussions regarding the methods, procedures and subject selections were either vague or completely omitted. Additionally, the reliability and validity of results of each study could have been evaluated further. Although a comparison of results from study to study is present and a clinical bottom line is stated, their discussions and critical analyses remain vague. On the other hand, this article is valuable because it maintains and strengthens the rationalization for more research to be conducted in this area, since there is only a small subset of literature that examines the impact of television on very young children. In addition, this article offers valuable suggestions on what type of research is needed in the future. Overall, Anderson and Pempek (2005) suggest that although considerably more research is needed, evidence thus far supports the AAP recommendation that children under the age of two years not be exposed to television, given the negative association it has on language development. Given that relevant research studies have emerged since the time of this publication, it is thought that the recommendations of this paper are premature and are not supported by a sufficient amount of research. Study #1. Linebarger and Walker (2005) conducted a longitudinal process-product design study to examine the relationships between vocabulary size and expressive language outcomes with specific television programs, broader content categories and frequency of television viewing among infants. Viewing data was reported by 51 parents of infants, every three months, beginning at six months of age through to 30 months of age. All participants and their families were recruited from 17 different child care centers in the state of Kansas. Information and data was collected through direct observations, parent interviews and various standardized assessments, at specific times during the course of this study. Measures included cognitive development (Bayley Scale of Infant Development 2), vocabulary development (MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory), expressive language production (Early Childhood Indicator) and child and family characteristics (demographic information, detailed television viewing logs, Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory). Hierarchical linear modeling techniques were used to examine the relationships between television exposure and the child s vocabulary knowledge and expressive language skills. In the first step, analyses were conducted to examine each child s language growth over time. In the second step, analyses investigated whether or not the individual differences in language growth over time, varied because of different factors. These factors included frequency of television viewing, broad content categories and specific television programs. Parents education levels, child s home environment and child s current cognitive functioning at the time of each assessment were controlled for statistically. Given the small sample size, compromise power analysis was carried out to examine adjusted critical t values and alpha levels. The alpha level used was 0.19. The authors concluded that content and program type can have both negative and positive effects on vocabulary size and expressive language scores at 30 months of age. More specifically, viewing of specific shows such as Dora the Explorer, Blue s Clues, Arthur, Clifford and Dragon Tales, at 30 months, resulted in greater vocabularies and higher expressive language scores. On the other hand, watching Teletubbies was related to fewer vocabulary words and smaller expressive language scores. Similarly, Sesame Street was associated with smaller expressive language scores and Barney and Friends was related to fewer vocabulary words, yet higher expressive language scores. Although these results are quite compelling, limitations in the study design do exist. Given the correlational nature of the design, causality cannot be determined. Additionally, since the researchers were working with a small sample size, they opted to carry out a compromise power analysis. This type of power analysis is controversial as it increases the probability of a Type I error occurring. Alternatively, researchers included a variety of measures which were well defined and described in detail. The authors provided rational for including each measure, and used reliable and valid standardized assessments to evaluate each area. Overall, researchers were able to conclude that viewing television before the age of 30 months is

associated with both positive and negative effects on language development. This relationship is dependent on content and program type. It is felt that given the design of the study and the analyses carried out, as well as the strengths and limitations present, these results can be interpreted with a good degree of confidence. Therefore, it is recommended these findings be considered when determining the content of television programs that are exposed to infants. Study #2. Zimmerman, Christakis and Meltzoff (2007) examined the relationship between media exposure and language development by surveying parents of children from two to 24 months of age. The sample was selected based on the following criteria: parent(s) had to speak English fluently and have a working telephone number. In addition, only one child per household was eligible and children with significant disabilities were excluded. The final cross-sectional sample of children consisted of 385 infants (8-16 months) and 345 toddlers (17-24 months). The survey instrument included questions regarding the following independent variables: parentchild demographics, parent-child interactions and the child s frequency of viewing several content types of television and DVDs/videos. Parents were also asked to complete level one (infants) or level two (toddlers) of the short form of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Multivariate regression was used to determine how the independent variables may explain or predict variability in the measure of interest (normed CDI scores). Separate regressions were performed for the infant and toddler samples. Results of the analyses indicated that viewing baby DVDs/videos accounted for a significant amount of variability in CDI scores in infants. More specifically, for each hour per day of viewing baby DVDs/videos, this sample of infants CDI scores reflected a 16.99 point decrease. This difference corresponds to approximately six to eight words less on the CDI, for each hour per day of viewing baby DVDs/videos. Interestingly, no association was found among toddlers between any type of media exposure and CDI scores. Although more research is required to determine why this association exists, this article has its strengths. First, the CDI instrument used to measure language development has excellent internal and testretest reliability. This means that the data collected from the CDI provides reliable and valid information on each child s communicative development. In addition, including a variety of independent variables in the regression analyses was important to evaluate which variables best predicted CDI scores. Despite these strengths, this study is not without limitations. The first reflects the nonexperimental design of the study. Given that there was no experimental manipulation, causality cannot be determined. Additionally, the sample selected was not representative of the general population, since the sample selected had relatively higher income and educational levels. Lastly, media viewing was defined as and measured based on content and frequency of television viewing, in combination. It would be beneficial to look at each factor in isolation, to explore independent associations with language development. Overall, the results of this study indicate that a negative association exists between content and frequency of television exposure and language development in infants (8-16 months). It is felt that given the design of the study and the analyses carried out, as well as the strengths and limitations present, these results can be interpreted with a moderate degree of confidence. That is, the results should be taken into consideration when attempting to answer the proposed research question. Study #3. Chonchaiya and Pruksananona (2008) conducted a retrospective case-control study to investigate the impact of television viewing on language development, as well as identify individual risk factors for language delay. The case group consisted of 56 new patients with language delay at a developmental clinic, whereas the control group consisted of 110 typically developing children who were randomly selected from a well childcare clinic. Participants in this study were between 15 and 48 months of age. Assessment procedures were carried out by developmental pediatricians. This consisted of clinical history taking, a physical examination, measures of head circumference and hearing level, as well as observations of child s play, language, cognitive ability, sociability, repetitive hyperactive behavior and joint attention. A pediatrician also administered the Denver-II to assess expressive language abilities. A separate researcher conducted an interview with the parents of participating children, and asked questions regarding the child, parental/family/home environment, child disciplinary patterns and television/time characteristics. This information was analyzed using Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) and chi-square distribution. The authors did not state why these particular methods of analyses were used, nor did they specify the variables used in each analysis. There was no indication which dependent variables were considered categorical and therefore used in the chi-square distribution analysis. Similarly, the variables used in the ANOVA were not specified. Given this lack of clarity, the results of this study are considered to be equivocal. It is, therefore, not recommended that the results be taken into consideration when deciding how best to answer the research question targeted in this paper. Nevertheless, results of the analyses reported by the authors indicated that children in the case group started watching television earlier (7.22 months of age) than the control group (11.92 months of age). In

addition, the case group was found to spend more time watching television (3.05 hours per day) than the control group (1.85 hours per day). The authors suggest that these results indicate that there is a relationship between early onset and high frequency of television viewing and language delay, as children who started watching television at less than 12 months of age and watched greater than two hours per day, were six times more likely to have language delays. These results must be interpreted with great caution as there are several limitations that exist in the design of this study. The first concern relates to subject selection of the control group, who were said to have been selected from a well childcare clinic that they attended weekly. Since this type of clinic is not described in detail, it brings up questions regarding the suitability of this set of participants as a control group. Additional concerns relate to the vague descriptions of the methodology and assessment procedures. Although descriptions of general areas of assessment are listed, the authors failed to describe why and how each area was evaluated. A further limitation involves use of the Denver-II assessment tool. Since it is considered to have a low to moderate sensitivity and specificity level, children with language delay may not have been identified accurately using this instrument. Finally, given that this design is retrospective in nature, Chonchaiya and Pruksananona (2008) question the accuracy of information obtained from the parents, given limitations in human recall. It is important to mention that the article was somewhat difficult to interpret because of the presence of grammar errors and unusual wording. Given this limitation, it is difficult to interpret the results of this study with great confidence. On the other hand, this study is valuable because it brings awareness to parents and professionals on the potentially harmful effects of television viewing in infancy. This study also highlights the importance of continuing research to fill in the gaps in our knowledge. Overall, the authors report that a negative association between early onset and high frequency of television viewing and language delay exists. These results are equivocal and it is, therefore, suggested that the results be interpreted with great caution, given the limitations discussed. Discussion Overall, the examined research provides variable evidence regarding the impact of television viewing on language development in children from birth to 30 months of age. More specifically, researchers have identified both positive and negative associations between viewing television and language development in this population. Despite limitations discussed with each article, all of the literature reviewed suggests that there is a negative association between television viewing and language development in this population; however there appears to be other factors influencing this relationship. Other factors that were identified include frequency of exposure and content of television material being viewed (Chonchaiya & Pruksananonda, 2008; Linebarger & Walker, 2005; Zimmerman et al., 2007). More specifically, baby DVD/videos, Sesame Street, Teletubbies and Barney and Friends were found to be negatively associated with language development in this very young population (Linebarger & Walker, 2005; Zimmerman et al., 2007). Linebarger & Walker (2005) speculate that language and vocabulary development may be inhibited because of specific features present in these specific programs and content types. For example, it may be that these particular television shows contain poor language models (e.g. baby talk), little dialogue or a loose narrative structure (Zimmerman et al., 2007; Linebarger & Walker, 2005). Linebarger & Walker (2005) also hypothesize that having many modes of input (e.g. visual stimulation, language, music) may be too difficult for this young population to comprehend. On the other hand, results of Linebarger and Walker s (2005) investigation suggest that television viewing can have a positive impact on language development. That is, the impact, whether positive or negative, is dependent on television content. Authors Linebarger and Walker (2005) further suggest that, when specific language-promoting or languageinhibiting strategies are used with infants and toddlers in a televised format, it would be expected that these strategies would promote or inhibit communication in the same ways they have been found to in face-to-face interactions (p. 642). Conclusion At present, a concrete statement regarding the impact of television viewing on language development in this population can not be made, due to the limited research directed at this question. It is thought that viewing television can have both positive and negative effects on language development, depending on frequency of exposure and content of the material being viewed. Recommendations 1. Funding for further research would enable us to make firmer statements about the risks and benefits associated with viewing television. A. A large-scale randomized trial needs to be carried out, in which some families are actively discouraged from allowing their children to watch any television or specific television content. B. A longitudinal prospective study would be helpful to see if the effects of television

viewing on language development observed at a young age carry over or dissipate with age. C. Analysis of the content of specific television programs and movies must be investigated to determine particular features that are inhibiting or promoting language development. 2. Advertisers, creators and manufacturers of infant television programs and DVD/videos must recognize their responsibility to create viewing material that promotes language development. 3. Parents and health professionals must acknowledge the power of media exposure on infants and toddlers. They must recognize the possible risks and benefits associated with viewing television and set limitations on the frequency of exposure and content of television being viewed. 4. SLP s must educate parents on language promoting strategies and encourage them to use these strategies and avoid replacing face-to-face interactions with time spent in front of the television. References American Academy of Pediatrics. (1999). Media Education. Pediatrics, 104, 341-342. Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505-522. Chonchaiya, W., & Pruksananonda, C. (2008). Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Pediatrica, 97, 977-982. Garrison, M., & Christakis, D. (2005). A teacher in the living room? Educational media for babies, toddlers and preschoolers. Retrieved from http://www.kff.org/entmedia/upload/7427.pdf Linebarger, D. L., & Walker, D. (2005). Infants and toddlers television viewing and language outcomes. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 624-645. Rideout, V. J., Vandewater, E. A., & Wartella, E. A. (2003). Zero to six: Electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers and preschoolers. Retrieved from http://www.kff.org/entmedia/upload/zero-to- Six-Electronic-Media-in-the-Lives-of-Infants- Toddlers-and-Preschoolers-PDF.pdf Strasburger, V. C. (2007). First do no harm: Why have parents and pediatricians missed the boat on children and media. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151, 334-336. Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151, 364-368.