The Troublesome Subjunctive: An Examination of the Frequent Mistakes Made in Tense Conjugation

Similar documents
ESL 005 Advanced Grammar and Paragraph Writing

Annotated Examples of Student Writing

Comparative Analysis on the Armenian and Korean Languages

Avoiding Run-On Sentences, Comma Splices, and Fragments

National Quali cations SPECIMEN ONLY

GCSE Speaking Support Meetings. GCSE Polish Speaking. Introduction 2. Guidance 3. Assessment Criteria 4-5. Student 1 - Speaking Commentary 6-7

stress, intonation and pauses and pronounce English sounds correctly. (b) To speak accurately to the listener(s) about one s thoughts and feelings,

ELPS TELPAS. Proficiency Level Descriptors

Assessing Writing Performance Level B1

English for Academic Skills Independence [EASI]

EAST PENNSBORO AREA COURSE: LFS 416 SCHOOL DISTRICT

COMPUTER TECHNOLOGY IN TEACHING READING

Handouts for Conversation Partners: Grammar

Aim To help students prepare for the Academic Reading component of the IELTS exam.

Patricia Ryan: Don t insist on English

Discourse Markers in English Writing

Online Tutoring System For Essay Writing

GRAMMAR, SYNTAX, AND ENGLISH LANGUAGE LEARNERS

CORRECTING AND GIVING FEEDBACK TO WRITING

How to become a successful language learner

Course Syllabus My TOEFL ibt Preparation Course Online sessions: M, W, F 15:00-16:30 PST

Get Ready for IELTS Writing. About Get Ready for IELTS Writing. Part 1: Language development. Part 2: Skills development. Part 3: Exam practice

Teaching Vocabulary to Young Learners (Linse, 2005, pp )

Albert Pye and Ravensmere Schools Grammar Curriculum

Section 8 Foreign Languages. Article 1 OVERALL OBJECTIVE

Mother Tongue Influence on Spoken English

BBC Learning English - Talk about English July 11, 2005

English Grammar Passive Voice and Other Items

A Report on my Foreign Language Learning Experience BA English Language and Communication

Abstract The purpose of this paper is to present the results of my action research which was conducted in several 7 th /8 th grade language arts

BBC LEARNING ENGLISH 6 Minute Grammar Past perfect continuous

2. PRINCIPLES IN USING CONJUNCTIONS. Conjunction is a word which is used to link or join words, phrases, or clauses.

Pasadena City College / ESL Program / Oral Skills Classes / Rubrics (1/10)

LEVEL New Headway Intermediate

Exam Information: Graded Examinations in Spoken English (GESE)

Tibiscus University of Timişoara, România. 1. Aspects of the Computer Assisted English Language Learning

How to prepare for IELTS Speaking

English Language (first language, first year)

Please see current textbook prices at

EAP Grammar Competencies Levels 1 6

Proofreading and Editing:

GESE Initial steps. Guide for teachers, Grades 1 3. GESE Grade 1 Introduction

Year 3 Grammar Guide. For Children and Parents MARCHWOOD JUNIOR SCHOOL

Speaking for IELTS. About Speaking for IELTS. Vocabulary. Grammar. Pronunciation. Exam technique. English for Exams.

Rethinking the relationship between transitive and intransitive verbs

English Appendix 2: Vocabulary, grammar and punctuation

A Computer-aid Error Analysis of Chinese College Students English Compositions

TeachingEnglish Lesson plans. Conversation Lesson News. Topic: News

Pupil SPAG Card 1. Terminology for pupils. I Can Date Word

PARALLEL STRUCTURE S-10

Student Performance Q&A:

Modern foreign languages

Cambridge English: First (FCE) Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs)

How do the principles of adult learning apply to English language learners?

Assessing Speaking Performance Level B2

2013 Spanish. Higher Listening/Writing. Finalised Marking Instructions

Ling 201 Syntax 1. Jirka Hana April 10, 2006

Year 7. Grammar booklet 3 and tasks Sentences, phrases and clauses

Bilingual Education Assessment Urdu (034) NY-SG-FLD034-01

Tips for New Teachers of ESL. Prepared by UMBC Students

Italian Language & Culture Courses for Foreigners. ITALY Language Training

Accelerated Professional Program (APP) Absolute Beginner Varies (Typically 50+ units)

Virginia English Standards of Learning Grade 8

FRENCH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE TRAINING

Correlation: ELLIS. English language Learning and Instruction System. and the TOEFL. Test Of English as a Foreign Language

Spoken English for Work (SEW) exams Guide for Teachers (B1)

Roane State Community College Spanish Program Humanities Divisions. Syllabus

Student Performance Q&A:

International Journal of Asian Social Science, 2013, 3(12): International Journal of Asian Social Science

Top 2 grammar techniques, and ways to improve

0510 ENGLISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE (ORAL ENDORSEMENT)

Refer to: Present & future If-clause Main clause. ZERO Present + Present. If you can meet me at the car, that s easiest for me.

Depth-of-Knowledge Levels for Four Content Areas Norman L. Webb March 28, Reading (based on Wixson, 1999)

How Can Teachers Teach Listening?

Listening Student Learning Outcomes

The University of Findlay. College of Liberal Arts. Fall, 2013

How To Pass A Cesf

A Guide to Cambridge English: Preliminary

Types of meaning. KNOWLEDGE: the different types of meaning that items of lexis can have and the terms used to describe these

PTE Academic Preparation Course Outline

Integrating Reading and Writing for Effective Language Teaching

Assessment in Modern Foreign Languages in the Primary School

Speaking skills for Cambridge English: First for Schools (2015)

Chapter 2 Phrases and Clauses

Adlai E. Stevenson High School Course Description

Unit 1: The Three Worlds: Native Americans, Europeans and Africans Meet in NYS

Comparison of Computer Assisted Language Learning Software before Investment Dinçer Biçer [1], Ramazan Şükrü Parmaksız[2]

Subject: Spanish as a Foreign Language, Middle School Program

SYLLABUS UNIVERSIDAD DE BURGOS CURSOS INTERNACIONALES SPANISH LANGUAGE AND CULTURE COURSE B1. July 2015 NIVEL: B1

The Cambridge English Scale explained A guide to converting practice test scores to Cambridge English Scale scores

KET for Schools Reading and Writing Part 9 teacher s notes

PUSD High Frequency Word List

Indiana Department of Education

That s Not Fair! ASSESSMENT #HSMA20. Benchmark Grades: 9-12

Developing an Academic Essay

Planning a Class Session

Mastering Spanish Vocabulary by José María Navarro and Axel J. Navarro Ramil, Barron\'s Educational Series, Inc., Second Edition

Grade 5. Ontario Provincial Curriculum-based Expectations Guideline Walking with Miskwaadesi and Walking with A`nó:wara By Subject/Strand

Get Ready for IELTS Reading. About Get Ready for IELTS Reading. Part 1: Vocabulary. Part 2: Skills development. Part 3: Exam practice

Hungarian teachers perceptions of dyslexic language learners

Transcription:

The Troublesome Subjunctive: An Examination of the Frequent Mistakes Made in Tense Conjugation Đurħica AnišiĤ Senior Lecturer of English Language, Faculty of Political Science, University of Zagreb, Croatia djurdjica.anisic@zg.t-com.hr Maša Brala English teacher University College of International Relations and Diplomacy, Croatia masa.brala@gmail.com Abstract: Why do some English grammar issues pose problems to otherwise fully competent and fluent students in Croatia? From our experience, reported speech and conditional sentences rank high on the list of such issues. The aim of this paper is to explore some problems Croatian students have with the concept of the subjunctive and its application in conditional sentences. The key problems for Croatian students in forming conditional sentences derive from their inherent desire to translate Croatian to English directly, which does not address the changing tense of the verb in the conditional clause. For example, the sentence, If I were rich, I would travel the world would translate into Croatian, If I am rich, I would travel the world. In general, the concept of using a verb in the past tense to express a present or future desire is simply alien to native Slavic language speakers. Examples of common errors in the translation of conditional sentences type 2 and 3 from Croatian to English and vice versa are presented in the paper followed by some suggestions of how to help students master the concept of the subjunctive and successfully apply it in conditional sentences. Key words: English grammar, conditional sentences, subjunctive, Croatian to English Introduction Prior to analyzing on the concepts of the subjunctive and its application in conditional tenses, it is imperative to elaborate on our experiences with first-year university students at the Faculty of Political Science, Journalism Department and at the University College of International Relations and Diplomacy, respectively. At the beginning of each academic year, foreign language instructors conduct a diagnostic test to assess the language skills of the incoming students. Most of our students have graduated from grammar schools, i.e. schools that prepare students for higher education. The mean length of studying English is eight to twelve years. As a result, the majority of students have language skills that vary from B2 to levels of the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR) 66. In addition, the respective institutions review the diagnostic exams to identify how the language skills of incoming students have improved over time, especially when compared to generations matriculating more than ten or fifteen years ago, which can be attributed to the current generation s heavy exposure to Englishlanguage television, movies, music and, most of all, the internet. These circumstances have made our teaching profession much more enjoyable and even easier, largely because today s students are competent, competitive, and eager to participate in the acquisition of language. Most, if not all, understand that English is extremely important to their future professional endeavors, not only because their mother tongue is relatively unknown outside the region, but as well due to the fact that English has become the Lingua Franca of the modern age. The topic of our paper, however, has not been chosen randomly. Rather, we have detected that an unexpectedly high percentage of otherwise language competent students continue to experience problems with conditional sentences (as well as indirect speech). Thus, we consider the topic worthy of further scientific research and 66 Council of Europe, Directorate of Education, Education and Languages, http://www.coe.int/t/dg4/linguistic/cadre_en.asp 372

welcome others to contribute similar inquiries in hopes that the use of the subjunctive in conditional sentences may be better understood for future generations of students. Method of the Study In order to establish the number of students who failed to translate conditional sentences correctly (Croatian to English), we have used descriptive statistics. Conditional sentences were only one segment of the grammar exam that we used to test students' skills of the most demanding grammar issues (conditional sentences, indirect speech, and passive/active voice). The test time was not limited and students were encouraged to ask for help with words they could not remember under the pressure of testing (however, the demands for help in vocabulary were not recorded, most likely because vocabulary was intentionally made basic not to interfere with our primary interest in analyzing. grammar skills). Spelling and preposition mistakes were marked as mistakes; however, they were not sanctioned. The test comprised of six conditional sentences, three sentences with the condition in the present, (but the condition cannot be fulfilled) and three sentences with condition in the past (obviously not a subject of fulfillment). Conditional sentences with realistic condition (condition can be fulfilled) were not in the test because, in our experience, students do not have significant problems with the concept of translating the condition that can be fulfilled. This can be attributed to the fact that similar structures appear in both Croatian and English. Following are the six conditional sentences that students were asked to translate from Croatian to English: Condition in the present: Ustajala bih ranije da sam na tvom mjestu. Vińe bih vjeņbala da imam vremena. Stigao bi na autobus da krene na vrijeme. Condition in the past: Bila bi poloņila ispit da je vińe uħila. Marko bi bio kupio novi automobil da je dobio zgoditak na lutriji. Bila bih joj pomogla da je zatraņila pomoĥ. In one test group (G1), conditional sentences were grouped one after the other in the order given above. In the other test group (G2), conditional sentences randomly appeared among other grammar structures (indirect speech and active/passive voice). The reason for creating two distinctive groups was to check whether the grouping of similar structures improves overall performance. Based on our experience of correcting and grading exams, our primary assumption was that students encounter problems with the protasis clause. On the contrary, we expected the apodosis clause to be less of a problem. In order to discern which elements pose most problems, we divided each conditional sentence into two parts, the apodosis clause and the protasis clause, and marked correct or incorrect for each. The results of the test, however, proved our initial expectations were incorrect.. Findings and Discussion Each group comprised of 34 tests, which made for a total of 408 conditional sentences to be translated. We considered that more than four hundred sentences was a large enough population to gain an insight into not only the patterns, but also the types of mistakes students routinely make. By dividing sentences into elements, we ended up with 1,632 elements for analysis according to the type of mistakes. The primary difference between the groups is that G1 had conditional sentences grouped while G2 had conditional sentences intermingled with other parts of speech. For the purpose of analysis, we divided each conditional sentence into two parts, entitled A (for apodosis) and P (for protasis), in order to ascertain a clearer understanding of what primarily (at least in terms of statistics and percentages) is the root of the problem. For the purpose of measuring grammar performance, we ignored other mistakes, including spelling, incorrect use of prepositions, etc. The tables below depict the performance of the two groups, G1 and G2. G1 373

Correct Incorrect Percentage of incorrect sentences sentences sentences Conditional sentences total 204 99 105 51.5 Condition in the present 102 54 48 47.0 (type 2) Condition in the past (type 102 45 57 55.9 3) Apodosis total 204 141 63 30.9 Protasis total 204 128 76 37.3 G2 Correct sentences Incorrect Percentage of sentences incorrect sentences Conditional 204 109 95 46.6 sentences total Condition in the 102 59 43 42.2 present (type 2) Condition in the 102 50 52 50.1 past (type 3) Apodosis total 204 157 47 23.0 Protasis total 204 135 69 33.9 G1 + G2 Correct sentences Incorrect Percentage of sentences incorrect sentences Conditional 408 208 200 49.0 sentences total Condition in the 204 113 91 44.6 present (type 2) Condition in the 204 95 109 53.4 past (type 3) Apodosis total 408 263 110 26.9 Protasis total 408 298 145 35.5 As can be seen from the tables, there is a difference of 5.9 % in the performance of groups G1 and G2 in favor of group G2, where conditional sentences were randomly placed among other grammar structures. This suggests that the grouping of the same grammar structures does not add to student performance. In other words, students do not see the same grammar pattern grouped as a facilitator. Quite the opposite, it seems that the grouping of the same structures only adds to the multiplication of errors. Nearly half of the sentences were incorrect. We looked further into the types of mistakes in both apodosis and protasis and divided all of the mistakes into two categories: the first category () 67 refers to the incorrect use of a grammatical unit, e.g. would had instead of would have, would have eat instead of would have eaten, etc. The second category of mistakes () 68 refers to the incompatibility of the concept in Language One (L1 = Croatian) with the grammatical structure that expresses it in Language Two (L2 = English). Our initial consideration of this topic was that the problems for most native Croatian speakers with the conditional is their inherent desire to translate Croatian to English directly, which does not address the changing tense of the verb in the conditional 67 Some examples of mistakes are: would had helped, would had pass, would had passed, would made, would have buy, would have help, would practiced, would have catched, would have had helped, had went, etc. 68 Some examples of mistakes are: He would catch the bus if he goes on time, I would have helped her if she asked for help, I would practice more if I have time, I would have practiced more if I had time, I would have practiced more if I have time, etc. 374

clause. Hence, we expected more protasis clauses to be incorrect rather than apodosis clauses because the concept of using a verb in the past tense (i.e. present subjunctive) to express impossibility of fulfilling a condition in the present seems to be unnatural to native Croatian speakers. Below is the table that shows the types of mistakes, according to the above explained categories. G1 Type 2 apodosis 102 76 74.5% 26 25.4% 0 0% protasis 102 65 63.7% 18 17.6% 19 18.6% Type 3 apodosis 102 65 63.7% 7 6.8% 30 29.5% protasis 102 63 34.0% 34 33.3% 5 4.9% G2 Type 2 apodosis 102 81 79.4% 4 3.9% 17 16.7% protasis 102 75 73.5% 20 19.6% 7 6.9% Type 3 apodosis 102 76 74.5% 4 3.9% 22 21.6% protasis 102 60 58.9% 25 24.5% 17 16.6% G1+G2 Type 2 apodosis 204 157 77.0% 30 14.7% 17 8.3% protasis 204 140 68.6% 38 18.6% 26 12.8% Type 3 apodosis 204 141 69.1% 6 2.9% 57 27% protasis 204 123 60.3% 59 28.9% 22 10.8% The analysis of the correct use of the apodosis in conditional sentences Type 2 and Type 3 reveals that 23% apodosis in Type 2 were incorrect, compared to 29.9% incorrect apodosis clauses in Type 3. The analysis of the correct use of the protasis in conditional sentences Type 2 and Type 3 shows that 31.4% protasis clauses in Type 2 were incorrect, compared to 39.7% incorrect protasis clauses in Type 3. Below is a summary table of incorrect apodosis and protasis according to the type of mistakes. Types of mistakes in percentages + Type 2 apodosis 14.7% 8.3% 23% protasis 2.9% 27% 29.9% Type 3 apodosis 18.6% 12.8% 31.4% protasis 28.9% 10.8% 39.7% Types of mistakes in percentages + apodosis 33.3% 21.1% 54.4% protasis 31.8% 37.8% 69.6% total mistakes 65.1% 58.9% As can be seen from the above table, and to our surprise, a higher percentage of mistakes in apodosis and protasis clauses belong to the incorrect formation of grammatical structure ( = 65.1%) than to inappropriate grammatical structure ( = 58.9%). Yet when the general mistakes are compared in apodosis and protasis clauses, the difference is 27.3%, i.e. 27.3% more protasis clauses were incorrect. The difference is significant enough to seek further explanation. 375

Conclusions and Recommendations When we decided to research the causes for the numerous mistakes that Croatian students commit with conditional sentences, we began from the simple premise that there is no subjunctive in the Croatian standard language, i.e. that the concept of hypothetical discourse in Croatian is expressed by the construction with the relative conditional adverb da and the present indicative for the hypothesis in the present ( Da sam bogata,... ), and the past tense indicative for the hypothesis in the past in the protasis clause ( Da sam bila bogata,... ). We believe that our research proved this hypothesis because out of all the mistakes, 71.2% were recorded in the protasis clauses. In other words, the problem of speakers of Croatian lies in the fact that they do not bind hypothetical discourse in Croatian with structures that express it in English. This is why speakers of Croatian mirror the present tense indicative into the English protasis clause where they should use present subjunctive. Another way to look at this type of mistake is to understand that speakers of Croatian see the present indicative in a Croatian sentence as a time reference, ignoring the implied content of hypothetical discourse. In the course of their study of English, students come across the concept of subjunctive in the English language, but since this category is expressed in a different way in Croatian, students seem to ignore the concept. The numerous mistakes within the category and the incorrect formation of grammatical structures were the biggest surprise for us. The total number of this type of mistakes is 6.2% higher than the number of mistakes in the application of the correct grammatical structure. What is the reason for this? We believe that such a high number of mistakes in the formation of grammatical structure lie in the fact that conditional sentences use grammatical structures that have at least two or three forming elements, which make native Croatian speakers more prone to committing mistakes. It is instructive to assess some of the mistakes that students made, for example in the structures with two elements... if he had win,... He would caught the bus,... I would got up earlier,... She would passed the exam if; or in the structures with three elements... I would had helped her if...,... she would had pass the exam,... Marko would be bought a new car...,... I would been helped her if..., Marko would have buy a new car if..., etc. This Rashomon type interpretation of the formation of complex grammatical structures is, in our opinion, easy to solve. The solution is in exercising particular structures that cause problems, both in oral and written form. In our experience, students master the area after consistent exercise and repetition over a relatively short period of time. The next, and more demanding task, is to explain and make students understand the concept of hypothetical discourse. We have observed that providing explanations in connection with grammar issues in Croatian gives much better results because students rarely have the knowledge of specialized grammar vocabulary, which makes it difficult for them to follow explanations in English. We make them understand that different languages use a variety of conditional constructions and verb forms to form conditional sentences discussing hypothetical situations and their consequences. The instruction about the present subjunctive as not connected to past time reference in conjunction with translation exercises quite soon brings positive results. We begin translation exercises with English conditional clauses that are translated to Croatian and, after that, vice versa. After a period of pause, we do the same exercises again and, most often this is enough to help students understand and master the concept of conditional sentences in both languages. 376