The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools

Similar documents
The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools Early Report Newsletter Spring 2002

TEXAS RISING STAR WEBINAR SERIES: CURRICULUM AND EARLY LEARNING GUIDELINES RECORDED OCTOBER 29, 2015 NOTES

Master of Science in Early Childhood Education Singapore,

Master of Science in Early Childhood Education Singapore,

M.A. in School Counseling /

Course Syllabus College of Education Winona State University. Department: Special Education Date: August 2011

Closing the Assessment Loop Report 2010 Early Childhood Education Program

Resource Guide for High School Transitions:

Illustration Sylvie Wickstrom

Evaluation of Minnesota s Race to the Top-Early Learning Challenge: Scholarships and Title I PreK Incentives

School of Social and Behavioral Sciences

University Child Care Centre EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

Position Statement IDENTIFICATION OF STUDENTS WITH SPECIFIC LEARNING DISABILITIES

Barriers to Family Involvement

Self-Concept and Self-Esteem in Adolescents

CURRICULUM VITA Janina Washington-Birdwell, M.A., LSSP, Psy.D.

DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY PROGRAM

How Our Early Childhood Products. help strengthen

Virtual Child Written Project Assignment. Four-Assignment Version of Reflective Questions

Evaluation of Minnesota s Race to the Top-Early Learning Challenge Access Strategies: Scholarships and Title I PreK Incentives- Year 2

DIABLO VALLEY COLLEGE CATALOG

DEPARTMENT OF SPECIAL EDUCATION AND DISABILITY POLICY

Early Childhood Education Scholarships: Implementation Plan

Task Request 1 Special Education Annotated Bibliography

The Elementary Education Program Brandeis University Waltham, MA 02454

The Summer Reading Challenge evaluation results

School of Education MASTER OF SCIENCE IN SPECIAL EDUCATION. Master of Science in Special Education

EA 597 School Planning and Facilities Management (3)

Core Qualities For Successful Early Childhood Education Programs. Overview

Targeted Needs Training. Training Topics. Training Techniques

III. FREE APPROPRIATE PUBLIC EDUCATION (FAPE)

How To Improve Your Head Start Program

Bachelor of Arts in Early Childhood Degree Offerings through the Catalog*

How To Get Credit For Prior Learning/Work Experience In Early Childhood Education

Youth Mental Health Training

VITA. Mailing Address: 275 Eastland Road, Division of Education, Berea, Ohio Office Telephone Number/Fax Number: (440) ; (440)

KIMBERLY EIKO HIRABAYASHI

EXCEPTIONAL. Concordia University School of Human Services 275 Syndicate Street North, St. Paul, MN (651) Fax (651)

Bullying Prevention and Intervention:

Alignment of the Hawaii Preschool Content Standards With HighScope s Preschool Child Observation Record (COR), 2nd edition

Creating Successful Early Learning and All-Day Kindergarten Programs. Minnesota Department of Education Office of Early Learning

PLAY STIMULATION CASE STUDY

EARLY CHILDHOOD EDUCATION

Pre-Requisites EDAM-5001 Early Literacy Guiding Principles and Language

Teachers Special Education

Games and Activities that Build Academic Vocabulary

Key Components of Literacy Instruction

Parent Education Activities

Marcia E. Humpal, M.Ed., MT-BC Ronna S. Kaplan, M.A. MT-BC. Journal of Music Therapy

SPECIAL EDUCATION AND DISABILITY POLICY (SEDP)

Admissions Requirements

School Psychology Program Goals, Objectives, & Competencies

APA 2003 Poster Session Proposal. Preventing Bullying in Schools: A Community Action Research Approach

Dual Language Program Questions and Answers: What does research say?

Educational Psychology Spring, 2010 Dr. Kenn Mann

BOK Course Title Course Description Access to Children

To answer the secondary question, if hands-on activities would increase student interest and comprehension, several hands-on activities were used:

Child s Developmental Stages: A Challenge to Relevancy and Curriculum Development in Children s Church

Child Care Center and Preschool Quality Framework. November 2013

Western Carolina University Program Assessment Plan Program: School Psychology College of Education and Allied Professions

Creating and Maintaining Positive Partnerships With Parents. Mona Spells Adou

Risk and Resilience 101

x 5 Africa Education Review x 6 African American Review x 7 Alberta Journal of Educational Research

Elina Saeki, PhD, NCSP

MA EDUCATION MA Education: Childhood and Youth Studies MA Education: Higher Education MA Education: Leadership and Management MA Education: TESOL

The University of North Carolina at Pembroke Academic Catalog

Given the increasing diversity of

EXHIBIT 4.1 Curriculum Components & Experiences that Address Diversity Proficiencies

Saint Paul Public Schools Special Education Family Resource Fair February 20, 2015

The Role of Occupational Therapy for Children with ACC

Master s in Educational Psychology. School Counseling Track

APA Div. 16 Working Group Globalization of School Psychology

Child Abuse: Working with Abused & Neglected Children

Department of. Curriculum, Foundations, and Reading. Curriculum, Foundations, and Reading. Degrees. Endorsement. Doctoral Degrees

HEAD START PERFORMANCE STANDARDS W/ MENTAL HEALTH FOCUS

POSITIONS AND POLICIES ON EDUCATION Early Childhood Education/Preschool

Lac du Flambeau Tribal Early Childhood Education Program for Expectant Families, Infants, Toddlers and Preschoolers

Foundations of the Montessori Method (3 credits)

Assessment That Drives Instruction

What Is the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program?

SUSTAINABILITY. Goal: Sustain environmental literacy by ensuring effective implementation of the 2010 Environmental Literacy for Illinois Plan.

Maria V. Dixon, M.A., CCC-SLP 402 Ridge Rd. #8 // Greenbelt, MD (301)

School-based Support Personnel

ETR PR-06 Form. Annotations for the New. Contents. Using the ETR Form document. Evaluation Team Report

Policy Brief: Penn State Study of Early Childhood Teacher Education James E. Johnson, Richard Fiene, Kate McKinnon, & Sudha Babu

Behaving Intelligently: Leadership Traits & Characteristics Kristina G. Ricketts, Community and Leadership Development

NASP Position Statement on Home-School Collaboration: Establishing Partnerships to Enhance Educational Outcomes

The Impact of Digital Technology on Learning: A Summary for the Education Endowment Foundation

Research Assistant/Project Assistant, University

Requirements EDAM WORD STUDY K-3: PRINT AWARENESS, LETTER KNOWLEDGE, PHONICS, AND HIGH FREQUENCY WORDS

SAME DIVERSE DIFFERENT. Understanding Children Who Are Dual Language Learners (DLLs)

A Manual for Using the Massachusetts EEC Preschool Learning Experiences Course For. Professional Development. Self Study Guide

Coun 589 Action Research in Counseling Wednesdays, 4-6:30 pm, Fall Term 1 credit

Lone Star College-Tomball Community Library Tomball Parkway Tomball, TX

Elementary MEd I. The Relationship of the Program with the Unit s Conceptual Framework

* Lab Experience Course Code: (1) General Studies/Core (3) Major ** Field Experience (2) Enhanced General Studies (4) Professional Education

SPECIAL EDUCATION AND DISABILITY STUDIES

Kristen Marie Bottema-Beutel, Ph.D. Campion Commonwealth Ave. Chestnut Hill, MA

Supporting Students in Their Transition to Middle School Association for Middle Level Education (AMLE)

... and. Uses data to help schools identify needs for prevention and intervention programs.

Transcription:

spring 2002 vol. 29 no. 1 center for early education and development http://education.umn.edu/ceed The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools Anthony Pellegrini, University of Minnesota Peter Blatchford, University of London in this issue The Developmental and Educational Significance of Recess in Schools / pp. 1 7 UPDATE Ceed-Affiliated Projects / pp. 8 12 New CEED Publications / pp. 13 14 Recent Faculty Publications / pp. 14 18 Upcoming Events / p. 19 CEED-Affiliated Projects / p. 20 In Britain and the USA, recess (or break time) is slowly being phased out of the school day. For example, a national survey conducted in England in 1995-1996 showed that lunchtime break had been reduced, relative to 1990 1991, in 38% of the elementary schools. Further, afternoon breaks have been eliminated altogether in 27% of the elementary schools surveyed and 12% and 14%, respectively, of preschool and middle schools (see Pellegrini & Blatchford, 2000). Proponents of recess reduction in both countries maintain that recess detracts from an already limited instructional time budget and provides opportunities for students to exhibit anti-social behavior, such as negative peer relations and aggression. However, we maintain that recess plays a positive role in children s academic achievement and social development and, ultimately, in school adjustment. Our research has shown that breaks, or play, actually encourage academic achievement. Furthermore, youngsters learn valuable social skills by interacting with peers at break time, and these social skills help them to adjust to school. Recess and Academic Achievement Educational research, in contrast to current educational policy, consistently indicates that break time does have positive educational value. For example, in four field experiments conducted in American elementary schools, we found (Pellegrini & Davis, 1993; Pellegrini, Huberty, & Jones, 1995) that the longer children worked without a break on standardized tasks, the less attentive to the task they became. In addition, children were more attentive to class work after recess than before.

early report page 2 Anthony Pellegrini Dr. Pellegrini is Professor of Educational Psychology at the University of Minnesota. His interests include observational research methods, children s peer relations, and social contextual influences on classroom achievement. Currently he has research grants to study aggression in middle schools, oral language bases of early literacy, and children s playground games. Pellegrini s awards and honors include: Fellow, American Psychological Association (Educational and Developmental Psychology); Traveling Fellow, British Psychological Society; Fellow, National Conference on Research in Language and Literacy; Fellow, National Institute of Health/Senior International; University of Georgia Creative Research Medal in Social Science; and, honorary professor, University of Cardiff (Wales). He was the keynote lecturer for the British Psychological Society: Developmental and Educational Psychological Section Meeting. Why is this the case? Two current theories in educational research attempt to answer this question. One theory maintains that breaks inserted between periods of intense work serve to distribute effort and increase cognitive performance. For over a century, experiments have been conducted in this area with consistent results participants, regardless of age, learn better and more quickly when their efforts are distributed compared to when they are massed. Children and adults alike benefit from breaks during periods of intense work. Another theory states that when children are exposed to a series of demanding cognitive tasks, cognitive interference occurs, with a resulting decline in performance. Learners of different ages are released from this interference in different ways young children respond well to non-structured breaks, whereas older learners benefit from simply changing tasks. Recess and Peer Relations In addition to its apparent academic benefit, recess is extremely valuable to social development. Recess may be one of the few times during the school day when children can interact with peers on their own terms and learn and practice important social skills. Games, in particular, seem to serve an important role for young children since they provide a familiar routine (Bateson, 1976) around which unacquainted children

earlyreport page 3 can interact. These interactions form the basis for subsequent social relationships in school. In a recent study of playground games in elementary schools in Minneapolis, we found that children s recess was spent in cooperative interaction, much of which involved rule-governed games with peers. Importantly, and consistent with other research (Pellegrini, 1988), very little aggressive behavior was observed. Being good at games on the playground was important to general social competence, which we measured as a combination of peer endorsements and adult-rated social competence. In addition, children s success at games during the first part of the school year predicted their social competence at the end of the year. Playground games are especially important at the start of the school year, when peers are not familiar with each other. Shared knowledge of a game can be used by relatively unfamiliar children as an initial basis for interaction. After repeated interactions in games, children become familiar with each other and then interact in other domains. Expertise with the sorts of games played on the playground results in high peer status as children of this age choose to affiliate with others with whom they share play activities (Hartup, 1983). Children who are leaders in playground games are sought out by peers as sources of group activity. We found, however, that children s roles in games changed across the school year. In a study of playground games in elementary schools in Guilford, England, children came to school with knowledge of games learned from an adult or an older sibling, but in the course of interacting with their peers, they adapted the behavior to their new environment. At the very beginning of the school year, games appeared to support new social contacts

early report page 4 initiation of games could be seen as initiation of a new social relationship. As the school year continued, games reinforced existing social groups and friendships. However, children differ in their willingness and ability to initiate and sustain playground games. The Guilford Study identified a few pupils who took on the responsibility of suggesting, maintaining, and terminating games. These key players appeared to have a crucial role in the development of friendships and games. Case studies indicated that key players are likely to be popular and to be seen by peers as group leaders, but differences within this group were also apparent, particularly between boys and girls. Boys identified as key players seemed to hold this position because of physical prowess, girls because of social skills and imagination, possibly associated with academic progress. These results were replicated in the Minneapolis study, wherein children nominated by their peers as game leaders were also the most socially competent, or popular. Game leader status also increased with the size of the play group in which children were observed. Size of play groups, in turn, was correlated with social competence. We suspect that the leadership skills of game leaders make them attractive to their peers, as indicated by their receipt of peer nominations and their attraction of many peers to their play groups. Once in these play groups, leaders gain additional skills to boost their social competence by practicing and learning additional social skills. Children s success with one dimension of peer relations (being good at playground games) predicted their adjustment to school. Using preliminary data from our Minneapolis sample, we found a positive association between children nominated by their peers as being good at games and sports and school adjustment in the first year of elementary school (as rated by teachers and researchers). We suspect that the success that children experience at playground games translates to more general feelings of competence in school. Related Courses Offered by the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of Minnesota EPsy 5200: Special Topics: Seminar on Children s Play (Spring 2003) EPsy 5216: Introduction to Research in Educational Psychology EPsy 8216: Seminar in Research Processes For more information about these courses, visit http:// education.umn.edu/edpsych/ and click on Courses.

earlyreport page 5 These findings are especially interesting given the ethnic diversity of the children in the Minneapolis sample. In the two elementary schools studied in Minneapolis, the majority of children were of African American, Latino, or Asian American (mostly Vietnamese and Hmong) backgrounds, not of European American backgrounds. A major developmental task for children of this age, of course, is adjustment to school. This task is especially difficult for different cultural groups because the culture upon which American public education is based is, generally, different from that of these minority cultures (Heath, 1983). Our results suggest that when children, regardless of their cultural background, are valued by their peers for success in one dimension of school (success with games in the playground), it has a positive influence on their adjustment to this new environment. Playground games are also important in the development of heterosexual relationships for older children. Differences in how boys and girls play, spend break time, and develop social networks are well documented (Maccoby, 1998; Pellegrini, 2001). For example, in elementary school boys tend to play with other boys because they enjoy physically vigorous activities, and girls segregate themselves from boys play groups because they do not enjoy rough play (Pellegrini & Smith, 1998). Boys and girls do, however, begin to interact with each other socially during early adolescence (Pellegrini, 1995, 2001). For example, Pellegrini (2001) found that as boys progressed through middle school, they engaged less frequently in physically vigorous play and more frequently in sedentary social interaction during break times. Adolescents used these break time opportunities to explore heterosexual relationships (Pellegrini, 2001). For example, boys and girls alike used forms of poke and push courtships to interact with peers of the opposite sex (Maccoby, 1998). They would push, play hit, and tease opposite sex peers as a relatively safe way in which to initiate cross-sex interaction. These early forms of courtship are safe because they are ambiguous. If there is a positive response to the overture, fine interaction with an

early report page 6 opposite sex peer was successfully initiated. If the overture is rebuffed, the initiator saves face with his/her peers because it was playful, i.e., not a serious overture. Conclusions Recess improves academic achievement, peer relations, and school adjustment. For young children, it can provide a welcome respite from intense cognitive activity. During the elementary school years, it is especially valuable as rare time in which children can develop peer relations and social skills. These skills, in turn, are important to children s successful adjustment to school. Successful peer relations in school and academic success directly support each other (e.g., Coie & Dodge, 1998; Pellegrini & Blatchford, 2000). The idea that school breaks provide opportunities for children to learn and practice social skills with their peers is noted in the context of decreasing opportunities for peer interaction in children s lives. For example, a survey in the early 1990s found that London students are now far less likely to walk to school, presumably accompanied by other students, than they were 20 years ago (Hillman, 1993). Likewise, after school, many American children return to empty homes, waiting for their parent(s) to return from work (Steinberg, 1988), instead of going to a friend s house or an after-school program. In both America and England there is concern with antisocial behavior in youth generally and in schools, specifically. By way of solutions, many American politicians are calling for special programs to teach social skills in school. We suggest that most children learn social skills by interacting with their peers in meaningful social situations, such as recess. Recess provides an extended classroom in which children can learn important social skills. These skills should be considered educational. In addition, spaced break times directly improve children s attention to class work. Further, the social skills learned on the playground relate to more general school adjustment.

earlyreport page 7 References Bateson, P. P. G. (1976). Discontinuity in development and changes in the organization of the play in cats. In K. Immelmann, G. Barlow, L. Petronovich, & M. Main (Eds.). Behavioral development (pp. 281-295). London: Cambridge University Press. Blatchford, P. (1998). Social life in school. London: Falmer. Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.). Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3 (pp 779-862). New York: Wiley. Hartup, W. (1983). Peer relations. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.) Handbook of child psychology, Vol. IV (pp. 103-196). NY: Wiley. Heath, S. (1983). Ways with words. New York: Cambridge University Press. Hillman, M. (1993) One false move. In M. Hillman (Ed.) Children, transport and the quality of life. London: Policy Studies Institute. Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Pellegrini, A. D. (1988). Elementary school children s rough-andtumble play and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 24, 802-806. Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). School recess and playground behavior. Albany: State University of New York Press. Pellegrini, A. D. (2001). A longitudinal study of heterosexual relationships, aggression, and sexual harassment during the transition from primary school through middle school. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 119-133. Pellegrini, A. D., & Blatchford, P. (2000). The child at school: Interactions with peers and teachers. London: Arnold. Pellegrini, A. & Davis, P. (1993). Relations between children s playground and recess behavior. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 88-95. Pellegrini, A. D., Huberty, P. D., & Jones, I. (1995). The effects of recess timing on children s playground and classroom behaviors. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 845-864. Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith. P K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 68, 577-598. Steinberg. L. (1988). Latchkey children and susceptibility to peer pressure. Developmental Psychology, 22, 433-439.

early report page 8 U P D AT E Ceed-Affiliated Projects UPDATE Ceed-Affiliated Projects New Grant Announcement: Early Literacy Training Project CEED has been awarded a $1.4 million grant from the Office of Elementary and Secondary Education of the U.S. Department of Education. This exciting opportunity will fund the Early Literacy Training Project. Under the overall direction of Mary McEvoy, Chair and Professor of Educational Psychology and Lead Researcher, the project will be coordinated by Angèle Passe and Kate Horst. The project is a creative collaboration between the University of Minnesota, CEED, the Minnesota Child Care Resource and Referral Network, Minneapolis Public Schools, the Minnesota Department of Children, Families and Learning Head Start/Child Care Team, the White Earth Indian Reservation, and the Greater Minneapolis Day Care Association. Over the next two years this grant will allow CEED and its partners to revise and refine an existing early education curriculum focusing on early literacy and language; develop a cadre of educators who will train preschool teachers across the state; work with preservice training programs to assure that teacher preparation includes early literacy and language competencies; and manage an interactive Web site to disseminate research-based information about literacy and language development for young children. A critical component of this project will be to provide intensive technical assistance to preschool educators in four pilot sites in Minneapolis and at the White Earth Indian Reservation and to study the impact of this formal training by assessing children s early literacy skills as they enter kindergarten. The end goal is to help Minnesota preschool children develop better early literacy and language skills as a solid foundation for later reading ability. Get It, Got It, Go! Tools for Improving Children s Developmental Outcomes Get It, Got It, Go! is a comprehensive Web site with informational materials and database-driven systems to help educators and researchers select measurement tools to monitor children s development, manage the data acquired, and collaborate with parents and peers.

earlyreport page 9 Get It Obtain informational materials and assessment tools for measuring the developmental growth of children from birth to age eight. Got It Enter individual child data and generate graphical reports to monitor the developmental growth of individual children and groups of children, and determine if intervention is necessary. Go! Communicate and collaborate about a child s progress over time and about intervention plans to improve child outcomes. We are currently alpha-testing the Get It and Got It parts of the Web site To get feedback on the overall structure and design. To obtain input from informed users so we can fill-in and revise the content to ensure that it is accurate and complete. To capture feedback and locate problems about the data-driven tools. U P D AT E Ceed-Affiliated Projects Head Start QNet Project Update In addition to providing support to specific Head Start grantees through partnership agreements, the Great Lakes Head Start Quality Network (QNet) is working with the Department of Children, Families and Learning (CFL), the Minnesota Head Start Association, and QUILT. QUILT is a national training and technical assistance project funded by the federal Head Start and Child Care Bureaus to support full-day, full-year partnerships among child care, Head Start, pre-kindergarten, and other early education programs at the local, state, tribal, territorial, and regional levels. This year QNet is working with the state collaboration officers, state monitors, and the integration team of the Minnesota Department of Children, Families and Learning to align QNet and CFL priorities and work (when possible and as necessary). This effort is focused on finding the best way to support Minnesota Head Start grantees.

early report page 10 Minnesota Infant Mental Health Project This year the focus of the Minnesota Infant Mental Health Project is on creating an online credit course on working with preschoolers and their families using a relationship-based approach grounded in infant mental health theory and practice. Susan Schultz, Ph.D., and Anne R. Gearity, LICSW, are helping to develop the curriculum for the new course, Working with Preschoolers and their Families: Relationship-based Practice. The new course is being prepared to complement the course, Positive Approaches to Challenging Behavior in Young Children, being developed through CEED (see update on page 12.) Together, the two courses will offer teachers a complementary set of theories and assessment and intervention strategies for use in meeting the diverse needs and addressing the specific behaviors of children in their classrooms. In addition, the Project sponsored a presentation by Stella Acquarone, founder and director of the School of Infant Mental Health in London, and an early childhood strand at the annual conference sponsored by the Minnesota Association for Children s Mental Health in Duluth this past April. U P D AT E Ceed-Affiliated Projects The Minnesota Infant Mental Health Project is a cross-agency initiative funded by the Minnesota Departments of Health; Human Services; and Children, Families, and Learning that seeks to establish and support a statewide framework of mental health services for infants, young children, and their families by enhancing existing programs and resources. For more information, go to the CEED Web site at http:// education.umn.edu/ceed/projects/ infantmentalhealth/. PICA Head Start and CEED Collaborative Researchers from CEED have joined the Parents in Community Action (PICA) Head Start program of Minneapolis in an exciting new collaborative venture. The goal of this project is to introduce PICA Head Start teachers to CEED s Individual Growth and Development Indicators, or IGDIs. IGDIs are efficient, easy to administer tools that produce reliable information about a child s development in areas such as early literacy and language skill. IGDIs are analogous to height and weight charts. They provide an indication about a child s overall health in important developmental domains. In early January, researchers from CEED and administrators from PICA worked together to teach teachers across all Head

earlyreport page 11 Start centers in Minneapolis to administer and interpret early literacy and language IGDIs. Specifically, they taught teachers to administer rhyming, alliteration, and picture naming IGDIs. Once teachers were trained, they administered the measures to all children in their preschool classrooms within a three-week data collection window. This spring, teachers will be administering the IGDIs to children again. Currently, CEED researchers are in the process of analyzing and interpreting the IGDI data and hope to disseminate the data back to teachers in the coming weeks. This project provides a worthwhile and engaging opportunity for both teachers and researchers. While IGDIs have been administered and tested by researchers at the Early Childhood Research Institute, this is one of the first large-scale dissemination efforts in which teachers have the opportunity to use and give feedback about the measures. A main purpose of IGDIs is that they be sensitive to instructional change. PICA and CEED staff hope that the use of IGDIs by teachers will add to an awareness of the importance of creating classroom environments that are language and literacy rich. U P D AT E Ceed-Affiliated Projects I PROMICE Improving Preschoolers Reading Outcomes Through Measurement and Intervention in Classroom Environments I PROMICE examines relationships between preschoolers growth of their expressive language/pre-literacy skills and their later performance in becoming proficient readers. These preschoolers include children with disabilities, children whose primary language is Spanish, and children attending Head Start programs. I PROMICE is continuing to follow a cohort of children recruited in the first year of the project. Administration of preschool Individual Growth and Development Indicators (IGDI) and kindergarten Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) general outcome measures are conducted seasonally as are ecobehavioral classroom observations. An additional cohort of children were recruited this past year and are being followed as well. Preschool (IGDI) general outcome measures are administered monthly to these children and ecobehavioral classroom observations are conducted seasonally. Children are still being recruited for this cohort and researchers hope that all of the children will continue to be involved in the project next year.

early report page 12 U P D AT E Ceed-Affiliated Projects This will allow them to collect more data regarding the relationship between early childhood measures and beginning reading measures. Another component of the project is scheduled to begin in late Spring 2002. Project staff will begin to recruit teachers to assist in developing a classroom intervention program. The program will be designed using information about the relationship between child performance on language and early literacy measures and classroom ecological variables. This intervention component will be implemented with a new group of children to be recruited in the Fall. Early Childhood Behavior Project The Early Childhood Behavior Project is currently providing training in three communities to develop teams to address the needs of young children who engage in challenging behavior. The multidisciplinary teams have completed the Positive Approaches to Problem Behavior in Young Children coursework and are currently receiving technical assistance from project personnel as they proceed with their first referrals. Responses to referrals have included developing successful environmental arrangements, conducting functional behavioral assessments and developing positive support plans. In addition to responding to referrals, the two rural community teams have presented workshops to child care providers in their areas. The Positive Approaches to Problem Behavior in Young Children Web site (http:// ici2.umn.edu/preschoolbehavior) has seen a steady increase in use and popularity. CEED staff are continually enhancing the Web site with information and tools for parents and professionals, including a public discussion board and live chats with experts on early childhood. It is anticipated that development of two online courses will be completed for 2002-2003 1. Positive Approaches to Problem Behavior in Young Children focuses on functional behavioral assessment and developing positive behavior support plans. Interventions taught in this course include strategies in communicative replacement. 2. Working with Preschoolers and their Families: Relationshipbased Practice will enhance professionals awareness of the relational and developmental dimensions of children s experiences and behaviors. Availability of these courses will be announced on our Web site, or CEED can notify you via e-mail if you contact Karen Anderson at ander352@umn.edu, type Notify me as the subject, and include course #1 or course #2 in your message.

earlyreport page 13 New CEED Publications Talking Reasonably and Responsibly About Brain Development Trainer Edition This guide has been developed to assist in training child care providers, families, parent educators, and others about myths and misunderstandings on the subject of early brain development,. It is also intended for use by those who would like to incorporate early brain development information into their trainings on other topics. The guide is divided into four modules 1. Overview of early brain development. 2. How to be a savvy consumer of research about brain development (or any topic). 3. Analysis of public messages about early brain development. 4. How to blend brain development information with child development information. The guide is complete with readyto-use diagrams and overheads, active learning exercises, glossary of terms, and resource list. Cost is $25, to order, visit http://education. umn.edu/ceed and click on publications, or call 612-625-3058. Questions About Kids? If you re a parent or professional with questions about children s development, Questions About Kids is for you! Questions About Kids are flyers that provide answers to important questions parents have about their children s development. We re introducing a new series of eight Questions About Kids focused on infants and toddlers. They include a range of topics that address contemporary concerns and highlight the unique delights and challenges of the first years of life. They were written by experts at the University of Minnesota and in the Twin Cities community and were reviewed by experts at the University of Minnesota. For the first time, some of the Questions About Kids are available in Spanish, Somali, and Hmong. They are being distributed via the web and through community health, social service, and parent education programs. We encourage you to use them as you see fit. The new topics are How can parents and caregivers support a baby s healthy development? What s going on in my baby s brain?

early report page 14 How do I get to know my newborn? How can trauma affect my young child? Am I spoiling my baby? What does it mean when my young child is assessed? Do dads really make a difference? How can I help my young child to become a reader? Questions About Kids is a joint project of the Irving B. Harris Training Center for Infant and Toddler Development and the Center for Early Education and Development, University of Minnesota. Questions About Kids is on the Web at http://www.harristraining center.org and http://education.umn. edu/ceed/publications. Questions About Kids may be downloaded and reproduced as necessary, with no copyright restrictions. Our goal is to provide current, accurate information to parents. Hand them out individually to families, use them to foster group discussion, or follow up on a conversation. Recent Faculty Publications Deborah Ceglowski Ceglowski, D. (in press). Critical perspectives on Project Head Start: Revisioning the hope and challenge. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. (Review of book). Ceglowski, D. (in press). Research as relationship in N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.) The Qualitative Inquiry Reader (pp. 88-103). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ceglowski, D. (2001). Who s making these policies anyway? In J. Jipson & R. Johnson (Eds.) Resistance and representation in early childhood (pp. 239-251). New York: Peter Lang. Ceglowski, D., & Bacigalupa, C. (in press). Keeping current in child care research: An update of the 1985 annotated bibliography. Early Childhood Research and Practice. Ceglowski, D., & Bacigalupa, C. (in press). Four perspectives on child care quality. Early Childhood Journal.

earlyreport page 15 Scott McConnell Carta, J. J., Atwater, J. B., Greenwood, C. R., McConnell, S. R., McEvoy, M. A., & Williams, R. (2001). Effects of cumulative prenatal substance exposure and multiple environmental risks on children s developmental trajectories. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 30, 327-337. McConnell, S. R. (2001). Parent involvement and family support: Where do we want to go, and how will we know we are headed there? Journal of Early Intervention, 24, 15-18. McConnell, S. R. (In press). Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young children with autism: Review of available research and recommendations for educational intervention and future research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. McConnell, S. R., McEvoy, M. A. & Rush, K. (In press). Social interaction, social competence, and social development of Head Start children: A call for intervention development and validation in a culturally diverse context. In J. Heller (Ed.) Head Start-University partnership: Issues in child development. Washington, D.C., ACYF. McConnell, S. R., Missall, K. N., Silberglitt, B., & McEvoy, M. A. (2002). Promoting social development in preschool classrooms. In M. Shinn, G. Stoner, & H. M. Walker (Eds.), Interventions for academic and behavior problems II: Preventive and remedial approaches (pp. 501-536). Washington, D.C.: National Association of School Psychologists. McConnell, S. R., Priest, J. S., Davis, S. D., & McEvoy, M. A. (2002). Best practices in measuring growth and development for preschool children. In A. Thomas and J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology (4th ed.)(pp. 1231 1246). Washington DC: National Association of School Psychologists. McConnell, S. R., Rush, K. L., McEvoy, M. A., Carta, J. J., Atwater, J. & Williams, R. (In press). Descriptive and experimental analysis of child-caregiver interactions that promote development of young children exposed prenatally to drugs and alcohol. Journal of Behavioral Education. McEvoy, M. A., Priest, J. S., Kaminski, R. S., Carta, J. J., Greenwood, C. R., McConnell, S. R., Good, R. H., Walker, D. & Shinn, M. R. (In press). General growth outcomes: Wait! There s more! Journal of Early Intervention. Priest, J.S., McConnell, S. R., Walker, D., Carta, J. J., Kaminski, R. A., McEvoy, M. A., Good, R. H., Greenwood, C. R., & Shinn, M. R. (in press). General growth outcomes for children between birth and age eight: Where do we want young children to go today and tomorrow? Journal of Early Intervention.

early report page 16 Mary McEvoy McConnell, S., Missall, K., Silberglitt, B., & McEvoy, M. (2001). Promoting social development in preschool classrooms. To appear in M. Shinn, G. Stoner, & H. Walker (Eds.), Interventions for academic and behavior problems II: Preventive and remedial approaches. Washington, DC: National Association of School Psychologists. McEvoy, M., & Neilsen, S. (in press). Functional assessment of behavior. To appear in M. McLean, M. Wolery, & D. Bailey (Eds.), Assessing infants and preschoolers with special needs. Columbus, OH: Merrill Prentice Hall. McEvoy, M., Priest, J., Kaminski, R., Carta, J., Greenwood, C., McConnell, S., Good, R., Walker, D., & Shin, M. (2002). General growth outcomes: Wait! There s more! Journal of Early Intervention, 24. Olive, M., & McEvoy, M. (2001). Issues in early intervention. To appear in A. McCray, H. Rieth, & P. Sindelar (Eds.), Issues in Special Education. Priest, J., McConnell, S., Walker, D., Carta, J., Kaminski, R., McEvoy, M. et al. (2001). General growth outcomes for children between birth and age eight: Developing and validating a foundation for a continuous progress measurement system. Manuscript submitted for publication. Anthony Pellegrini Pellegrini, A. (2001). Sampling instances of victimization in middle school. In S. Graham and J. Juvonen (Eds.) School-based peer harassment: The plight of the vulnerable and the victimized. New York: Guilford. Pellegrini, A., & Smith, P.K. (2001). Play and the development of children. In N. J. Smelser and P. Baltes (Eds.). Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences. Kidlington, UK: Elsevier. Pellegrini, A. (2001). From the lab of Anthony Pellegrini: Bullying in school. In R. J. Sternberg. In search of the human mind (3 rd Edition). Fort Worth: Harcourt College Publisher. Pellegrini, A. (2001). Two social contextual routes to the development of school-based literacy. In S. Neuman and D. Dickinson (Eds.) Handbook on research in early literacy for the 21 st century. New York: Guilford. Pellegrini, A., & Galda, L. (2001). I m so glad, I m glad, I m glad: The role of emotions and close relationships in children s play and narrative language. In A. Goncu and E. Klein (Eds.) Young children in play, story, and school: Essays in honor of Greta Fein. New York: Guilford. Pellegrini, A., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Children s play: A developmental and evolutionary orientation. In J.

earlyreport page 17 Valsiner and K. Connolly (Eds.). Handbook of developmental psychology. London: Sage. Pellegrini, A. (2002). The development and possible functions of rough-and-tumble play. In C. H. Hart and P. K. Smith (Eds.). Handbook of social development. Oxford: Blackwell. Pellegrini, A., & Bjorklund, D. (2002). Evolutionary psychology and children s social development. In C. H. Hart and P. K. Smith (Eds.). Handbook of social development. Oxford: Blackwell. Pellegrini, A., Bjorklund, D., & Yunger, J. (in press). The evolution of parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.). Handbook of parenting, Volume II: Biology and ecology of parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pellegrini, A. (2002). Perceptions of play and real fighting: Effects of sex and participation status. In J. Roopnarine (Ed.). Play and culture, Vol. 4. New York: Elsevier Science. Pellegrini, A., Smith, P.K., Smees, R., & Menesini, E. (2002). Comparing pupil and teacher perceptions of playful fighting, serious fighting, and positive peer interaction. In J. Roopnarine (Ed.). Play and culture, Vol. 4. New York: Elsevier Science. Pellegrini, A., & Holmes, R. (in press). Children s social behavior during videogames with aggressive and non-aggressive themes. In J. Goldstein and J. Raessens (Eds.) Handbook of computer game studies. London: Cambridge University Press. Pellegrini, A., & J. A. Long (in press). Part of the solution and part of the problem: A dominance theory view of bullying and victimization during the transition from primary school through middle school. In D. Espalage and S. Swearer (Eds.) Bullying in American schools: A social ecological perspective on prevention and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pellegrini, A., & Bjorklund, D.J. (2002). Phylogeny and ontogeny: The emergence of evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association.

early report page 18 Joe Reichle Drager, K., & Reichle, J. (in press). The effect of divided and nondivided attention on the comprehensibility of synthesized speech. Augmentative and Alternative Communication. Drager, K., & Reichle, J. (accepted). Speech intelligibility and comprehensibility in young and elderly persons. Submitted to American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. Freeman, R., Horner, R., & Reichle, J. (2001). Expanding functional assessment procedures to include physiological measurement in relation to environmental stimuli in cases involving self-injurious behavior and self-restraint. In S. Schroeder, M.L. Oster-Granite, & T. Thompson (Eds.), Self injurious behavior: Gene-brain-behavior relationships. Washington, DC: APA Books. Reichle, J. (in press). A review of communication intervention strategies in AAC. To appear in J. Light, D. Beukelman, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Communicative competence for individuals who use AAC: Theory and research. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Reichle, J., Beukelman, D., & Light, J. (Eds.). (2001). Implementing an augmentative communication system: Exemplary strategies for beginning communicators. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Reichle, J., Dettling, E., Drager, K., & Lester, A. (2001). A comparison of correct responses and response latency for fixed and dynamic displays: Performance of a learner with severe developmental disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication. Reichle, J., Drager, K., & Davis, C. (in press). Using requests for assistance to obtain desired items and to gain release from nonpreferred activities: Implications for assessment and intervention. Education and Training of Mentally Retarded. Reichle, &, & Dropik, P. (2001, February). Developing an intervention strategy to replace challenging behavior used to escape undesired activities: A case example. Sig 12 Newsletter. Sigafoos, J., O Reilly, M., Drasgow, E., & Reichle, J. (2001). Strategies to achieve socially acceptable escape and avoidance. In J. Reichle, J. Light, & D. Beukelman (Eds.), Implementing an augmentative communication system: Exemplary strategies for beginning communicators. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

earlyreport page 19 Upcoming Events Minnesota s Early Intervention Institute, 2002 A Collaborative Intensive Graduate Course for Professionals Working with Young Children with Disabilities Date: August 5 7, 2002, St. John s University, Collegeville, MN Topic: Diversity: Challenges & Opportunities in Early Childhood Assessment & Programming Keynote: World Without Walls: Cultural Competency as Skilled Dialogue, Isauara Barrea, Ph.D., Associate Professor, Early Childhood Special Education & Coordinator of Multicultural ECSE Graduate Program, University of New Mexico Session Topics: International Adoption-Post Institutional Medical & Developmental Issues; Cultural Dynamics of Child Development and Childcare; Working with Linguistically Diverse Families; Techniques to Support Collaboration with Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Families; Cultural Competence in Early Childhood Assessment; Anti-Bias Curriculum; Special appearance by Tou Ger Xiong, Diversity consultant, Comedian, Storyteller, and Rap Artist. Registration: For more information about this event or to register, contact Jill Haak at 320-255-9994 or jahreh@cloudnet.com.

CEED-Affiliated Projects Early Childhood Behavior Project Contact: Judy Swanson, (612) 626-9528, swans114@umn.edu http://ici2.umn.edu/preschoolbehavior Early Childhood Research Institute on Measuring Growth and Development (Get It, Got It, Go! and I PROMICE) Contact: Scott McConnell, (612) 624-6365, smcconne@tc.umn.edu http://ici2.umn.edu/ecri/ Early Literacy Training Project Contacts: Kate Horst,horst011@umn.edu; Angele Passe, passe008@umn.edu, (612) 626-8723 http://education.umn.edu/ceed/projects/literacy Minnesota Infant Mental Health Project Contact: Christopher Watson, (612) 625-2898, watso012@tc.umn.edu http://education.umn.edu/ceed/projects/infantmentalhealth Head Start Project Contact: Deborah Ceglowski, (612) 624-2034, deborah.a.ceglowski-1@tc.umn.edu Early Report is published by the Center for Early Education and Development (CEED), University of Minnesota, 207 Pattee Hall, 150 Pillsbury Dr. SE, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0223 Christopher Watson, Editor Christen Opsal, Copy Editor (612) 624-5780 (phone) ceed@icimail.coled.umn.edu (email) http://education.umn.edu/ceed (Web) CEED provides information regarding young children (birth to age eight), including children with special needs, in the areas of education, child care, child development, and family education. CEED activities include research, training, and publications geared toward improving professional practices, supporting parents, and informing policy development. The University of Minnesota is an equal opportunity employer and educator. The College of Education and Human Development is committed to recruiting, enrolling, and educating a diverse population of students who represent the overall composition of our society. This publication is available in alternate formats upon request. Scott McConnell, Director Christopher Watson, Coordinator Judy Bartlett, Executive Assistant Karen Anderson, Administrative Assistant Sophia Herrera, Office Specialist Connie Burkhart, Graphic Design University of Minnesota 207 Pattee Hall 150 Pillsbury Dr. SE Minneapolis, MN 55455-0223 Non Profit Org. U.S. Postage PAID Minneapolis, MN Permit No. 155 Address Service Requested