ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

Similar documents
How To Understand How A Temptation Affects Your Goals

The Dilution Model: How Additional Goals Undermine the Perceived Instrumentality of a Shared Path

Self-Control. A Function of Knowing When and How to Exercise Restraint. Kristian Ove R. Myrseth and Ayelet Fishbach

No theory of consumption can be complete without an

CURRICULUM VITAE CATALINA E. KOPETZ. October 2014

PSYCHOLOGY AND PROJECT MANAGEMENT

How To Know What Motivates Someone To Persist After Failure

Externally imposed controls are common and help individuals

On Nature and Prosocial Behavior. Prosocial behavior inclinations to share, care, cooperate, and assist is critical

Psychodynamic Psychotherapy Deborah L. Cabaniss, M.D.

The Relationship between the Fundamental Attribution Bias, Relationship Quality, and Performance Appraisal

BEPS ACTIONS Revised Guidance on Profit Splits

Prospect Theory Ayelet Gneezy & Nicholas Epley

Running head: THE EFFECTS OF EXTRA-CURRICULAR ACTIVITIES

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function - Adult Version BRIEF-A. Interpretive Report. Developed by

Running head: GOAL PRIMING AND EATING BEHAVIOR. Goal priming and eating behavior: Enhancing self-regulation by environmental cues

The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys

ADAPTATION OF EMPLOYEES IN THE ORGANIZATION AND ITS IMPORTANCE IN TERMS OF HUMAN RESOURCE MANAGEMENT

TOWARDS A STRUCTURAL THEORY OF CREATIVITY IN PRINT ADVERTISING: THE REMOTE ASSOCIATE MATCHING MODEL

National Disability Authority Resource Allocation Feasibility Study Final Report January 2013

Branding and Search Engine Marketing

Developing Fictionally Immoral Attitudes: Spaces of Moral and Narrative Possibilities in Computer Games

In an article titled Ethical Absolutism and the

ONLINE GAMBLING WHEN THE REALITY OF THE VIRTUAL CATCHES UP WITH US

Emotional Quotient. Michael Sample. CEO Sample Co Your Address Here Your Phone Number Here Your Address Here

How to Use Problem-Solving Simulations to Improve Knowledge, Skills, and Teamwork

Simulations, Games and Experiential Learning Techniques:, Volume 1, 1974

Introducing Social Psychology

Stigmatisation of people with mental illness

Self-Awareness as a Therapeutic Tool for Nurse/Client Relationship

Accountability for Others being responsible for the consequences of the actions of those whom you manage.

Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations

AN ELECTRONIC LEARNING ENVIRONMENT FOR APPLIED STATISTICS: QUALITY CARE AND STATISTICS EDUCATION IN HIGHER EDUCATION

Using Self-Efficacy Theory as a guide for instructional practice Wynn Shooter

MODULE 1.3 WHAT IS MENTAL HEALTH?

FUNDAMENTALS OF FAMILY THEORY 3. THE EIGHT INTERLOCKING CONCEPTS

Yael Zemack-Rugar. October, Assistant Professor of Marketing Virginia Tech

THE INFLUENCE OF MOTIVATIONAL COMPONENTS IN CHOOSING THE MASTER S PROGRAM

Mediation Skills in Conflict Resolution

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介

Identifying and Battling Temptation. Ayelet Fishbach. Benjamin A. Converse. University of Chicago

Ayelet Fishbach. University of Chicago

INTERNATIONAL FRAMEWORK FOR ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS CONTENTS

FUNDAMENTALS OF FAMILY THEORY 9. SIBLING POSITION

American Journal Of Business Education July/August 2012 Volume 5, Number 4

April Online Payday Loan Payments

The Wisconsin Comprehensive School Counseling Model Student Content Standards. Student Content Standards

University of Cincinnati Cognitive Behavioral Treatment for Substance Abuse Curriculum

Influenced by - Alfred Binet intelligence testing movement

In an experimental study there are two types of variables: Independent variable (I will abbreviate this as the IV)

Guide 7 Iceberg beliefs underlying beliefs that can undermine our resilience

Re-energizing the Practice of Leadership for the Public Good Public Sector Core Leadership Practices Paradoxes of Leadership

Examining Motivation Theory in Higher Education: An Expectancy Theory Analysis of Tenured Faculty Productivity

Brand Loyalty in Insurance Companies

Change. Each of these themes will be discussed below. Employees

IQ Testing: A critique for parents of children with developmental disabilities

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) in The Workplace. Dr. Paul Flaxman

IFAD S ORIGINAL APPROACH TO KNOWLEDGE MANAGEMENT: AN EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING SYSTEM ON RURAL POVERTY

Behavioral Interventions Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior

Test-Retest Reliability and The Birkman Method Frank R. Larkey & Jennifer L. Knight, 2002

Melissa Gutworth's Search for a PhD and the IAT

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

Drive By Daniel H. Pink

Formatting data files for repeated measures analyses in SPSS: Using the Aggregate and Restructure procedures

1/9. Locke 1: Critique of Innate Ideas

A Shot in the Arm for Medicare Part D: Four Ways for the Government to Boost its Customer Communications

Minnesota Co-occurring Mental Health & Substance Disorders Competencies:

Self-directed learning: managing yourself and your working relationships

Sponsor-CRO Collaboration Study. Executive Summary

Organizational Change and Anxiety: Developing an Instrument for the 5Rs Model

Does unconscious thought outperform conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination

Counteractive Self-Control in Overcoming Temptation

HOW DO EMOTIONS INFLUENCE SAVING BEHAVIOR?

Interview Questions. Accountability. Adaptability

KNOWLEDGE REVIEW 13 SUMMARY. Outcomes-focused services for older people: A summary

Code of Professional Conduct & Ethics Association of Accounting Technicians of Sri Lanka

Taking Hold of Your Mind: What Skills:

Is Willpower a Limited Resource?

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing

Identifying Family and Relationship Theories in Family Life Education Materials

STEP 1: IDENTIFY A BEHAVIOR TO CHANGE

The Action Learning Toolkit

Emotional Intelligence Style Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

The Evaluative Criteria of Industrial Buyers: Implications for Sales Training,

Target marketing and the product: categorizing products to understand the resulting marketing communication outcome measures

Appendix A: Science Practices for AP Physics 1 and 2

COLLEGE OF EDUCATION TECHNICAL STANDARDS/ FOUNDATIONAL COMPETENCIES POLICY 1 (May 2004; revised August 2005; title revised August 2007)

Egon Zehnder International. The Leading Edge of Diversity and Inclusion. 11th International Executive Panel October 2012

Definition. Hyperbolic discounting refers to the tendency for people to increasingly choose a

Why do people publish weblogs? An online survey of weblog authors in Japan

BILINGUALISM AND LANGUAGE ATTITUDES IN NORTHERN SAMI SPEECH COMMUNITIES IN FINLAND PhD thesis Summary

What is Customer Experience Management?

Learner-Valued Interactions: Research into Practice

Measurement: Reliability and Validity

Memory for pictures and words as a function of level of processing: Depth or dual coding?

Success/Failure Feedback, Expectancies, and Approach/Avoidance Motivation: How Regulatory Focus Moderates Classic Relations

TIPS FOR SUPERVISORS

Chapter 13 & 14 Quiz. Name: Date:

Reversed Facial Images and the Mere-Exposure Hypothesis

Transcription:

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Special Session Summary the Application of Goal Systems Theory to Consumer Behavior [to cite]: Andrew Mitchell and Meng Zhang (2005),"Special Session Summary the Application of Goal Systems Theory to Consumer Behavior", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 32, eds. Geeta Menon and Akshay R. Rao, Duluth, MN : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 17-20. [url]: http://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/9015/volumes/v32/na-32 [copyright notice]: This work is copyrighted by The Association for Consumer Research. For permission to copy or use this work in whole or in part, please contact the Copyright Clearance Center at http://www.copyright.com/.

SPECIAL SESSION SUMMARY The Application of Goal Systems Theory to Consumer Behavior SESSION OVERVIEW Goals are becoming an increasingly important concept in consumer behavior. Goals have been found to influence what information we attend to (Huffman and Huston 1993), what attribute we used to make a decision (Garbarino and Johnson 2001) and what product category is considered (Ratneshwar et al. 2001). Previous research on goals, however, has tended to treat and manipulate goals on an individual basis, instead of considering them as a system of goals and the means for achieving the goal. Recently, such a system has been proposed by Kruglanski and colleagues (2002). Within this system, goals are viewed as cognitive structures and are associated so the activation of one goal may either activate or inhibit another goal. In addition, the means for achieving each goal are associated with the goal. Consequently, the activation of a goal also activates the means for achieving the goal. This theory provides a rich framework for examining effect of goals on consumer behavior. The purpose of this session is to introduce the Theory of Goal Systems to consumer behavior researchers and demonstrate its value in examining important consumer behavior issues. In the first paper, Kruglanski and colleagues present the theory. The principles of multifinality and equifinality are illustrated as well as the transfer of affect from goal achievement to the means. Research conducted within the goal systemic framework has found, among others, that 1) goals may pull attentional resources from one another, 2) individuals shield their goal commitment by inhibiting competing goals, and 3) means can activate their associated goals in a bottom up fashion. The implications of this framework for consumer psychology are discussed. In the second paper, Fishbach uses the theory to understand successful self-control mechanisms. The results of a series of studies indicate that successful self-control occurs when momentary goals (i.e. temptations) activate overriding goals (e.g. when chocolate cake activates the goal of dieting). This activation pattern is asymmetrical: whereas temptations bring to mind the opposing goals, goals inhibit interfering temptations. In the third paper, Zhang and Mitchell, discuss their current research which applies and extends aspects of the theory to consumer behavior. More specifically, they examine whether brands are associated with consumer goals as the attainment means. The results of two studies find that the priming of both high-level and low-level goals automatically activate brands which are used to achieve those goals. Moreover, the affect felt with respect to the achievement of a consumer goal is transferred to the associated brands. By linking brands to the consumer goal system, a new perspective on brand choice and consumer brand relationship is provided. A Theory of Goal Systems: Properties and Applications Arie W. Kruglanski, University of Maryland James Y. Shah, University of Wisconsin Madison Ayelet Fishbach, University of Chicago Woo Young Chun, University of Maryland David Sleeth-Keppler, University of Maryland The present paper introduces the theory of goal systems and discusses its implication for motivational phenomena and consumer psychology. The theory of goals systems adopts a cognitive approach to motivation that treats goal-concepts as knowledgestructures governed by general cognitive principles. In addition, goal-concepts possess distinctly motivational contents that determine their manner of functioning. From that perspective, (1) goal systems constitute stored mental representations capable of being learned, altered or activated; (2) self-regulatory phenomena are jointly governed by cognitive principles that goal systems share in common with other cognitive systems, and motivational principles derived from the uniquely motivational contents of goal-systemic elements. Major cognitive properties of goal systems derive from their interconnected nature. Interconnections have two essential aspects, namely: (1) strength of the links between goal-systemic elements, and (2) their form, that is, configurations these links create. Because goal systemic elements are motivational in content (constituting goals and means ) they possess several motivational properties as well: (1) Jointly, goal value and expectancy of attainment determine a goal s subjective utility driving goal-commitment. (2) Sustained motivation contributes to persistence at an activity and to the quality of its performance. Goal activation contributes to the sustenance of motivation and hence it should contribute to persistence and performance. (3) Goal attainment and nonattainment are typically accompanied by positive and negative affect respectively, which specific quality varies with the type of goal (e.g. of promotion or prevention); (4) Individuals choices strive to maximize total outcomes, that is, to satisfy a maximal number of goals with a minimal expenditure of resources. Prior research within the goal systemic framework has shown that goals are cognitively activated by their correspondent means and means are cognitively activated by their correspondent goals. We found that such activation patterns: (1) arise in course of an active pursuit of goals via particular means, hence cannot be fully explained by mere semantic associations between goal and means terms in the general language, (2) have unique consequences for goal commitment, task performance and activity experience. We found that the strength with which a goal activates a means contributes to goal commitment by enhancing the expectancy of goal attainment. Furthermore, the degree to which a means cognitively activates a goal enhances task persistence and performance by focusing the actor s attention on the goal at hand. We also found evidence for emotional transfer between goals and means whereby the specific affect typically attendant upon goal attainment (or nonattainment) imbues the way activities leading to that goal (i.e., the means) are experienced. In addition to looking at relations between goals and means, goal systemic research also has examined the relations between goals and other, incompatible goals. We found that goals may pull attentional resources away from one another, and in order to shield highly committed goals from such a pull individuals may inhibit competing goal alternatives. To maintain effective self regulation some individuals may also learn to (1) activate high order goals upon encountering temptation goals that threaten to undermine progress to the former, high order, objectives, and (2) inhibit low order temptation goals (e.g. tasty but fattening foods) upon activation of a high order objectives (e.g. dieting). 17 Advances in Consumer Research Volume 32, 2005

18 / The Application of Goal Systems Theory to Consumer Behavior Goal systemic research also addressed the form of goalsystemic interconnections, in addition to looking at their strength and function (facilitative, inhibitory). Specifically, the number of means to a given goal may vary as may the number of goals served by a given means. The set of means linked to a given goal defines the configuration of equifinality. The set of goals linked to a single means define the configuration of multifinality. Equifinality poses the problem of choice between the means, assumed to be often resolved via the multifinality principle, i.e. the choice of means that the focal goal satisfies, often outside of conscious awareness in addition to other, background, goals. We found evidence for the operation of the multifinality principle in unconscious choice phenomena involving selection among different products. We also found that the pursuit of multifinality is greater for individuals under high need for cognitive closure (Kruglanski, 1989; with Webster, 1996; in press). The pursuit of multifinality by high need for closure individuals has been demonstrated in a variety of product domains as well as in the tendency to prefer multifinal over unifinal friends. An important phenomenon related to the notion of equifinality is that of substitutability of one means for another in the case of failure or thwarting of progress to the goal via a given means. In accordance with the goal systemic analysis we find that substitutability is a matter of cognitive framing. Thus, the same two activities may be treated as mutually substitutable in one context, where their common goal is activated, and as nonsubstitutable in another context wherein their noncommon goals are activated. Furthermore, different individuals may vary in their general tendency to view the means-goal structure as equifinal. For example, individuals with a promotion focus (Higgins, 1997) tend to view means as more substitutable than individuals with a prevention focus who tend to view means as necessary (rather than as sufficient) to goal attainment. One implication of this is that individuals with a promotion focus may mistake promises for goal attainments, and hence be less likely to act after making a promise than the case would be in the absence of a promise. Evidence for these notions will be discussed, and the implications for consumer issues will be drawn. Temptations Elicit Overriding Goal Activations Ayelet Fishbach, University of Chicago A delicious chocolate cake in the storefront of a bakery may remind individuals of the unfortunate fact that they should go on a diet. A thought of an exotic place, ideal for a relaxing vacation, may conjure up approaching deadlines at work. On these and similar occasions, elaborating upon a seemingly desirable course of action may bring to mind the opposing, higher priority objective. The present research explored the nature of automatic associations formed between short-term motives (temptations) and the overriding goals with which they interfere. Five experimental studies, encompassing several self-regulatory domains, found that temptations tend to activate such higher priority goals, whereas the latter tend to inhibit the temptations. These activation patterns occurred outside of participants conscious awareness and did not appear to tax their mental resources. Moreover, they varied as function of subjective goal importance and were more pronounced for successful versus unsuccessful self-regulators in a given domain. Finally, priming by temptation stimuli was found to influence not only the activation of overriding goals, but to also affect goal-congruent behavioral choices. More specifically, Study 1 assessed the strength of temptationgoal associations through the use of a subliminal sequential priming procedure. Using participants self-generated goal-temptation pairs (e.g. study-basketball, faithful-sex ), we assessed the time for recognizing goals preceded by subliminal temptations and the time for recognizing temptations preceded by subliminal goals. We then found that when the target was a goal, participants were faster to recognize it after having been primed with a temptation rather than a neutral prime. However, when the target was a temptation, participants were slower to recognize it after having been primed with the goal rather than a neutral prime. The second study was designed to further test for the automaticity of temptation-elicited goal activation by replicating this effect under cognitive load. Once again, goal-temptation associations were assessed through a subliminal sequential priming procedure. We measured time for recognizing religious goals (e.g., prayer and bible ) following subliminal sinful temptation primes (e.g., drugs or sex ), and time for recognizing sinful temptations following subliminal religious goal primes. Half of the participants performed the task under cognitive load. In line with our prediction, the same pattern emerged in the presence and in the absence of cognitive load: Participants were faster to recognize the goal following a temptation prime than following a neutral prime. However, they were slower to recognize the temptation after a goal prime than after a neutral prime. Assuming that goal activation by temptation indeed represents a functional means of self-control, it should be associated with successful, rather than unsuccessful, goal attainment. Accordingly, with respect to the academic goal of excelling at one s coursework, Study 3 found that only successful students activate academic goals in the presence of cues for procrastination. This pattern was not manifested among less successful students who are presumably less efficient at shielding themselves against procrastination cues. In addition to self-regulatory success, goal activation by temptations further depends on the subjective value of the goal. That is, goal activation should occur only with regard to highly valued goals. Accordingly, in Study 4 we tested for the combined influence of variations in subjective goal value and success in goal pursuit on temptation-elicited goal activation. Specifically, with respect to the goal of weight-watching, we expected that activation of goals by temptations would be more pronounced for successful versus unsuccessful weight watchers only to the extent that they perceived weight watching to be a particularly important personal objective. In line with our predictions, increased accessibility of dieting-related concepts following food primes was found only with regards to successful dieters who expressed concerns with watching their weight. Our last study was designed to test for the behavioral consequences of goal priming. It was hypothesized that when fattening food is served, restricted eaters should activate the goal of dieting, which in turn should enable them to avoid the forbidden culinary delights. Due to the activation of self-control processes, temptation cues are also expected to prevent succumbing to temptations as much or more than direct goal-related cues. Accordingly, participants, all concerned about their weight, were exposed to either diet primes, fattening food primes, or control primes. Next, we assessed (1) activation of diet in a lexical decision task, (2) intentions to consume fattening food, and (3) choice of a gift: healthy apple vs. fattening Twix bar. In line with our predictions, participants in the food-prime condition were just as quick to recognize the word diet as individuals in the diet-prime condition and both conditions were faster than the control-prime. In addition, activating the goal made participants more conscious about their weight, consequently leading to more goal-congruent behavioral choices (i.e., of apple versus Twix-bar) in the food-prime and diet-prime conditions compared with the control condition. Finally, only participants in the food-prime condition showed diminished intentions to consume fattening food. Apparently, only fattening food primes bol-

Advances in Consumer Research (Volume 32) / 19 stered self-control attempts as such, motivating the formation of intentions to overcome specific temptations in the future. In sum, this research identifies facilitative links between temptation stimuli and the higher priority goals they threaten to undermine. These findings suggest that to the extent that they represent low priority temptations, external cues may set in motion self-control mechanisms designed to counteract their own influence, thus wresting behavioral control from the unconcerned environment and relegating it to one s own, internal priorities. Brands as Means in Consumer Goal Systems The theory of goal systems is applied to consumer behavior by examining whether brands are associated with both high-level (abstract and value-embedded) and low-level (specific and actionoriented) consumer goals as goal attainment means. Two experiments were conducted. The first used sequential priming in a lexical decision task and found that the priming of a goal automatically activate its associated brands. The second study investigated a unique feature of a consumer goal system, which is, the affect felt whose goal has been attained is transferred to the associated brands. Study 1: Participants were first asked to list two high-level goals (or two low-level goals) that came to mind. The typical high-level goals they listed included be a successful businessman and be intelligent etc., whereas the typical low-level goals they listed included cooling off and planning a vacation etc. They were then asked to list two brands that could help them attain each of these goals. For example, brands listed for the above-mentioned goals included: Holt Renfrew and IBM ; Nestle Ice Cream and Air Canada. The participants then completed a 20-minute filler task to eliminate the temporarily enhanced accessibility of goals and brands. They were then subliminally primed with either a goal or a control word (e.g. tree) and asked to judge whether a target letter string was a word or not. The target letter strings included brands they listed, unrelated brands, control words (e.g. house, plant) and an equal number of nonwords. After completing this task, participants were asked to think of each brand as if it was a person, and rated how similar the person was to them as well as how they aspired to be like that person. The purpose of this question was to better understand the personalities of brands activated by each goal. The results were as expected. Participants were significantly faster to respond to a brand after being primed with the goal associated with it. This indicated that brands were closely linked to goals and the presentation of a goal could automatically activate brands which were means to attain it. Study 1 also demonstrated a difference between the personalities of brands associated with high-level goals vs. low-level goals. Consistent with findings of the branding literature that consumers are more likely to consider brands with a personality compatible with their own (i.e. current self, working self) or they aspire to be like (i.e. desired self) (e.g. Marcus 1986), participants were more likely to rate the brand as similar to them, or as whom they would like to become when brands were associated with a high-level goal. However, when a low-level goal was activated, participants neither regarded the brand as similar to self nor would they aspire to be like it. The brand was simply a means to achieve a temporarily activated goal. This result echoed Aaker s (1999) distinction between selfcongruity and situation-congruity on brand attitudes, however, by linking brands to goals, we are able to move a step further by demonstrating when self-congruity and situation-congruity attitudes occur. When a high-level goal is activated, consumers will prefer a brand with a personality compatible with the self, whereas when a low-level goal is activated, consumers may not care much about whether the brand is compatible with self or not, instead, they proffer brands with characteristics compatible with the requirement of the temporary situation. Study 2: Study 1 demonstrated that goals could activate brands. Study 2, however, went a step further to demonstrate a unique feature of a consumer goal system, that is, the affect experienced by consumers when they attain a goal is transferred to associated brands. This transfer of affect is in proportion to the association strength between the goal and the brands. Participants were asked to list two high-level goals (or lowlevel goals) that came to mind and two brands that could help achieve each goal. After a questionnaire measuring the association strength, participants were asked to imagine that they had attained the high-level goals (or low-level goals) and report the affect they would experience. Both promotion-type (happy) and preventiontype (relax) were reported. They were also asked to indicate their promotion (happy) and prevention (relax) affect towards each of the brand. A series of multiple regressions indicated that participants affect towards a brand was a function of 1) their affect felt towards the attainment of the goal (both the magnitude-how strong the affect is and the type promotion or prevention affect), and 2) the association strength between the goal and the brand. In other words, the affect towards a consumer goal was being transferred to the related brands, in proportion to their association strength. These results were consistent with the prediction of goal system theory (Kruglanski et al. 2002) and replicated previous research on affect transfer (Fishbach et al. 2004). The results also found that different types of affect are transferred with respect to high-level goals and low-level goals. Specifically, affect associated with the attainment of promotion goals (happy) transfers from a high-level goal to its associated brands, whereas affect associated with the attainment of prevention goals (relax) transfers from a low-level goal to its associated brands. This finding is consistent with the recent research (Zhang and Mitchell 2004) showing a relationship between high-level goals and a promotion focus and a relationship between low-level goals and a prevention focus. Taking together, these two studies demonstrated that brands can be associated with consumer goals as attainment means, and both the activation and affect can transport from goals to the related brands. These intriguing results, together with the differences between high-level and low-level goals, provide substantial insight into many areas of consumer behavior, such as brand choice and consumer brand relationship. REFERENCES Aaker, Jennifer L. (1999), The Malleable Self: The Role of Self-Expression in Persuasion, Journal of Marketing Research, 36, 1 (Feburary), 45-57 Fishbach, Ayelet, James Y. Shah and Arie W. Kruglanski, (2004), Emotional Transfer in Goal Systems, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 723-738. Garbarino, Ellen and Mark S. Johson. (2001), Effects of Consumer Goals on Attribute Weighting, Overall Satisfaction, and Product Usage, Psychology & Marketing, 18(9):929-949 Higgins, E. Tory (1997), Self-Discrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect, Psychological Review, 94, 319-340

20 / The Application of Goal Systems Theory to Consumer Behavior Huffman, Cynthia and Michael J. Houston (1993), Goaloriented Experiences and the Development of Knowledge, Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (September), 190-207 Kruglanski, Arie W (2004), The Psychology of Closed Mindedness, New York: Psychology Press Kruglanski, Arie W., James Y.Shah, Ayelet Fishbach, Ron Friedman, Woo Young Chun and David Sleeth-Keppler (2002), A Theory of Goal System, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 331-377 Kruglanski, Arie W (1989), Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Base, New York: Plenum Kruglanski, Arie W and Donna M.Webster (1996), Motivated Closing of the Mind: Seizing and Freezing, Psychological Review, 103 (2). 263-283 Markus, Hazel and Ziva Kunda (1986), Stability and Malleability of the Self Concept, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (4), 858-66 Ratneshwar, S., Lawrence W. Barsalou, Cornelia Pechmann and Melissa Moore (2001), Goal-Derived Categories: The Role of Personal and Situational Goals in Category Representations, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 10 (3), 147-157 Zhang, Meng and Andrew Mitchell, (2004), Resolving Goal Conflicts: The Effect of Regulatory Focus on Product Choice, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 31, Mary Frances Luce and Barbara Kahn, eds., Valdosta, GA: Association for Consumer Research.