Free Voluntary reading: New Research, Applications, and Controversies. Stephen Krashen Paper presented at the RELC conference, Singapore, April, 2004



Similar documents
Applying the Comprehension Hypothesis: Some Suggestions

Dealing with English Fever

Can Second Language Acquirers Reach High Levels of Proficiency Through Self-Selected Reading? An Attempt to Confirm Nation's (2014) Results

Does Intensive Decoding Instruction Contribute to Reading Comprehension? Stephen Krashen Knowledge Quest (in press)

Making Reading Content Comprehensible for Intermediate Language Learners. Colin Dalton. University of Houston-Downtown, United States

CHALLENGES OF NON-NATIVE SPEAKERS WITH READING AND WRITING IN COMPOSITION 101 CLASSES. Abstract

Why We Should Stop Scolding Teenagers and Their Schools: Frequency of Leisure Reading Stephen Krashen

Top Ten Principles for Teaching Extensive Reading 1

Enhancing Technology College Students English Listening Comprehension by Listening Journals

Competence in Foreign Language Writing: Progress and Lacunae

The "Decline" of Reading in America, Poverty and Access to Books, and the use of Comics in Encouraging Reading

The Comprehension Hypothesis

Using Direct Instruction Programs as Intervention Programs in Grades K 3

Difficulties that Arab Students Face in Learning English and the Importance of the Writing Skill Acquisition Key Words:

9 The Difficulties Of Secondary Students In Written English

Parent Education Activities

Using Interest Inventories with Struggling and Unmotivated Readers. By Arleen P. Mariotti

Language Arts Literacy Areas of Focus: Grade 6

Culture and Second Language Acquisition. Christopher L. Spackman. Ohio Dominican University

AUDIOBOOKS & LITERACY

Discourse Markers in English Writing

Getting Kids Reading in Mandarin: The Comic Book Connection by Elizabeth Weise

The Facilitating Role of L1 in ESL Classes

This document has been produced to support the development of effective questioning and dialogue between teacher and pupils.

THE USE OF AUDIOBOOKS IN EFL CLASSES TO IMPROVE READING AND LISTENING SKILLS

CORRECTING AND GIVING FEEDBACK TO WRITING

COMMUNICATION COMMUNITIES CULTURES COMPARISONS CONNECTIONS. STANDARDS FOR FOREIGN LANGUAGE LEARNING Preparing for the 21st Century

Protecting Students Against the Effects of Poverty: Libraries Stephen Krashen New England Reading Association Journal (in press)

A Pilot Study of Some ROCMA Cadets Difficulties in English Speaking

NEW DEVELOPMENTS IN TEACHING READING COMPREHENSION SKILLS TO EFL LEARNERS

Promoting Learner Autonomy and Language Awareness Through Blogging

Research evidence on reading for pleasure. Education standards research team

Literature and Language Learning

INTRODUCING LANGUAGE TEACHER COGNITION

Language Arts Literacy Areas of Focus: Grade 5

COMR 260/ENG 560 page two

FOREIGN LANGUAGE TEACHING AN INTERVIEW WITH NINA SPADA

240Tutoring Reading Comprehension Study Material

Compiled By: Pat Elliott, Resource Teacher & Dale Mays, Grade 4 Teacher Simcoe County District School Board Ontario Canada

Integrating Reading and Writing for Effective Language Teaching

APEC Online Consumer Checklist for English Language Programs

An Analysis of the Eleventh Grade Students Monitor Use in Speaking Performance based on Krashen s (1982) Monitor Hypothesis at SMAN 4 Jember

CALIFORNIA S TEACHING PERFORMANCE EXPECTATIONS (TPE)

Guided Reading: Constructivism in Action. Donna Kester Phillips, Niagara University. Abstract

parent ROADMAP SUPPORTING YOUR CHILD IN GRADE FIVE ENGLISH LANGUAGE ARTS

Fiction. VS. Informational Texts. Which Will Kindergartners Choose?

Key Components of Literacy Instruction

The Impact of Extensive Reading on College Business Majors in Taiwan

Second Language Acquisition Stages Stephen Krashen (1986) Silent and Receptive Stage

COMPUTER TECHNOLOGY IN TEACHING READING

2 P age.

REGULATIONS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS IN TEACHING ENGLISH TO SPEAKERS OF OTHER LANGUAGES (MA[TESOL])

Literacy Institute August 2013 Jessica Plemons

Foreign Language (FL)

How do the principles of adult learning apply to English language learners?

Modern foreign languages

Nevis Public School District #308. District Literacy Plan Minnesota Statute 120B.12, Learning together... Achieving quality together.

Bibliografía. Baker, C. (1992). Attitudes and language. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

Has Whole Language Failed? Stephen Krashen

An Overview of Applied Linguistics

The International Reading Association calls for an immediate

Research Rationale for the Criterion SM Online Writing Evaluation Service

Field Observation Reflection Paper. Kelli Jordan. Manchester College. EDUC 111: Introduction to Teaching

How to become a successful language learner

Language Skills in a Multilingual Society Syed Mohamed Singapore Examinations and Assessment Board

Growing Strong Nonfiction Readers and Writers What Matters Most in and out of Class Presenter: Mary Ehrenworth

RESEARCH METHODS IN PSYCHOLOGY PSYC 381 FALL 2013 T & TH 12:10 p.m. to 1:25 p.m. (GRPS 1) SATURDAY 9/14 & 10/19 (8:00 a.m. to 12:00 noon Tech 101)

English Syllabus for Grades 1-4. Desktop/ Files Returned by Experts August 2008 / English cover, content & introduction Grades 1-4 cv2

Book Review of Rosenhouse, The Monty Hall Problem. Leslie Burkholder 1

Study Plan for Master of Arts in Applied Linguistics

TEACHING CREDENTIAL CANDIDATES HOW TO ADAPT LESSONS AND INCREASE VOCABULARY FOR ENGLISH LEARNERS. Abstract

FAQ about Reading Workshop

English. Aim of the subject

Fun for all the Family 3- Quite a few games for articles and determiners

NFL Quarterback Bernie Kosar told

BUILDING A READING WEBSITE FOR EFL/ESL LEARNERS

BEST PRACTICE IN ENGLISH LANGUAGE ARTS *

Adventure Games as Deaf Education Tools: Action Research Results. Scott Whitney Stephen F. Austin State University. Gabriel A. Martin Lamar University

Sueyon Seo 4282 N Wilson Drive #4 Shorewood WI /sseo@uwm.edu

Balanced Literacy in Seattle Public Schools

The Reading Practices and Study Behaviors of Developmental and Nontraditional Community College Students. M Cecil Smith. Robert J.

Foundations of the Montessori Method (3 credits)

Guided Reading with I HAD A HIPPOPOTAMUS written and illustrated by Hector Viveros Lee

Modern Foreign Languages (MFL) Policy 2013

Research on Graphic Organizers

A Comparative Study of Reading Strategies among College Students

Technology Use and Primary Music Education: Examining. Teacher Thinking And Practice

ELL Considerations for Common Core-Aligned Tasks in English Language Arts

How To Find Out If You Can Be Successful In A Career In Physical Education

Abstract. The checklist in this article provides educators with a valuable tool for evaluating reading

Recommended Course Sequence MAJOR LEADING TO PK-4. First Semester. Second Semester. Third Semester. Fourth Semester. 124 Credits

The following courses are required for all seventh grade students.

A reflective teaching practice experience: a case study

Adopting CALL to Promote Listening Skills for EFL Learners in Vietnamese Universities

The (Lack of ) Experimental Evidence Supporting the Use of Accelerated Reader

FAMILY LIFE EDUCATION ACPS Fifth Grade

Read books with appealing pictures that match their age and interests. They like books with rhythm, rhyme, and repetition.

Teaching Math to English Language Learners

Writing Topics WRITING TOPICS

Transcription:

Free Voluntary reading: New Research, Applications, and Controversies Stephen Krashen Paper presented at the RELC conference, Singapore, April, 2004 Evidence for the Power of Reading Evidence for the value of free voluntary reading, or recreational reading, continues to accumulate. In the last few decades, evidence from several areas continues to show that those who do more recreational reading show better development in reading, writing, grammar and vocabulary, These results hold for first and second language acquisition, and for children and adults. Correlational studies Correlational studies have consistently shown that those who read more show more literacy development. I reviewed a number of these studies in detail in Krashen (1988) that relied on simple correlations. The results of such studies are reassuring and consistent with the view that reading results in language and literacy development, but of course correlation is not causality; it is quite possible that those who read better, as a result of more direct instruction in school, then go on to do more recreational reading. More recent studies in second language acquisition make this interpretation less plausible: They consistently report a positive relationship between the amount of free reading done and various aspects of second and foreign language competence when the amount of formal instruction students had is statistically controlled (Y.O. Lee, Krashen, and Gribbons, 1996; Stokes, Krashen and Kartchner, 1998; Constantino, S.Y. Lee, K.S. Cho and Krashen, 1997; S. Y. Lee, 2001). Case histories Case histories provide convincing verification of the power of reading. While not considered by some to be "scientific," they clearly are, because in many cases one can only attribute gains in literacy and language development to recreational reading; there are no plausible confounds or alternative explanations for the obvious development that took place. In Krashen (1993) I described the cases of Malcolm X and Richard Wright, both of whom achieved very high levels of literacy, and both of whom attributed their literacy development to self-selected reading.

More recent studies include the Sweet Valley studies (Cho and Krashen, 1994, 1995a, 1995b): Adult second language acquirers made obvious and impressive progress in English as a second language simply by reading books from the Sweet Valley series, novels written for young girls (Sweet Valley Kids, Sweet Valley Twins) and teen-age girls (Sweet Valley High). Subjects did not attend ESL classes; their main source of English was the novels. "Maturation" and time in the US is also an unlikely factor here: All subjects had lived in the US for a considerable amount of time before starting their reading program, and had made little progress in English. For other cases in which recreational reading was the only plausible explanation for progress, see Krashen (2003). In-school free reading Studies of in-school free reading are considered the gold standard for demonstrating the effectiveness of recreational reading, because they include a comparison group that engages in traditional instruction while the experimental group does free voluntary reading. There are slightly different models of in- school free reading (sustained silent reading, self-selected reading, extensive reading) but they all have this in common: Students can read whatever they want to read (within reason) and there is little or no accountability in the form of book reports or grades. In my reviews of the research on in-school free reading (Krashen, 1993; 2001), I have concluded that with very few exceptions, students in these programs progress in reading at least as well as those in comparison groups, and often do considerably better. The most successful studies are those that last for longer than one academic year. Short-term studies produce less than spectacular results, most likely because it usually takes readers some time to settle in and find suitable reading material. The Comprehension Hypothesis Not only do we see triangulation among three different sources of evidence, all supporting the effectiveness of free reading, the Reading Hypothesis is also consistent with the more general Comprehension Hypothesis, the hypothesis that we acquire language by understanding it. The Comprehension Hypothesis finds support in several areas in second language acquisition, including beginning language teaching and intermediate language teaching: Students make better progress in classes that supply more "comprehensible input." I have also argued (Krashen, 1994, 2003) that other means of acquiring language are unlikely and without empirical support. Goodman (Furkey and Xu, 2003) and Smith (1994) have presented strong evidence that comprehension is the means by which we learn to read, providing evidence from sources very different from those cited here.

Applications Sustained silent reading Perhaps the most common application of the Reading Hypothesis in schools is the practice of sustained silent reading, or SSR. In SSR, a few minutes each day is devoted to recreational reading, usually between five and 15. As noted above, the research on SSR and related programs is quite positive. Pilgreen (2000) presents the principles of SSR and some helpful guidelines. I focus here on just a few aspects of SSR. Do they really read during SSR? Some critics have claimed that SSR is merely a time for "pretend" reading, that many students simply flip pages and look at pictures. Von Sprecken and Krashen (1998) observed SSR classes in a middle school in the middle of the school year and reported that 90 percent of the students were engaged in reading. Their observations were confirmed in subsequent studies (Cohen, 1999; Herda and Ramos, 2001). Von Sprecken and Krashen also reported that more reading tended to take place in those classes in which more books were available, in which teachers read at the same time students read (see also Wheldall and Entwhistle, 1988), in which students were not required to bring their own books, and in which teachers made deliberate attempts to promote certain books. From observing SSR classes and from talking to numerous professionals, I have concluded that is more effective to do a little each day than to devote large segments of time once or twice a week to free reading. The goal of SSR is to develop a taste for reading, to stimulate the once-reluctant reader to read more outside of school. Rather than forcing reading, and possibly making it distasteful, small doses are much more likely to work. It is not the actual time reading during SSR that counts, it is the desire to read more than counts. Thus SSR is not for very advanced readers. It is doubtful that readers of this paper will benefit by adding five to 15 minutes of free reading to their daily schedule. We have already developed a reading habit. Studies confirm that those who participate in SSR programs read more on their own when the program is over (Pilgreen and Krashen, 1993) and one study showed that they continue to read more even years later (Greaney and Clarke, 1973). Self-selected reading

Self-selected reading programs were popular in the United States in the 1950 s. In self-selected reading, the entire class period is devoted to recreational reading, except for a small amount of time devoted to teacher-student conferences in which teachers discuss what the child read, any problems that may have come up, and recommend additional reading. Sheltered popular literature Sheltered popular literature exists only in my imagination. It is a suggestion for combining the advantages of literature teaching with the advantages of free voluntary reading and attempts to solve a problem many second language readers have: A lack of familiarity with what is available. Sheltered popular literature is, first of all, a literature class with the same goals as any other literature class: The use of literature as a means of understanding the human condition, gaining insight into ethics, metaphysics, and culture. In sheltered popular literature, however, the focus is not on the "classics." Rather, the focus is on literature that second language students will find interesting and comprehensible. Depending on the age and background of the students, this could mean comic books, magazines, romance novels, mysteries, and newspapers. This "literature" would be discussed in class with the same seriousness we devote to the classics. Our hope is that such a class will provide the same kind of cognitive development and reflective attitude that the study of classical literature provides. At the same time, it will inform the intermediate second language acquirer what reading material is available, and will, we hope, lead to a reading habit in the second language. Narrow reading An interesting hypothesis is that narrow, rather than broad or wide reading, is more efficient for second language acquisition. This means the work of one author, one genre or topic (e.g. only detective novels). (I focus here on reading, but the idea of narrow input has been applied to listening as well; see e.g. Krashen, 1996; Rodrigo and Krashen, 1996; Dupuy, 1999). Narrow reading will be more interesting, by definition, because it is restricted to what the reader really wants to read. It will be more comprehensible, because the reader will already have a great deal of background knowledge, and will gain more background knowledge by reading. Deep reading in any topic, it is hypothesized, will provide exposure to a tremendous amount of syntax and vocabulary that is used in other topics. Any technical field, for example, will use "subtechnical" vocabulary, words such as "function," "inference," "isolate," "relation," etc. (Cowan, 1974).

There is some evidence supporting the narrow reading idea. Lamme (1976) found that good readers in English as a first language tended to read more books by a single author and books from a series, a result that many readers of this paper can identify with, former devotees of Nancy Drew, the Hardy Boys and Bobsey Twins. In addition, the Sweet Valley studies mentioned above are also an example of the efficacy of narrow reading. Of great interest is the fact that Sweet Valley readers eventually went on to read other authors. Sheltered popular literature introduces the second language reader to a range of interesting reading, in the hope that the reader will settle on one or two areas for narrow reading. Some Controversies Is light reading enough? It can be argued that light reading is not enough, that in order to master the academic language one needs to read truly challenging texts. This is, of course, true. My claim is that light reading is a bridge to heavier reading, that it supplies the competence necessary to understand challenging texts. Our usual practice in foreign language education is to force students to jump from elementary level courses where they read only simplified texts to very demanding texts. Light reading is the missing link, a conduit to heavier reading. There has been no research in this area in second language acquisition, but it has been shown that developing (high school) readers in English as a first language typically do not read only one author or in one area for the rest of their lives and do not stick with only light reading; they gradually expand their reading interests as they read more (LaBrant, 1958). Does supplementation help? What kinds of supplementation, if any, are necessary or even helpful in in- school reading programs? The most popularly used forms of supplementation are additional writing and grammar study. I have presented evidence showing that increasing output (speaking or writing) does not result in additional language or literacy development, whether it be in the form of "comprehensible output" or not (Krashen, 1994, 2003; see also S.Y. Lee, 2001). In addition, I remain convinced (in fact I am more convinced than ever) that formal grammar study makes only a peripheral and fragile contribution to competence in a second language; a close look

at the parade of studies claiming to show that "grammar works" only confirms the lack of impact of grammar study (Krashen, 1999, 2003). A recent study by Mason (2003) was a direct demonstration that these forms of supplementation do not add to the power of reading. EFL students in Japan participated in an extensive reading program for three semesters. One group wrote short summaries of what they read in Japanese. Thus, their progress was dependent on what they read in English. A second group wrote summaries in English, and a third group wrote summaries in English that were corrected, and returned, and students had to rewrite the summaries based on the corrections. Mason reported no difference in gains in English among the three groups, and reported that the group that wrote summaries in Japanese was the most efficient, acquiring as much English in about half the total time devoted to English. Other forms of supplementation may work, however. It is reasonable to hypothesize that at lower levels, when students first reach the level at which they can read independently in English, it will be of great help to inform students what is available; this is the goal of sheltered popular literature. Also, teacher-student conferences in which books are discussed can stimulate interest in more reading. Shin (2004) and Neuman and Celano (2004) provide cases suggesting that direct encouragement from teachers and librarians resulted in more reading; in these cases, students, it appears, had the competence to read, but lacked the confidence to try reading more challenging material and/or did not realize how interesting the reading was. Note that the forms of supplementation that are hypothesized to be useful are those that help make input more comprehensible, more interesting, and encourage students to obtain more input. Those that research has not shown to be effective are those that attempt to use means other than comprehensible input. The use of "unnatural" forms of supplementation is stimulated by the assumption that natural acquisition is not enough, that more needs to be done than simply supply interesting, comprehensible input. This fear could be a self-fulfilling prophecy: there has been little attempt to maximize the amount of comprehensible input, little attempt to combine interesting discussions, read alouds, recreational listening, listening to tapes, watching films, all in one educational program. We do not know the limits of comprehensible input, because it has not really been tried. References Cho, K.S. and Krashen, S. 1994. Acquisition of vocabulary from the Sweet Valley Kids series. Journal of Reading 37: 6620667.

Cho, K.S. and Krashen, S. 1995a. From Sweet Valley Kids to Harlequins in one year. California English 1,1: 18-19. Cho, K.S. and Krashen, S. 1995b. Becoming a dragon: Progress in English as a second language through narrow free voluntary reading. California Reader 29: 9-10. Cohen, K. 1999. Reluctant eighth grade readers enjoy sustained silent reading. California Reader 33 (1): 22-25. Constantino, R., Lee, S.Y., Cho, K.S., and Krashen, S. 1997. Free voluntary reading as a predictor of TOEFL scores. Applied Language Learning 8: 111-118. Cowan, J. R. 1974. Lexical and syntactic research for the design of EFL reading materials. TESOL Quarterly, 8(4), 389-400. Dupuy, B. 1999. Narrow listening: An alternative way to develop listening comprehension in the foreign language classroom. System 24(1):97-100. Flurkey, A. and Xu, J. (Eds) 2003. On the Revolution of Reading: The Selected Writings of Kenneth S. Goodman. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Greaney, V. and Clarke, M. 1973. A longitudinal study of the effects of two reading programs on leisure time reading habits. In D. Moyle (Ed.) Reading: What of the Future? London: United Kingdom Reading Association. pp. 107-114. Herda, R. and Ramos, F. 2001. How consistently do students read during sustained silent reading? California School Library Journal 24 (2): 29-31. Krashen, S. 1988. Do we learn to read by reading? The relationship between free reading and reading ability. In D. Tannen (Ed.) Linguistics in Context: Connecting Observation and Understanding. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. pp. 269-298. Krashen, S. 1994. The input hypothesis and its rivals. In N. Ellis (Ed.) Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages. London: Academic Press. pp. 45-77. Krashen, S. 1996. The case for narrow listening. System 24(1): 97-100.

Krashen, S. 1999. Seeking a role for grammar: A review of some recent studies. Foreign Language Annals 32(2): 245-257. Krashen, S. 2001. More smoke and mirrors: A critique of the National Reading Panel report on fluency. Phi Delta Kappan 83: 119-123. Krashen, S. 2003. Explorations in Language Acquisition and Use: The Taipei Lectures. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Krashen, S. 2004. The case for narrow reading. Language Magazine 3(5): 17-19. LaBrant, L. 1958. An evaluation of free reading. In C. Hunnicutt and W. Iverson (Eds.), Research in the Three R's. New York: Harper and Brothers, pp. 154-161. Lamme, L. 1976. Are reading habits and abilities related? Reading Teacher 30: 21-27. Lee, S.Y. (2001) What Makes It So Difficult to Write. Taipei: Crane Publishing Company. Lee, Y.O., Krashen, S., and Gribbons, B. 1996. The effect of reading on the acquisition of English relative clauses. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics 113-114: 263-273. Mason, B. 2003. A Study of Extensive Reading and the Development of Grammatical Accuracy by Japanese University Students Learning English. Ed.D. Dissertation, Temple University, Osaka, Japan. Neuman, S. and Celano, D. 2004. Save the libraries! Educational Leadership 61(6): 82-85. Pilgreen, J. 2000. The SSR Handbook: How to Organize and Maintain a Sustained Silent Reading Program. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Pilgreen, J. and Krashen, S. 1993. Sustained silent reading with English as a second language high school students: Impact on reading comprehension, reading frequency, and reading enjoyment. School Library Media Quarterly 22: 21-23. Rodrigo, V. and Krashen, S. 1996. La aplicación del argumento de la audición

enfocada en el Aula de Clase. Granada English Teaching Assocation, 4:2): 71-75. Shin, F. (2004) Should we just tell them to read? The role of direct encouragement in promoting recreational reading. Knowledge Quest 32(3) Smith, F. 1994. Understanding Reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erbaum. Fifth Edition, Stokes, J., Krashen, S., and Kartchner, J. 1998. Factors in the acquisition of the present subjunctive in Spanish: The role of reading and study. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics 121-122:19-25. Von Sprecken, D. and Krashen, S. 1998. Do students read during sustained silent reading? California Reader 32(1): 11-13. Wheldall, K. and Entwhistle, J. 1988. Back in the USSR: The effect of teacher modeling of silent reading on pupils reading behavior in the primary school classroom. Educational Psychology 8: 51-56.