Income Inequality and Happiness



Similar documents
Unlike other animals, human beings spend

Income inequality: Trends and Measures

Who Is Working For Minimum Wage In Ontario? By Sheila Block

Spending Money on Others Promotes Happiness

Europeans Work To Live and Americans Live To Work (Who is Happy to Work More: Americans or Europeans?)

Running Head: MONEY, WELL-BEING, AND LOSS AVERSION 1. Money, Well-Being, and Loss Aversion:

Well-being evidence for policy: A review

Living a happy, healthy and satisfying life Tineke de Jonge, Christianne Hupkens, Jan-Willem Bruggink, Statistics Netherlands,

An Examination of the Association Between Parental Abuse History and Subsequent Parent-Child Relationships

HYPOTHESIS TESTING: CONFIDENCE INTERVALS, T-TESTS, ANOVAS, AND REGRESSION

Gender Differences in Employed Job Search Lindsey Bowen and Jennifer Doyle, Furman University

R e a l i t y C h e c k

Figure 1: Gini coefficient

Inequality Trends in Family & Household Income, and Wealth (Gini Indices).

The Effects of State Public K 12 Education Expenditures On Income Distribution

The Effects of Parent Trust on Perceived Influence and School Involvement

Economic Development and the Gender Wage Gap Sherri Haas

Paid and Unpaid Labor in Developing Countries: an inequalities in time use approach

Sample Paper for Research Methods. Daren H. Kaiser. Indiana University Purdue University Fort Wayne

Life Satisfaction and Relative Income: Perceptions and Evidence

Economic inequality and educational attainment across a generation

Civic Pathways toward Economic Opportunity for Low-income Individuals

SOEPpapers on Multidisciplinary Panel Data Research

Barriers & Incentives to Obtaining a Bachelor of Science Degree in Nursing

IMPACT OF TRUST, PRIVACY AND SECURITY IN FACEBOOK INFORMATION SHARING

The New Stylized Facts about Income and Subjective Well-Being

Financial capability and saving: Evidence from the British Household Panel Survey

Would You Be Happier If You Were Richer? A Focusing Illusion

Does student performance reduce imprisonment? *

RISING HAPPINESS IN NATIONS A reply to Easterlin

Unhappy on the job by Margot Shields

Staying on Course: The Effects of Savings and Assets on the College Progress of Young Adults By William Elliott and Sondra Beverly

Why Are Conservatives Happier Than Liberals? Jaime L. Napier and John T. Jost

INFLATION, INTEREST RATE, AND EXCHANGE RATE: WHAT IS THE RELATIONSHIP?

the future of digital trust

Equality Impact Assessment Support for Mortgage Interest

INVESTIGATION OF EFFECTIVE FACTORS IN USING MOBILE ADVERTISING IN ANDIMESHK. Abstract

The rising costs of living and. How much happiness does money buy? Income and subjective well-being in New Zealand

GREGORY SHARP Curriculum Vitae November 2015

Who Tends to Trust in Australia? An Empirical Analysis of Survey Data

In the past two decades, the federal government has dramatically

1) Write the following as an algebraic expression using x as the variable: Triple a number subtracted from the number

Income inequalities in Italy: trend over time

THE EDUCATIONAL ATTAINMENT OF THE WORKFORCE

Women's Concerns About Financial Security

NHS Staff Management and Health Service Quality

Child Obesity and Socioeconomic Status

Fall 2011 National Survey of American Public Opinion on Climate Change Christopher Borick and Barry Rabe

Educational Attainment in the United States: 2015

Interaction of global and national income inequalities. Branko Milanovic and John E. Roemer 1

Summary. Accessibility and utilisation of health services in Ghana 245

SCHOOL OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SCIENCES DON T FORGET TO RECODE YOUR MISSING VALUES

PAREP: POPULATIONS AT RISK TO ENVIRONMENTAL POLLUTION

Mode and Patient-mix Adjustment of the CAHPS Hospital Survey (HCAHPS)

Population Percent C.I. All Hennepin County adults aged 18 and older 11.9% ± 1.1

The Role of Savings and Wealth in Reducing "Wilt" between Expectations and College Attendance By William Elliott III & Sondra Beverly

Research into Issues Surrounding Human Bones in Museums Prepared for

The Office of Public Services Reform The Drivers of Satisfaction with Public Services

Educational Attainment

SCIENCE-RELATED ATTITUDES AND INTERESTS OF STUDENTS

Income and Subjective Well-Being: How Important is the Methodology?*

The Digital Divide in Students' Usage of Technology Tools: A Multilevel Analysis of the Role of Teacher Practices and Classroom Characteristics

Gender Stereotypes Associated with Altruistic Acts

2. Incidence, prevalence and duration of breastfeeding

Premaster Statistics Tutorial 4 Full solutions

African American s Political Attitudes, Party Affiliation, and Voting Behavior

Answer: C. The strength of a correlation does not change if units change by a linear transformation such as: Fahrenheit = 32 + (5/9) * Centigrade

How s Life in the United States?

MATHEMATICS AS THE CRITICAL FILTER: CURRICULAR EFFECTS ON GENDERED CAREER CHOICES

Assessing the Impact of Relative Social Position and Absolute Community Resources on Depression and Obesity Among Smokers

budget brief On Tuesday, August 3, legislative leaders proposed a new budget plan, including a tax swap that would increase some

Linear Models in STATA and ANOVA

Determinants of Item Nonresponse to Web and Mail Respondents in Three Address-Based Mixed-Mode Surveys of the General Public

CALCULATIONS & STATISTICS

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE DEMAND FACTORS FOR ONLINE ACCOUNTING COURSES

So what s a state to do? Some recent research provides guidance.

Stock price reaction to analysts EPS forecast error

Market Potential and Sales Forecasting

Key Facts About Poverty and Income in Texas

In an experimental study there are two types of variables: Independent variable (I will abbreviate this as the IV)

Analysis of the perceptions of accounting students and practitioners regarding the ethnicity of earnings management post Sarbanes-Oxley

Rates for Vehicle Loans: Race and Loan Source

Striking it Richer: The Evolution of Top Incomes in the United States (Update using 2006 preliminary estimates)

Elasticity. I. What is Elasticity?

Guidelines for National Indicators 1

Taxation and more representation? On fiscal policy, social mobility and democracy in Latin America

Georgia Department of Education Common Core Georgia Performance Standards Framework Teacher Edition Coordinate Algebra Unit 4

Simple linear regression

THE EFFECT OF ECONOMIC GROWTH ON POVERTY IN EASTERN EUROPE

Perspectives. In view of the. Pathways to Financial Advancement

Youth bulges and youth unemployment Youth bulges are not a major factor explaining current levels of youth unemployment

Restaurant Tips 1. Restaurant Tips and Service Quality: A Weak Relationship or Just Weak Measurement? Michael Lynn. School of Hotel Administration

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2009(No.22) * Municipal Corruption Victimization 1

Lecture 13/Chapter 10 Relationships between Measurement (Quantitative) Variables

Test Bias. As we have seen, psychological tests can be well-conceived and well-constructed, but

The Pursuit of Happiness: Time, Money, and Social Connection

The Importance and Impact of Nursing Informatics Competencies for Baccalaureate Nursing Students and Registered Nurses

The Impact of Economic Globalization on Income Distribution: Empirical Evidence in China. Abstract

SPSS Guide: Regression Analysis

Women, Wages and Work A report prepared by the UNC Charlotte Urban Institute for the Women s Summit April 11, 2011

Transcription:

Research Report Income Inequality and Happiness Shigehiro Oishi 1, Selin Kesebir 2, and Ed Diener 3,4 1 Department of Psychology, University of Virginia; 2 Darden School of Business, University of Virginia; 3 Department of Psychology, University of Illinois; and 4 Gallup, Washington, District of Columbia Psychological Science 22(9) 1095 1100 The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797611417262 http://pss.sagepub.com Abstract Using General Social Survey data from 1972 to 2008, we found that Americans were on average happier in the years with less national income inequality than in the years with more national income inequality. We further demonstrated that this inverse relation between income inequality and happiness was explained by perceived fairness and general trust. That is, Americans trusted other people less and perceived other people to be less fair in the years with more national income inequality than in the years with less national income inequality. The negative association between income inequality and happiness held for lower-income respondents, but not for higher-income respondents. Most important, we found that the negative link between income inequality and the happiness of lower-income respondents was explained not by lower household income, but by perceived unfairness and lack of trust. Keywords happiness, income inequality, fairness, trust, well-being, sociocultural factors, social structure, socioeconomic status Received 4/14/11; Revision accepted 6/1/11 One of the most profound social changes in the United States over the last 40 years has been the growing income inequality among social classes (Hacker & Pierson, 2010; see Fig. 1). One commonly used index of income inequality is the Gini coefficient. 1 In the 1960s and 1970s, the Gini coefficient in the United States was much lower than in France and was on par with many other European nations (Atkinson, 1996). In contrast, by 2008, the Gini coefficient was much higher for the United States than for most European nations and for Canada (United Nations Development Programme, 2009). The social consequences of this growing inequality in the United States have been investigated in economics (Piketty & Saez, 2003), political science (Bartels, 2008), sociology (Blau & Blau, 1982), and epidemiology (Kawachi, Kennedy, Lochner, & Prothrow-Stith, 1997). In psychology, however, surprisingly little empirical work has been conducted on income inequality. What are the psychological consequences of income inequality? Are individuals happier when national wealth is distributed more evenly? Although there is a large body of research on income and happiness (Diener & Oishi, 2000; Dunn, Gilbert, & Wilson, 2011; Stevenson & Wolfers, 2008), few researchers have investigated the relation between income inequality and happiness. The small amount of existing research on this relationship has exclusively focused on cross-national (Berg & Veenhoven, 2010; Diener, Diener, & Diener, 1995; Helliwell & Huang, 2008), cross-state (Alesina, DiTella, & MacCulloch, 2004), or cross-city comparisons (Hagerty, 2000). Most important, existing research has produced mixed results. Some researchers have found a negative association between income inequality and happiness (e.g., Hagerty, 2000), but other researchers have found no association (e.g., Berg & Veenhoven, 2010). These cross-sectional analyses are also vulnerable to various thirdvariable accounts. For instance, nations (e.g., Brazil), states (e.g., Mississippi), and cities (e.g., New Orleans) with high income-inequality indices are also different from nations (e.g., Denmark), states (e.g., Massachusetts), and cities (e.g., Minneapolis) with low income-inequality indices in other factors, including climate, geography, population size, natural resources, and language. Cross-national comparisons on income inequality have also been criticized because Gini coefficients were not always calculated in the same fashion across nations (Deininger & Squire, 1996). In contrast, cross-temporal analyses within a single nation naturally control for many third variables (e.g., geography, language) inherent in cross-national comparisons and are also free from the technical issues surrounding the calculation of Gini coefficients. Therefore, in the study reported here, we conducted a much stronger test than has previously been conducted for the association between income inequality and happiness by focusing on changes in income inequality within the United States. Corresponding Author: Shigehiro Oishi, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400 E-mail: soishi@virginia.edu

1096 Oishi et al..46.44.42 Gini Coefficient.40.38.36.34.32.30 1947 1952 1957 1962 1967 1972 1977 1982 1987 1992 1997 2002 2007 Year Fig. 1. Income inequality in the United States from 1947 to 2009, as indexed by the Gini coefficient. In addition to these methodological limitations, previous research on income inequality and happiness has not identified any psychological mechanisms to account for the link between societal income inequality and individual-level happiness. In our study, we postulated and tested two psychological mechanisms. First, many people (especially low- and middle-income earners) are likely to perceive the world to be unfair only if the rich get richer. It is possible, then, that people will perceive less fairness in the years with greater income disparity, and this perception in turn could lower those individuals overall happiness in such years. Second, income disparity could disjoint and divide community members (Putnam, 2000), and as a result, it could make people trust others less (Ichida et al., 2009). To the extent that trust is positively associated with happiness (Inglehart, 1999), lowered general trust could explain why people are in general less happy in times of income inequality. In addition to examining these two mediating mechanisms, we also investigated whether the relation between national income disparity and individual happiness is moderated by that individual s income level. It is likely that income inequality disproportionately affects the happiness of low-income individuals because income inequality reflects the perceived phenomenon of the rich getting richer. Because the negative link between income inequality and the happiness of lowincome individuals could be due to reduced household income in the years with greater income disparity, we also tested whether the negative association between income inequality and the happiness of low-income individuals is due to this economic factor (i.e., reduced household income), as opposed to psychological factors (i.e., lower perceived levels of fairness and trust). Method The participants were 53,043 respondents to the General Social Survey (GSS; National Opinion Research Center, 2010) from 1972 to 2008 (29,675 females, 23,368 males; 43,323 self-identified as White, 7,314 self-identified as Black, and 2,406 self-identified as an ethnicity other than White or Black; age ranged from 18 to 89 years, with a mean of 45.52 years). Of the total sample, 48,318 provided valid responses to the happiness item on the GSS. Thus, the mean size per year of the final sample was 1,789.56 (range = 1,337 2,986). We measured subjective well-being with the three-point happiness item on the GSS. This item is the only measure of subjective well-being that has been included in every survey since the first in 1972. Respondents answered the following question: Taken all together, how would you say things are these days would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy, or not too happy? (1 = not too happy, 2 = pretty happy, 3 = very happy). To measure perceived levels of fairness and general trust, we used the following questions, which were presented much later than the happiness item on the questionnaire: Do you think most people would try to take advantage of you if they got a chance, or would they try to be fair? (1 = take advantage, 2 = depends, 3 = fair), and Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you can t be too careful in dealing with people? (1 = cannot trust, 2 = depends, 3 = can trust). Respondents also reported their household income (listed under the variable name realinc on the GSS), which was converted to 1986 U.S. dollars. For our analyses, we used the

Inequality and Happiness 1097 2.26 1974 1978 1988 2.24 2.22 1973 1977 1976 1984 1986 1990 1989 2000 Mean Happiness 2.20 2.18 2.16 1975 1980 1983 1982 1991 1985 1987 1993 1998 2006 1996 2002 2004 1994 2.14 1972 2008 2.12.35.37.39.41.43.45 Gini Coefficient Fig. 2. Scatter plot (with best-fitting regression line) showing mean American happiness scores as a function of income inequality, as indexed by the Gini coefficient, from 1972 to 2008. log-transformed household income. We obtained the index of income inequality (Gini index) from the U.S. Census Bureau (2009). Results Because respondents were nested within years, we created a multilevel random-coefficient model, using Mplus 4.2 software (Muthén & Muthén, 2007). The simple, direct-effect model showed a significant negative association between Gini coefficient and happiness, b = 0.385, 95% confidence interval = [ 0.730, 0.041], SE = 0.176, Z = 2.19, p <.05 (see Fig. 2). Americans were on average happier at times of relative national income equality than of relative national income inequality. We next used a multilevel mediation analysis to test whether perceived fairness and trust would explain the inverse association between income inequality and happiness 2 (Preacher, Zyphur, & Zhang, 2010). This analysis revealed that Americans perceived others to be less fair and less trustworthy in times of income inequality than in times of income equality (see Fig. 3). Once fairness and trust were included in the equation, the multilevel association between income inequality and happiness disappeared. As predicted, Americans perceived others to be less fair and trustworthy in the years with greater income disparity, and this perception in turn explained why Americans reported lower levels of happiness in those years (see Table 1). Considering that greater income inequality is mostly due to the rich getting richer, we next tested the possibility that the association between income inequality and happiness could be different for individuals across different income levels. Gini coefficients showed a strong negative association with the mean happiness level of the lowest-20% income group, r(25) =.54, p <.01, and the mean happiness level of the next lowest (20 40%) income group, r(25) =.63, p <.01. That is, lower-income respondents happiness was lower in the years with more income inequality than in the years with less income 2.19 (0.319)* Income Inequality General Trust 0.206 (0.242) 0.06 (0.005)* Happiness 2.04 (0.304)* 0.079 (0.004)* Perceived Fairness Fig. 3. Mediation model showing the relation between income inequality and happiness as mediated by perceived fairness and general trust. Unstandardized regression coefficients are shown, and standard errors are given in parentheses. Asterisks indicate significant coefficients (p <.01).

1098 Oishi et al. Table 1. Results of Multilevel Mediation Analyses Investigating Whether Perceived Fairness and General Trust Mediate the Relation Between Income Inequality and Happiness Group and predictor Indirect effect 95% confidence interval Z p Total sample (N = 31,873) Perceived fairness 0.161 (0.026) [ 0.211, 0.110] 6.25 <.001 General trust 0.131 (0.026) [ 0.182, 0.081] 5.08 <.001 Lowest-20% income group (n = 5,018) Perceived fairness 0.344 (0.058) [ 0.460, 0.228] 5.84 <.001 General trust 0.099 (0.027) [ 0.151, 0.047] 3.73 <.01 Household income 0.082 (0.057) [ 0.029, 0.193] 1.44.15 20 40% income group (n = 4,900) Perceived fairness 0.115 (0.028) [ 0.166, 0.065] 4.46 <.001 General trust 0.148 (0.039) [ 0.225, 0.071] 3.76 <.001 Household income 0.057 (0.074) [ 0.203, 0.089] 0.77.44 40 60% income group (n = 6,093) Perceived fairness 0.068 (0.026) [ 0.119, 0.017] 2.61 <.01 General trust 0.071 (0.026) [ 0.123, 0.020] 2.73 <.01 Household income 0.002 (0.028) [ 0.053, 0.057] 0.07.94 60 80% income group (n = 7,767) Perceived fairness 0.095 (0.022) [ 0.139, 0.052] 4.285 <.001 General trust 0.092 (0.033) [ 0.156, 0.027] 2.796 <.01 Household income 0.069 (0.072) [ 0.072, 0.210] 0.956.34 Top-20% income group (n = 5,105) Perceived fairness 0.030 (0.021) [ 0.071, 0.011] 1.432.15 General trust 0.029 (0.025) [ 0.079, 0.021] 1.144.25 Household income 0.112 (0.107) [ 0.097, 0.322] 1.051.29 Note: Standard errors are given in parentheses. Variance for the intercept of happiness in the simple, direct multilevel model was.00079, χ 2 (26, N = 48,318) = 113.57, p <.001. Residual variance was.403 (SE =.004). Residual variance in the mediation analysis for the total sample was.394 (SE =.004). inequality. In contrast, income inequality of the year was unrelated to the mean happiness of the middle (40 60%) income group, r(25) =.12, p =.56, the upper-middle (60 80%) income group, r(25) =.09, p =.65, and the top-20% income group, r(25) =.03, p =.88. We tested the moderation effect more formally using a multigroup, multilevel random-coefficient analysis. As predicted, the model fit was excellent when the association between Gini coefficients and happiness was allowed to vary across the five income groups, χ 2 (8) = 9.55, p =.33, comparative fit index =.969, root-mean-square error of approximation = 0.004, and significantly better than when the association was fixed at the same value, χ 2 (4) = 33.70, p <.01. The negative link between income inequality and the happiness of low-income individuals could be due to reduced household income among low-income individuals in the years with greater income disparity. Indeed, average household income was lower in the years with more income inequality than in the years with less income inequality for the lowest- 20% income group, b = 4.455, SE = 0.512, Z = 8.70, p <.01, and the 20 40% income group, b = 1.42, SE = 0.353, Z = 4.03, p <.01. Gini coefficients were not related to household income for the 40 60% income group, b = 0.025, SE = 0.341, Z = 0.072. As expected, high-income groups earned more money in the years with greater income inequality than in the years with less income inequality, b = 1.92, SE = 0.469, Z = 4.10, p <.01, for the 60 80% group; b = 4.18, SE = 0.75, Z = 5.58, p <.01, for the top-20% group. Thus, we next tested whether the negative association between income inequality and the happiness of low-income individuals was due to reduced household income, as opposed to lower perceived levels of fairness and trust. In conducting this multigroup, multilevel mediation analysis, we also included four demographic variables at the individual level (i.e., sex, race, marital status, and age). As Table 1 shows, the decreased happiness of lower-income individuals in the years with greater income inequality was not due to the economic factor of reduced income. Instead, it was explained by lower perceived fairness and general trust for the lowest-20% income group, the 20 40% income group, and the 40 60% income group. That is, the negative association between income inequality and happiness was explained by lower levels of perceived fairness and general trust in these three income groups, and once these mediators were included, the direct association between income inequality and happiness disappeared, b = 0.026, Z = 0.07, for the lowest-20% group; b = 0.42, Z = 0.94, for the 20 40% group; and b = 0.54, Z = 1.34, for the 40 60% income group. Unexpectedly, we found that when controlling for perceived fairness, general trust, and household

Inequality and Happiness 1099 income, respondents in the 60 80% income group were happier in the years with more income inequality than in the years with less income inequality, b = 0.58, SE = 0.287, Z = 2.02, p <.05. Finally, among the top-20% income group, income inequality was not associated with perceived fairness, b = 0.64, SE = 0.409, Z = 1.56, n.s. Also, general trust among this group was not associated with happiness, b = 0.014, SE = 0.011, Z = 1.27, n.s. Thus, the aforementioned mediation processes (high income inequality yields lower perceived fairness and general trust, which yields less happiness) did not hold for the richest-20% income group. Discussion We investigated the relation between income inequality and happiness over a 37-year period in the United States. As predicted, Americans were on average less happy in years with more societal income inequality than in years with less societal income inequality. We demonstrated that the negative association between societal income inequality and individual-level happiness was explained by perceived fairness and general trust. We also found that the negative association between income disparity and happiness was present among Americans with lower incomes but not among Americans with higher incomes. Moreover, we showed that it was not the reduced income but the lowered levels of perceived fairness and trust that made low-income Americans feel less happy in the years with greater income inequality. Although there is a large body of research on income inequality in other social and behavioral sciences (see Wilkinson & Pickett, 2009, for a review), relatively few researchers have investigated the role of income inequality in psychological science. More important, the small body of existing research on income inequality and happiness has not examined any psychological mechanisms. To this end, our mediation findings for the first time delineate the psychological mechanisms linking a socioecological factor (income inequality) with individual-level happiness, and therefore contribute to the emerging topics in socioecological psychology (Oishi & Graham, 2010; Oishi, Kesebir, & Snyder, 2009). Social scientists have debated why Americans have not become happier over the last 50 years despite the enormous growth in national wealth (Easterlin, 1974). At first, researchers assumed that economic growth was not associated with an increase in individual happiness because of social-comparison processes (other people s wealth was also increasing), upward shifts in aspirations, and hedonic adaptation (Easterlin, 1974). Recently, however, researchers have found that economic growth is in fact associated with an increase in happiness over time in many nations other than the United States (Stevenson & Wolfers, 2008). It has been unclear, however, why massive economic growth over the past decades has translated to an increase in happiness among the Danish, French, and Germans, but not among Americans. The existing theories cannot explain the anomaly of the United States, as an upward shift in aspiration, hedonic adaptation, and social comparisons should apply similarly to other nations with economic growth. Our findings provide a novel clue for this puzzle. Income growth without income disparity is likely to result in an increase in the mean happiness of a general population. This new hypothesis needs to be carefully tested in the future. It is important to recognize four limitations of our research. First, happiness, fairness, and general trust were each measured by single items. Thus, measurement error is expected to be far from trivial. Although researchers have used the same single-item happiness (Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2006), fairness, and trust measures (Kawachi et al., 1997), it is important to replicate the current findings with better-validated multi-item scales. Second, we examined only perceived fairness and general trust as potential mediators. There might be other potential mediators that were not measured in this study. Third, although we emphasized the negative aspects of income inequality, there might be circumstances under which income inequality reflects that individuals who contribute more receive greater rewards. Furthermore, the relation between societal income inequality and individual happiness is likely to vary across time, nations, and political cultures (e.g., Alesina et al., 2004; Napier & Jost, 2008). In conclusion, Americans are happier when national wealth is distributed more evenly than when it is distributed less evenly. If the ultimate goal of society is to make its citizens happy (Bentham, 1789/2008), then it is desirable to consider policies that produce more income equality, fairness, and general trust. Acknowledgments We thank Tim Wilson, Felicity Miao, and Uli Schimmack for their help on this project. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Notes 1. The U.S. Census Bureau (2009) reports the Gini coefficients for families and for households, respectively. We used the Gini coefficient for families in our analyses. Between 1972 and 2008, the correlation between the Gini coefficients for families and for households was.997. 2. The slopes of trust and perceived fairness were fixed across years, as variance was nonsignificant for both slopes. References Alesina, A., DiTella, R., & MacCulloch, R. (2004). Happiness and inequality: Are Europeans and Americans different? Journal of Public Economics, 88, 2009 2042. Atkinson, A. B. (1996). Income distribution in Europe and the United States. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 12, 15 28. Bartels, L. M. (2008). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the new gilded age. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

1100 Oishi et al. Bentham, J. (2008). An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. New York, NY: Barnes & Noble. (Original work published 1789) Berg, M., & Veenhoven, R. (2010). Income inequality and happiness in 119 nations. In B. Greve (Ed.), Social policy and happiness in Europe (pp. 174 194). Cheltenham, England: Edgar Elgar. Blau, J. R., & Blau, P. M. (1982). The cost of inequality: Metropolitan structure and violent crime. American Sociological Review, 47, 114 129. Deininger, K., & Squire, L. (1996). A new data set measuring income inequality. World Bank Economic Review, 10, 565 591. Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 851 864. Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 185 218). Boston, MA: MIT Press. Dunn, E. W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn t make you happy, then you probably aren t spending it right. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 115 125. Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In P. A. David & W. R. Melvin (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth (pp. 89 125). New York, NY: Academic Press. Hacker, J. S., & Pierson, P. (2010). Winner-take-all politics: How Washington made the rich richer and turned its back on the middle class. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Hagerty, M. R. (2000). Social comparisons of income in one s community: Evidence from national surveys of income and happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 764 771. Helliwell, J. F., & Huang, H. (2008). How s your government? International evidence linking good government and well-being. British Journal of Political Science, 38, 595 619. Ichida, Y., Kondo, K., Hirai, H., Hanibuchi, T., Yoshikawa, G., & Murata, C. (2009). Social capital, income inequality, and selfrated health in Chita Peninsula, Japan: A multilevel analysis of older people in 25 communities. Social Science & Medicine, 69, 489 499. Inglehart, R. (1999). Trust, well-being and democracy. In M. E. Warren (Ed.), Democracy and trust (pp. 88 120). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science, 312, 1908 1910. Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B. P., Lochner, K., & Prothrow-Stith, D. (1997). Social capital, income inequality, and mortality. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 1491 1498. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Mplus: Statistical analysis with latent variables user s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Author. Napier, J. L., & Jost, J. T. (2008). Why are conservatives happier than liberals? Psychological Science, 19, 565 572. National Opinion Research Center. (2010). Download data. Retrieved from http://www.norc.uchicago.edu/gss+website/download/ Oishi, S., & Graham, J. (2010). Social ecology: Lost and found in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 356 377. Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., & Snyder, B. H. (2009). Sociology: A lost connection in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 334 353. Piketty, T., & Saez, E. (2003). Income inequality in the United States, 1913 1998. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1 39. Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15, 209 233. Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008, Spring). Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, pp. 1 102. United Nations Development Programme. (2009). Human development report 2009: Economy and inequality. Retrieved from http://hdrstats.undp.org/en/indicators/161.html U.S. Census Bureau. (2009). Income inequality. Retrieved from http:// www.census.gov/hhes/www/income/data/historical/inequality/ index.html Wilkinson, R. G., & Pickett, K. E. (2009). Income inequality and social dysfunction. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 493 511.