PATTERNS OF LEADER CHARACTERISTICS: IMPLICATIONS FOR PERFORMANCE AND DEVELOPMENT



Similar documents
Development of leadership skills: Experience and timing

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Battery Bias

Self-Reported Leadership Experiences in Relation to Inventoried Social and Emotional Intelligence

IT S LONELY AT THE TOP: EXECUTIVES EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE SELF [MIS] PERCEPTIONS. Fabio Sala, Ph.D. Hay/McBer

o and organizational data were the primary measures used to

Center for Effective Organizations

Influenced by - Alfred Binet intelligence testing movement

English Summary 1. cognitively-loaded test and a non-cognitive test, the latter often comprised of the five-factor model of

An Analysis of IDEA Student Ratings of Instruction in Traditional Versus Online Courses Data

THE ASSESSMENT OF GRADUATE PROGRAMS IN ART AND DESIGN. National Association of Schools of Art and Design

GMAC. Predicting Success in Graduate Management Doctoral Programs

Lesson 2: Introduction and History of I/O Psychology

Integrating HR & Talent Management Processes

Measuring the MBTI... And Coming Up Short

Instructional Design Basics. Instructor Guide

Analyzing Research Articles: A Guide for Readers and Writers 1. Sam Mathews, Ph.D. Department of Psychology The University of West Florida

History and Purpose of the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, FRESNO DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAM

Do Personality Profiles Differ in the Pakistani Software Industry and Academia A Study

CREATING LEARNING OUTCOMES

Using Personality to Predict Outbound Call Center Job Performance

Level 1 Articulated Plan: The plan has established the mission, vision, goals, actions, and key

Guidelines and Timeline for the MAP and Oral Exam

A TECHNICAL REPORT ON THE CLIFTON STRENGTHSFINDER WITH COLLEGE STUDENTS. Laurie A. Schreiner, Ph.D. Azusa Pacific University

The Nature and Power of Interests

Self-Other Agreement on a 360-Degree Leadership Evaluation. Stefanie K. Halverson Rice University. Scott Tonidandel Rice University

Course Descriptions. Seminar in Organizational Behavior II

Review of Creative leadership: Skills that drive change

How To Know What Motivates Someone To Persist After Failure

ACT Research Explains New ACT Test Writing Scores and Their Relationship to Other Test Scores

Building Online Learning Communities: Factors Supporting Collaborative Knowledge-Building. Joe Wheaton, Associate Professor The Ohio State University

A successful market segmentation initiative answers the following critical business questions: * How can we a. Customer Status.

The Science and Art of Market Segmentation Using PROC FASTCLUS Mark E. Thompson, Forefront Economics Inc, Beaverton, Oregon

Forecasting the sales of an innovative agro-industrial product with limited information: A case of feta cheese from buffalo milk in Thailand

Teachers Emotional Intelligence and Its Relationship with Job Satisfaction

A Comparison of Training & Scoring in Distributed & Regional Contexts Writing

GMAC. Which Programs Have the Highest Validity: Identifying Characteristics that Affect Prediction of Success 1

Position Statement IDENTIFICATION OF STUDENTS WITH SPECIFIC LEARNING DISABILITIES

David H. Gray. 5 March, 2008

Learning about the influence of certain strategies and communication structures in the organizational effectiveness

ROLE OF ORGANIZATIONAL CULTURE ON PROJECT SUCCESS

Assessment Plan Department of Psychology Park University. Preparing learners to think critically. Preparing learners to think

Student Intelligence and Academic Achievement in Albanian Universities. Case of Vlora University

Study Plan in Psychology Education

Chapter 6: The Information Function 129. CHAPTER 7 Test Calibration

Educational Goals and Objectives A GUIDE TO DEVELOPING LEARNER BASED INSTRUCTION

Writing Learning Objectives

How To Know How To Be A Successful Academic Recipe Cards

AGRICULTURAL EDUCATION COMPETENCIES AND PROGRESS TOWARD A DOCTORAL DEGREE

California State University, Stanislaus Doctor of Education (Ed.D.), Educational Leadership Assessment Plan

A Qualitative Investigation of the Human Resource Management Practices in Small Businesses

Leading with Personality Type, Satir s Communication Model and the Learning Styles Inventory

THE NEGLECTED ORGANIZATIONAL "WHAT" OF LEADERSHIP

Social Work Program Outcomes

Thinking in Big Data. Tony Shan. Nov 5, 2014

Industrial Engineering Definition of Tuning

IN THE CHANGING WORLD OF HUMAN RESOURCES: MATCHING MEASURES TO MISSION

My Thinking Styles TECHNICAL REPORT. by Judy Chartrand. Copyright 2011 NCS Pearson, Inc. All rights reserved.

MALONE UNIVERSITY ASSESSMENT ANNUAL REPORT CARD. Psychology Program: Department:

Abstract. Introduction

Local outlier detection in data forensics: data mining approach to flag unusual schools

Our Lady of the Lake University

Welcome to Strategies for Unbiased Performance Evaluation!

The Impact of Management Information Systems on the Performance of Governmental Organizations- Study at Jordanian Ministry of Planning

Fair Labor Standards Act Decision Under section 4(f) of title 29, United States Code

The University of Texas at Austin

Articles. Implementing Technology Education Problem-Solving Activities. V. William DeLuca

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEGREE. Educational Leadership Doctor of Philosophy Degree Major Course Requirements. EDU721 (3.

Is the choice of a degree program a factor of personality types and degree of congruence

CFSD 21 ST CENTURY SKILL RUBRIC CRITICAL & CREATIVE THINKING

Teacher Evaluation. Missouri s Educator Evaluation System

Content and Process Theories of Motivation

SigmaRADIUS Leadership Effectiveness Report

CLUSTER ANALYSIS FOR SEGMENTATION

Technical Report No. 13

Current Problems and Resolutions. The Relative Effects of Competence and Likability on Interpersonal Attraction

IMPACT OF CORE SELF EVALUATION (CSE) ON JOB SATISFACTION IN EDUCATION SECTOR OF PAKISTAN Yasir IQBAL University of the Punjab Pakistan

The primary goal of this thesis was to understand how the spatial dependence of

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data

Effect of Job Autonomy Upon Organizational Commitment of Employees at Different Hierarchical Level

Preferred Learning Styles for Respiratory Care Students at Texas State University San Marcos

GEORGIA STANDARDS FOR THE APPROVAL OF PROFESSIONAL EDUCATION UNITS AND EDUCATOR PREPARATION PROGRAMS

Career Development in a College

STATISTICA. Clustering Techniques. Case Study: Defining Clusters of Shopping Center Patrons. and

Running Head: HUMAN RESOURCE PRACTICES AND ENTERPRISE PERFORMANCE. Pakistan. Muzaffar Asad. Syed Hussain Haider. Muhammad Bilal Akhtar

Bridging Micro and Macro Domains: Workforce Differentiation and Strategic Human Resource Management

How Firms Learn From the Uses of Different

The Academic and Co-Curricular Experiences of UCSD Freshmen Students Evidence from the Your First College Year Survey

THE ROLE OF MARKETING IN MULTINATIONAL SUBSIDIARIES: STANDARDIZATION VERSUS LOCALIZATION

LEARNING OUTCOMES FOR THE PSYCHOLOGY MAJOR

Jay Caughron, Ph.D. Caughron, J. J Whistler Rd. Radford University

Studying Gender and Ethnic Differences in Participation in Math, Physical Science, and Information Technology

Knowledge Management & E-Learning

STANDARDS FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE WITH GROUPS. Second Edition

BEST PRACTICES IN UTILIZING 360 DEGREE FEEDBACK

Chapter 5. Summary, Conclusions, and Recommendations. The overriding purpose of this study was to determine the relative

Elements of Strategic Management Process and Performance Management Systems in U.S. Federal Agencies: Do Employee Managerial Levels Matter?

HUMAN RESOURCES DEVELOPMENT NEEDS IN HIGHER EDUCATION

Turnover on Information Security Professionals: Findings from Korea

Transcription:

PATTERNS OF LEADER CHARACTERISTICS: IMPLICATIONS FOR PERFORMANCE AND DEVELOPMENT Michael D. Mumford* University of Oklahoma Stephen J. Zaccaro Julie F. Johnson Marisa Diana George Mason University Management Research Institute Janelle A. Gilbert California State University at San Bernardino Management Research Institute K. Victoria Threlfall American Institutes for Research Management Research Institute Current theories of leadership suggest that certain types of individuals are especially likely to succeed as leaders in certain organizations. The intent in the present article was to first identify types or subgroups of individuals entering the U.S. Army based on ability, personality, and motivational characteristics, and subsequently identify which types tend to be found in upper level positions. A Ward and Hook cluster analysis revealed seven types in a sample of junior Army officers. Three of these types are also particularly prominent among upper level Army leadership positions. The seven types displayed some differences in skills and patterns of career development. Based on these findings, it was concluded that organizations should recognize the need for different types of people to fill different types of organizational leadership roles. * Direct all correspondence to: Michael D. Mumford, Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, 455 West Lindsey St., Room 740, Norman, OK 73019; e-mail: mmumford@ou.edu. Leadership Quarterly, 11(1), 115 133. Copyright 2000 by Elsevier Science Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. ISSN: 1048-9843

116 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 INTRODUCTION Students of leadership have typically adopted the notion that certain traits can influence leader emergence and leader performance (Hogan, Curphy, & Hogan, 1994; Lord, DeVader, & Alliger, 1986). For example, there is now good reason to suspect that traits such as intelligence, dominance, extroversion, and achievement motivation consistently relate to leadership (Bass, 1990; McClelland & Boyzatis, 1982). This apparently straightforward conclusion, however, poses a new and more subtle question: How do traits act to shape leader development? One potential answer to this question may be found in traditional trait or variableoriented models. Here, dispositional characteristics are seen as discrete entities acting to condition certain responses to certain kinds of events. In fact, Connelly, Gilbert, Zaccaro, Threlfall, Marks, and Mumford (2000) have provided some support for this approach, showing that general cognitive ability and personality make unique contributions to the prediction of leader performance. In contrast to this variable-oriented approach, many theorists have argued that the influence of dispositional characteristics on performance is better understood using a person-centered approach (Magnusson, 1988; Mumford & Owens, 1984). The intent in the present study was, broadly speaking, to examine some of the implications of this personcentered approach for understanding leader development and performance. Leader Types and Job Matches The person-centered or typological model is not a new approach to understanding work behavior. As Borman (1991) and Katzell (1994) point out, matching models, especially person-centered or type approaches, have received substantial attention in recent years. For example, Barrett (1995) and Cable and Judge (1994) have shown that the congruence of a pattern of dispositional characteristics with environmental demands can contribute to performance prediction. Other work by Gustafson and Mumford (1995) suggests that certain types of people may need a particular environment in which to develop, while others develop regardless of the environmental conditions with which they are presented. Person-job matching models also have a long history in studies of leadership. One of the earliest efforts along these lines may be found in the work of Fiedler and his colleagues (e.g., Fielder, 1964; Fiedler & Garcia, 1987). Fiedler has proposed that types of leaders can be identified in terms of their characteristic patterns of relations to others, as assessed using the least preferred coworker scale. High and low scores on the least preferred coworker scale are in turn linked to differences in the kinds of situations in which leaders are likely to perform well, and the kind of behavior required for successful leadership. In recent years, two major models of leader types have appeared that attempt to provide explanations for the emergence and consequences of type membership. One of those models, which will be referred to as the homogeneity model, was proposed by Schneider and his colleagues (Schneider, 1987; Schneider, Goldstein, & Smith, 1995; Schneider & Schneider, 1994). Essentially, Schneider has argued that an organization s founder creates an environment consistent with his or her pattern of personality characteristics. Similar individuals are attracted to this culture, and

Leader Types 117 organizations tend to select and retain those who are good matches. This model implies that a limited set of types, perhaps only one type, will eventually come to occupy senior leadership roles. In contrast, other scholars have argued for role diversity models (e.g., Hart & Quin, 1993). Essentially, diversity models hold that organizations present different kinds of niches or roles. People with different characteristics will be attracted to and perform differently in these different roles. Accordingly, this model hypothesizes that a number of different types of leaders will be found in senior leadership positions in accordance with the need of the organization to fill diverse leadership roles. Fit Processes It would be desirable to have more clear-cut evidence indicating the applicability of these models in the description of organizational leadership. Even bearing this important caveat in mind, both the homogeneity and the diversity models pose another significant question: How does fit, or the lack thereof, operate in shaping leader behavior? One potential answer to this question may be found in the work of Holland (1973, 1985). Holland, along with a number of other scholars, has argued that fit influences people s careers and career outcomes primarily through affective mechanisms. In other words, individuals tend to be attracted to organizations or roles consistent with their personalities because they find the perceived goals and rewards attractive given their broader patterns of dispositional characteristics. Organizations or organizational roles which provide these desired rewards are typically perceived as satisfying, leading the individual to remain in this area of endeavor (Dawis, 1991). Another potential explanation for these kinds of fit effects has been proposed by Mumford and his colleagues (e.g., Mumford & Owens, 1984; Mumford, Snell, & Reiter-Palmon, 1994; Mumford, Stokes, & Owens, 1990; Stokes, Mumford, & Owens, 1989). They have argued that people who have characteristics consistent with demands of certain roles not only find activities in these roles rewarding, they actively seek other consistent or congruent situations. As part of this self-propagating pattern of situational choice, individuals will begin to acquire the knowledge and skills needed for effective performance within certain roles or classes of situations. Thus, fit effects may be as much a result of skill development and performance as they are a result of the person s affective reaction to the situation. In their study of managerial careers, Bray, Campbell, and Grant (1974), provide some support for this performance-based explanation for fit effects. They found that enlargers, or people seeking diverse new experiences, tended to prosper in managerial careers, showing growth in assessed skills. Meanwhile, enfolders, or people withdrawing into the familiar, showed less skill growth and had less successful careers, even when initial ability was held constant. It is possible, however, that these kinds of performance-based fit effects may apply for some types, while affect may drive the actions of other types (Mumford, Snell, & Hein, 1993). In other words, certain explanations of fit effect may apply for some types of leaders, but not for others.

118 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 The intent in the present investigation was to address three central issues posed by the foregoing observations. First, by examining leaders across several levels of leadership in an organization, one goal was to determine whether certain types of leaders are more prevalent at senior versus junior organizational levels. Second, based on the number and nature of the types most prevalent at upper level leadership positions, the hope was to draw some conclusions about whether homogeneity or selective diversity models provide a more appropriate basis for describing fit effects among organizational leaders. Third, using the measures of leadership skills developed by Zaccaro, Mumford, Connelly, Marks, and Gilbert (2000), skill acquisition and indices of leadership performance are examined for the different leader types at both the junior and senior levels. METHOD Sample The sample used to test these hypotheses was obtained as part of a larger study of leadership skills among U.S. Army officers. The sample used in this study has been described in some detail by Zaccaro et al. (2000). For the purpose of the present investigation, however, the larger sample was divided into two subsamples. The first subsample contained 821 second lieutenants, first lieutenants, and junior captains all officers at the beginning of their careers. The second subsample contained 426 senior captains, majors, lieutenant colonels, and colonels. Person Measures As detailed in the Zaccaro et al. study (2000), the members of both samples were asked to complete a battery of standard psychometric measures. The first set of measures examined various motivational and personality characteristics known to be related to leadership. Achievement and dominance motives were described using the pertinent scales included in the Jackson Personality Research Form (Jackson, 1989). A measure of commitment to social systems was drawn from Gough s (1975, 1987) California Psychological Inventory, while the NEO inventory provided a scale intended to measure openness (McCrae & Costa, 1985). Sample members were also asked to complete the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Briggs & Myers, 1987) which was scored for each of the relevant subclasses (e.g., introversion, extroversion, thinking, feeling, sensing, perception, intuition, and judgment). These measures are known to have reliabilities in the.80s, while evidencing adequate construct validity. In addition to these measures of personality and motivational characteristics, sample members were also asked to complete a battery of ability tests, including measures of verbal reasoning and writing skill. Details on these instruments are described in Zaccaro et al. (2000). Criterion Measures A second set of measures administered to members of the two samples was intended to provide a set of scales for assessing leader performance and is briefly

Leader Types 119 described here. Readers are again referred to Zaccaro et al. (2000) for additional details on the construction, reliability, and validity of these measures. The first criterion measure consisted of a series of self-report career history items intended to appraise distinctive career achievements (e.g., medals won, citations awarded). Kilcullen (1993) has provided evidence for the validity and accuracy of these kind of verifiable career achievement items. The second criterion measure also relied on a self-report format. Here, officers were asked to write brief descriptions or critical incidents describing their best performance over the course of the last year on four major dimensions of leadership identified by Fleishman, Mumford, Zaccaro, Korotkin, Levin, and Hein (1991): information gathering, information use, managing people, and managing things. Three judges, all doctoral candidates, were asked to rate the quality of each of these incidents and the average scores of judges across the four dimensions provided the incident scores used in the present effort. It is of note that these ratings yielded agreement coefficients in the.70s within this sample of officers. A third criterion measure was intended to reflect the quality of solutions to open-ended military problems. On this measure, leaders were presented with two novel, ill-defined problem-solving military scenarios. The resulting two to three paragraph solutions to these open-ended scenarios were then evaluated by three doctoral candidates to assess the quality of the responses. These ratings were made in accordance with the procedures recommended by Baughman and Mumford (1995) and yielded interrater agreement coefficients in the.70s. Skill Measures The final group of measures, the skill measures, were intended to assess knowledge, problem-solving skills, and solution characteristics held to contribute to leader performance and problem solving (Zaccaro et al., 2000). The measure of complex problem-solving skills, presented a novel, ill-defined military leadership problem in which officers were asked to respond to a series of probe questions intended to elicit key skills involved in creative problem solving (e.g., problem construction and information encoding; Mumford, Mobley, Uhlman, Reiter-Palmon, & Doares, 1991). The measure of social judgment was intended to assess attributes related to social and systems perception as identified by Connelly, Marks, and Mumford (1993), including self-objectivity, reflection, systems perception, systems commitment, solution fit assessment, and judgment under conditions of uncertainty. This measure was based on two ill-defined organizational problems in which a problem that could have been averted occurred in solution implementation. The officers were asked to indicate why the problem occurred, what could have been done differently, and what they would have done in the given circumstances. The third measure, examining solution construction, presented two novel, illdefined military problems in which officers were asked to identify the key aspects of the problem, the information they would need to address it, and the issues they would need to consider. The officers responses were then to be assessed with respect to time span, attention to restrictions, self goals, and organizational goals. The fourth measure examined leader knowledge or expertise using a Q-sort proce-

120 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 dure where leaders were asked to group Army leadership tasks into a set of categories. The resulting categories and the assignment of tasks to these categories were assessed with respect to (1) use of principles, (2) organization of categories, (3) categories mapping onto research-based taxonomies of leader behavior, (4) coherence of task assignment to the categories, and (5) complexity, as indicated by the number of categories proposed. With the exception of the number of categories proposed in the knowledge measure, all responses to the probe questions of the performance and skill measures were assessed using ratings. These ratings were obtained from three judges, with different pools of judges being used to obtain different ratings. On average, these judges, again all doctoral candidates, provided interrater agreement coefficients above.70 after training. Evidence for the construct and predictive validity of these ratings, and more detailed descriptions of the measures and rating dimensions have been provided by Zaccaro et al. (2000). In a separate analysis, Mumford, Marks, Connelly, Zaccaro, and Reiter-Palmon (2000) used these scales to assess changes in requisite skills from lower to higherlevel leadership positions. To accomplish this, they began by showing that scores on all these skills increased from junior-level positions (e.g., second lieutenant, first lieutenants, and junior captains) to mid-level positions (e.g., senior captains and majors) and from mid-level to more senior positions (e.g., lieutenant colonels and colonels). In a subsequent set of analyses, function scores were formulated using the scaled subscores on each measure by comparing high-performing junior officers to mid-level officers, and high-performing mid-level officers to more senior officers. The resulting discriminant function scores provided a set of cross-sectional skill change measures that might be used to examine changes in leadership skills at increasingly higher organizational levels. It is of note in this regard that scores on these skill change measures do not appear to be strongly distorted by the biases common in cross-sectional studies (Mumford, Marks, et al., 2000). The procedures used in forming these groups were explicitly intended to control for the effects of prior performance by contrasting high-performing junior officers with more senior officers using above average scores on the career achievement measure the measure likely to be most closely related to promotion criteria. Analyses Initially, leader types were defined in the junior officer sample. Defining types using only lieutenants and junior captains was based on two considerations. First, these officers were in the beginning of their careers, providing an appropriate point for comparing the survival of types at the senior level. Second, the stability of this type structure could be assessed by determining whether more senior officers could be assigned to the junior officer type categories. Types were identified in the junior officer sample using the procedures recommended by Owens and his colleagues (e.g., Brush & Owens, 1979; Mumford, Stokes, & Owens, 1990; Owens & Schoenfeldt, 1979). First, each sample member s profile of scores on the personality, motivation, and ability measures was obtained. A d 2 index was used to assess the similarity of these profiles, and groups of more

Leader Types 121 or less similar individuals were identified by entering the resulting distance matrix into a Ward and Hook (1963) clustering. The Ward and Hook procedure is an iterative, hierarchical clustering procedure that begins by treating each individual as a type unto himself/herself. The two most similar types are then combined, a mean profile formed, and the intergroup distance is recalculated. This process is repeated until all individuals have been merged into distinct groups. The number of groups, or types, to be retained is determined by identifying the point at which further combinations result in a sharp increase in within-group heterogeneity. After the number of clusters or types to be retained has been identified, mean profiles for each type are obtained and used as seed points for a non-hierarchical k-means analysis. This procedure serves as a control for drift in early assignment into groups and provides the final assignment of individuals to types (Owens & Schoenfeldt, 1979). A discriminant analysis was then conducted in which final type assignment served as the criterion, and scores on the personality, motivation, and ability measures served as the predictors. The resulting discriminant functions were then used as a basis for assigning higher-level officers (senior captains, majors, lieutenant colonels, and colonels) to the types identified in the junior officer validation sample, based on their scores on the personality, motivation, and ability measures used in initial type identification. After a stable set of clusters had been identified in the junior and senior officer samples, the primary analyses of concern in the present investigation were conducted. The first set of analyses was intended to provide basic descriptive data concerning the types that appeared in the junior and senior officer samples. Here, the nature of each type was assessed by identifying personality, motivation, and ability measures that yielded mean differences in excess of half of the pooled withingroup standard deviations for those measures in the junior officer validation sample. It is of note that this mean differences procedure rather than significance testing was used in assessing type characteristics due to the fact that clustering intentionally induces wide variation in cluster cell size and different levels of within-group variance across clusters (Owens & Schoenfeldt, 1979; Schmidt, 1996). Based on the differences observed in this analysis, substantive interpretation was formulated describing the key characteristics of each type score with a brief label summarizing this interpretation. Having described the officer types, the next set of analyses examined whether certain types of leaders found in the junior officer sample would be likely to be more prevalent than others in the senior officer sample. Here, the number of junior and senior officers assigned to each type in the discriminant analyses was determined. Additionally, the percentage of junior and senior officers assigned to each type was assessed relative to the total number of junior and senior officers assigned to the validation sample. This information was used to examine changes in type composition as a function of experience in Army leadership roles. In the final set of analyses, an attempt was made to assess the relationship between changes in type composition and performance, as well as changes in the skills held to influence performance. To provide some initial evidence along those lines, mean scores on each of the performance measures were obtained for those members of the junior and senior officer validation samples assigned to each type.

122 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 Additionally, mean differences were assessed on the skill change functions identified by Mumford, Marks, et. al. (2000). RESULTS Cluster Analysis Inspection of the plot of incremental within-group variation indicated that a nine-cluster solution should be retained. This solution appeared to provide the smallest possible number of relatively homogeneous clusters. In the k-means analysis, it was found that the first seven clusters accounted for 96% of the sample, with the largest cluster containing 166 officers while the smallest cluster contained 67 officers. The two remaining clusters contained 26 and 10 officers, respectively. Because these two clusters were too small to permit meaningful analyses, they were dropped. Thus, in the discriminant analysis, scores on the personality, motivation, and ability measures were used to assign individuals to only the first seven types. In the discriminant analysis, it was found that 91% of the officers were assigned (p.80) to the same cluster, or type, to which they were assigned in the k-means analysis. More centrally, when the discriminant functions were used to assign junior officers in the cross-validation sample to these types, a classification rate of 88% was obtained. Thus, this seven-cluster solution appeared to provide a stable description of the types found in the junior officer sample. When this discriminant function was used to assign senior officers to the seven types, 92% of the senior officers in the validation sample and 93% of the senior officers in the cross-validation sample could be unambiguously assigned (p.80) to a cluster. Thus, it appears that this same set of clusters provided a stable description of the senior officer, as well as the junior officer sample. Type Characteristics Table 1 presents, for each of the seven groups identified in the cluster analysis, the personality, motivation, and ability measures that yielded mean differences in excess of half of the pooled within-group standard deviations for junior officers. Members of the first type obtained high scores on achievement motivation and planning, but low scores on measures of openness, verbal reasoning, intuition, and perception. This pattern of characteristics paints a picture of a concrete, pragmatic, achievement-oriented leader, prompting us to label this type Concrete Achievers. The second type to emerge in this analysis painted a picture consistent with stereotypical conceptions of leadership. Members of this type were extroverted, responsible, dominant, and achievement motivated. In addition, they obtained high scores on the verbal reasoning measures and two of the measures of writing skills, generation, and revision. Low scores occurred on measures of intuition, feeling, and perception. This externally focused, rather social achievement orientation suggested that type members might be aptly described by the label Motivated Communicators. The third and fourth types obtained high scores on introversion. For members of the third type, this pattern of characteristics was accompanied by low reasoning and written ability scores resulting in the label Limited Defensives. The fourth type,

Leader Types 123 Table 1. Summary of Type Characteristics Type Label a Pattern of Scores on Ability, Personality, and Motivation Variables Concrete Achievers High Achievement, Planning Low Intuition, Perception, Openness, Verbal Reasoning Motivated Communicators High Extroversion, Responsibility, Achievement, Dominance, Verbal Reasoning, Generation, Revision Low Intuition, Feeling, Perception Limited Defensives High Introversion, Sensing, Thinking, Judging Low Intuition, Verbal Reasoning, Planning, Revision Disengaged Introverts High Introversion, Intuition, Perception, Planning, Generation Low Responsibility, Achievement, Dominance, Extroversion Social Adaptors High Extroversion, Feeling, Perception, Openness, Verbal Reasoning Low Thinking, Judging, Sensing Struggling Misfits High None Low Introversion, Intuition, Thinking, Judging, Openness, Verbal Reasoning, Planning, Generation, Revision Thoughtful Innovators High Introversion, Intuition, Thinking, Achievement, Dominance, Openness, Verbal Reasoning, Planning, Generation, Revision Low Sensing, Feeling Note: a High and Low indicates mean differences in excess of half of a standard deviation from junior officers as a group. in contrast, obtained high scores on two measures of written skills, planning and revision, but low scores on virtually all of the motivational measures, including responsibility, dominance and achievement motivation. Based on the manifest lack of motivation, members of the fourth type were labeled Disengaged Introverts. The fifth type identified in the Ward and Hook clustering had high verbal reasoning scores. This apparent intelligence was accompanied by openness and extroversion, suggestive of an open, effective pattern of social interaction. In keeping with the interpretation, type members obtained high scores on the feeling and perceiving scores of the Myers-Briggs, but low scores on the thinking, judging, and sensing scales. Accordingly, members of this type were labeled Social Adaptors. Members of the sixth type failed to obtain high scores on any of the personality, motivation, or ability measures. Moreover, they obtained particularly low scores on the cognitive scales, both verbal and writing skills. Low scores were also obtained on measures of openness, thinking, intuition, judgment, and introversion. Based on the manifest lack of ability and openness, members of this type were described as Struggling Misfits. In contrast, the members of the final type obtained high scores on both of the cognitive measures, as well as two measures of motivation, achievement, and dominance. This manifest motivation and capability, however, was accompanied by an inward, rather creative, and intellectual orientation as indicated by type

124 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 Table 2. Proportion of Type Members in Junior and Senior Positions Jr. Officers Sr. Officers Types N N (%) N N (%) Concrete Achievers 161 20 30 11 Motivated Communicators 136 17 111 40 Limited Defensives 118 15 12 4 Disengaged Introverts 83 10 16 6 Social Adaptors 100 12 30 10 Struggling Misfits 103 13 3 1 Thoughtful Innovators 85 11 73 26 members high scores on the introversion, intuition, thinking, and openness scales, but low scores on the sensing and feeling scales. As a result, the label Thoughtful Innovators was used to described the leader type with this pattern of characteristics. Type Survival One of the central issues investigated in the present study is whether or not certain types are more likely than others to exist in senior leadership positions. Table 2 presents the number of officers in the junior and senior officer samples assigned to each type, along with the proportion of sample members drawn from each type. All seven types had substantial representation in the junior officer sample with at least 10% and no more than 20% of the sample being drawn from a single type. Bearing in mind this uniform distribution, the Concrete Achievers (20%) and Motivated Communicators (17%) were the two largest types. In the senior officer sample, however, a different pattern of type assignments emerged. Few Limited Defensives, Disengaged Introverts, and Struggling Misfits were identified in the senior officer sample. In fact, in no case did members of these types account for more than 6% of the senior officer sample. Apparently, types lacking the motivation or the ability or both needed in senior leadership positions are less likely to exist in such positions. Along similar lines, fewer Concrete Achievers were observed in the senior officer sample (11%) than in the junior officer sample (20%), a finding suggesting that at least some individuals commonly attracted to an organization may not have the characteristics needed to progress to more senior leadership roles. Three types, the Motivated Communicator, Social Adaptors, and Thoughtful Innovators appeared with greater or equal frequency in the senior officer sample. Of these three types, the Social Adaptors occurred in roughly equal proportions in both the junior (12%) and senior (10%) officer samples. However, the proportion of Motivated Communicators (17% vs. 40%) and Thoughtful Innovators (11% vs. 26%) increased in moving from the junior to senior officer samples. Not only do these findings suggest that certain types of people tend to hold more senior leadership roles, they indicate that rather different types of people may be found in those roles. More specifically, the Motivated Communicators displayed a pattern of characteristics consistent with a stereotypical view of leaders being

Leader Types 125 extroverted and achievement oriented. In contrast, the Thoughtful Innovators were introverted, apparently more intellectually oriented individuals who were less focused on status and more concerned with social systems than were the Motivated Communicators. Type Performance and Skill Development Another question arises at this point: Why do certain types of people tend to move into more senior leadership roles? One explanation for this pattern of effects is that people remain and progress in organizations or organizational roles that are consistent with their needs and values. Alternatively, one might argue that the progression of certain types is due to performance and the acquisition of requisite leadership skills. Some evidence bearing on this question may be found in Table 3, which presents the mean scores of type members on each of the performance measures. The means presented in Table 3 provide some support for the notion that the movement of certain types into more senior leadership roles is contingent on performance. The types that had relatively few representatives in senior positions, the Limited Defensives, Disengaged Introverts, and Struggling Misfits, had lower averaged scores on measures of the quality of solutions to novel, ill-defined military problems than types such as the Motivated Communicators and Thoughtful Innovators, who had a larger number of representatives in senior leadership positions. It is of note that this pattern of differences was evident when type members were compared in both junior and more senior leadership positions. This pattern of performance differences was not restricted to measures of leaders capability in solving military problems. The critical incidents measure provided selfreport descriptions of past performance, while the leader achievement measure asked leaders to report objective, verifiable achievements (e.g., medals won). As both junior and senior officers, the Motivated Communicators and Thoughtful Innovators obtained higher scores on the critical incidents and leader achievement measures than the Limited Defensives, Disengaged Introverts, and Struggling Misfits. Thus, it appears that leader types entering the more senior leadership roles were those who had evidenced better performance in junior-level positions, and indicated enhanced performance in the senior positions as well. Connelly et al. (2000) found leadership skills to be significantly related to indices of leader performance. Thus, it is possible that the differences observed among types with respect to their performance may also be skill-related. Table 4 presents the scores of type members on the skill change functions identified by Mumford, Marks, et al. (2000). The clearest finding to emerge in this analysis pertains to skill change for those types that were found to be poor performers. The Limited Defensives, Disengaged Introverts, and Struggling Misfits all displayed below average scores on the skill change functions obtained when contrasting high-performing junior officers with mid-level officers and high-performing mid-level officers with senior officers. Thus, poor performance is accompanied by below average increases in problem-solving, solution construction, and social judgment skills, as well as leader expertise. With

126 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 Table 3. Mean Performance Measure Scores by Types in Junior and Senior Officer Samples Solution Quality Critical Incidents Leader Achievement Junior Senior Junior Senior Junior Senior Types M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Concrete Achievers 2.40.58 3.04.62.52.94 1.23 1.32 24.59 9.15 38.48 6.86 Motivated Communicators 2.60.40 3.05.61 1.14 1.18 1.54 1.09 27.44 9.16 39.25 5.19 Limited Defensives 2.19.64 2.44.59.54.77.17.29 24.51 8.77 35.92 4.44 Disengaged Introverts 2.29.65 2.82.62.48.71.82 1.17 20.80 9.23 36.31 4.70 Social Adaptors 2.52.53 2.94.70.80 1.06 1.67 1.28 25.09 9.21 37.07 5.82 Struggling Misfits 2.23.53 2.54.60.87 1.06 24.45 8.69 36.67 Thoughtful Innovators 2.71.51 3.20.60.97 1.12 1.85 1.13 26.68 9.09 40.42 6.29 Note: indicates N insufficient for analyses.

Leader Types 127 Table 4. Mean Discriminant Function Skill Change Scores by Type Problem Solving Solution Construction Social Judgment Leader Expertise Junior to Mid-level Junior to Mid-level Junior to Mid-level Junior to Mid-level Types Mid-level to Senior Mid-level to Senior Mid-level to Senior Mid-level to Senior Concrete Achievers.03.09.08.15.09.19.10.26 Motivated Communicators.23.06.21.02.20.05.03.09 Limited Defensives.16 1.30.79 1.18.44 1.54.22.89 Disengaged Introverts.07.30.30.65.73.98.27.49 Social Adaptors.14.15.04.36.02.28.13.09 Struggling Misfits.07.58.24.66 Thoughtful Innovators.11.25.47.41.17.02.41.22 Note: indicates N insufficient for analyses. Negative scores indicate below average performance.

128 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 the exception of problem-solving skills across junior and mid-level positions, the Social Adaptors also showed below average increases in these skills. The Concrete Achievers, although not as gifted as the Social Adaptors, did show above average increases in skill levels in leader expertise (M.26) and solution construction skills (M.15) across mid-level and more senior positions. However, increases on the other skills, particularly early at junior and mid-levels, was average or slightly below average. In contrast, the two types of leaders who were found in more senior positions evidenced large skill increases. The Motivated Communicators showed their largest skill increase in moving from junior to mid-level positions, particularly with respect to problem-solving (M.23), solution construction skills (M.21), and social judgment skills (M.20). The Thoughtful Innovators also evidenced skill increases in moving from junior to mid-level positions, producing above average scores on the change functions constructed for problem-solving skills (M.11), solution construction skills (M.47), social judgment skills (M.17), and leader expertise (M.41). In contrast to the Motivated Communicators, the Thoughtful Innovators continued to increase their skill levels in moving from mid-level to more senior positions. In moving from mid-level to more senior positions, the Thoughtful Innovators obtained above average scores on the functions exhibiting increases in problemsolving skills (M.25), solution construction (M.41) and leader expertise (M.22). One implication of this pattern of findings is that different types of leaders may display different patterns of skill development. One might, moreover, argue that the progressive skill development observed for the thoughtful innovators suggests that in the long run, they may prove more successful than the Motivated Communicators in functioning within the high-level demands commonly associated with executive leadership positions (Zaccaro, 1996). DISCUSSION Before further consideration of the foregoing observations, several limitations of the present study should be mentioned. First, one limitation of this study is its cross-sectional design, for it limits the extent to which conclusions can be drawn towards the true progression from junior-level leadership to the senior level. Second, the examination of leaders and leader types was over a substantial but nonetheless limited range of organizational levels. It might be argued that if the present study had been extended to more senior general officer level positions, the Thoughtful Innovators may have become more predominant than the Motivated Communicators. The present study cannot directly address this or other related effects of not including executive-level leaders in the sample. The findings speak more to leadership and leader types, as they are manifest in junior, mid-level, and senior positions. It is possible that somewhat different results might have emerged if executive, general officer positions had been examined. Along related lines, it should be recognized that the findings with regard to leader types were obtained in a single organization, the U.S. Army. Although the U.S. Army has a strong culture associated with clearly articulated values, it differs from many organizations in that it tends to have an up or out policy. This policy,

Leader Types 129 in turn, may have resulted in stronger effects for type progression than would be the case in other organizations. The existence of this policy, however, provides a more appropriate framework for assessing the effects of person type on leader progression. Finally, it should be recognized that the types identified in the present study do not provide an absolute description of the kind of person-types found in organizational leadership positions. The types identified in this study were classified in a single organization. These types, moreover, were developed empirically using a limited set of personality, motivation, and ability measures. Thus, it can be expected that other types might be identified in different organizations using different measures. Although it is important to bear these limitations in mind, it is believed that the present study has some important implications for understanding leadership in general and the application of person-job matching models in particular. Earlier, the literature review revealed two general matching models: the homogeneity model (e.g., Schneider, Goldstein, & Smith, 1995) and the role diversity model (e.g., Hart & Quin, 1993). Broadly speaking, the results seem to provide some support for the role diversity model, given that multiple leader types, in particular the Thoughtful Innovators and Motivated Communicators, were found in more senior leadership positions at proportions greater than the proportions characterizing these types in more junior positions. Moreover, the types that evidenced higher proportions in more senior leadership positions did seem to display a pattern of characteristics consistent with the demands of Army leadership roles. The Motivated Communicators present a pattern of characteristics consistent with the demands of operational unit leadership roles, which tend to be more tactical in nature. The Thoughtful Innovators, however, presented a pattern of characteristics more consistent with the demand of staff planning roles, which are more strategic in nature. Thus, the type of leaders who remain seem to have a pattern of characteristics consistent with the demands of different primary leadership roles with different types of leaders being retained to support different role demands. Although the findings appear to provide some support for the role diversity hypothesis, a cautionary note seems in order. As noted above, leaders were examined only in a limited number of roles. Given this point, and the fact that the Thoughtful Innovators were exhibiting greater skill increases in more senior positions of their careers, it is possible that greater homogeneity might be observed in executive, general officer positions. Thus, further research seems needed to confirm the initial support for the role diversity model. In this regard, however, it is important to bear in mind another set of findings that emerged in the present study. Diversity models typically stress role performance as the basis for leader progression as opposed to affective fit to the organization. Although the present study did not examine perceptions of and relations to climate and culture, it was found that senior leader types in the organization tended to display above average performance and more rapid skill development. This pattern of findings suggests that the retention of certain types may be based on their capability to develop the skills needed to perform successfully in certain key leader-

130 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 ship roles as suggested by Mumford, Stokes, and Owens (1990) and Stokes, Mumford, and Owens (1989). One implication of this finding is that person-job matching models may need to be extended to take into account performance and skill development as well as affective reactions to the organizational environment. Alternatively, performance and skill development may serve as key variables mediating between environmental perceptions and subsequent skill acquisition and eventual performance in organizational leadership roles (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). Although there is a need to attend to these alternative explanations, the overall pattern of results obtained in the present investigation appears to argue for a skill acquisition, performance explanation. More specifically, three types, the Disengaged Introverts, Struggling Misfits, and Limited Defensives, all failed to be substantially represented in more senior leadership positions. In examining the characteristics of these types, it was apparent that they either lacked requisite motivation, as was the case for the Disengaged Introverts, or lacked requisite ability, as was the case for the Struggling Misfits. Types lacking the ability and motivation needed for skill development (Snow, 1986) seemed to be less prevalent in the more senior leadership positions in the organization, a finding suggesting that those who are not capable of developing performance relevant skills tend to voluntarily leave or are forced to leave. These observations, however, point to another conclusion stemming from the present study. Although prior studies indicated that discrete personality, motivation and ability did not evidence strong direct relationships with leader performance (Zaccaro, Mumford, Marks, Connelly, Threlfall, Gilbert, et al., 1996), patterns of personality, motivation and ability did exert somewhat stronger effects on skill development and performance. The reason that these patterns appeared to evidence somewhat stronger effects is illustrated by comparing those types who failed to attain more senior positions, the Disengaged Introverts, Struggling Misfits and Limited Defensives, with types such as the Concrete Achievers and Social Adaptors, who had somewhat higher rates of movement and more senior positions. This comparison suggests that a lack of one characteristic, such as intelligence, may be compensated for over time by other characteristics such as motivation. By providing a more comprehensive assessment of compensatory capacities, these patterns or types may therefore permit better long-term prediction of leader performance (Katzell, 1994). Better long-term prediction of leader performance, however, is not the only practical implication of this kind of typological model. Models of this sort remind us that individuals act and develop as holistic, somewhat unique entities. Thus, different types of leaders may develop in different ways and at different times in response to various kinds of interventions. This point was illustrated in the differences observed between the Motivated Communicators and the Thoughtful Innovators in the nature and timing of skill development. Thus, general models of leadership and leader development, such as those proposed by Mumford, Zaccaro, Harding, Jacobs, and Fleishman (2000) and Mumford, Marks, et al. (2000), must be considered in terms of the individual s unique needs and unique pattern of characteristics in designing optimal developmental interventions intended to develop requisite leadership skills.

Leader Types 131 Acknowledgments: The research described in this article was supported by a contract to Management Research Institute (MRI) from the Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences (ARI) under the Small Business Innovation Research (SBIR) program and was also carried out at George Mason University. During this study the first author was at George Mason University. Parts of this research stem from earlier work completed at the Georgia Institute of Technology and Advanced Research Resources Organization (ARRO). The material presented in this article represents the opinions of the authors and does not necessarily represent the views of the United States Army or the Department of Defense. REFERENCES Barrett, R. S. (1995). Employee selection with the performance priority survey. Personnel Psychology, 48(3), 653 662. Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass and Stodgill s handbook of leadership (3rd ed.). New York: Free Press. Baughman, W. A., & Mumford, M. D. (1995). Process-analytic models of creative capacities: Operations influencing the combination-and-reorganization process. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 37 62. Borman, W. C. (1991). Job behavior, performance, and effectiveness. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed.; vol. 2; pp. 833 872). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Bray, D. W., Campbell, R. S., & Grant, D. L. (1974). Formative years in business: A longterm AT&T study of managerial lives. New York: Wiley. Briggs, K. C., & Myers, I. B. (1987). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Form G. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Brush, D. H., & Owens, W. A. (1979). Implementation and evaluation of an assessment classification model for manpower utilization. Personnel Psychology, 32, 369 383. Cable, D. M., & Judge, T. A. (1994). Person-organization fit, job choice decisions, and organizational entry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decisions Processes, 67(3), 294 311. Connelly, M. S., Marks, M. A., & Mumford, M. D. (1993). An integrated dimensional structure of wisdom. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Arlington, VA. Connelly, M. S., Gilbert, J. A., Zaccaro, S. J., Threlfall, K.V., Marks, M. A., & Mumford, M. D. (2000). Exploring the relationship of leader skills and knowledge to leader performance. Leadership Quarterly 11(1), 65 86. Dawis, R. (1991). Vocational interests, value, and preferences. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., vol. 2; pp. 833 872). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Fiedler, F. E. (1964). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 1.). New York: Academic Press. Fiedler, F. E., & Garcia, J. E. (1987). New approaches to effective leadership: Cognitive resources and organizational performance. New York: Wiley. Fleishman, E. A., Mumford, M. D., Zaccaro, S. J., Levin, K. Y., Korotkin, A. L., & Hein, M. B. (1991). Taxonomic efforts in the description of leader behavior: A synthesis and functional interpretation. Leadership Quarterly, 2, 245 287. Gough, H. G. (1975). Manual for the California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

132 LEADERSHIP QUARTERLY Vol. 11 No. 1 2000 Gough, H. G. (1987). Administrator s guide for the California Psychology Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Gustafson, S. B., & Mumford, M. D. (1995). Personal style and person-environment fit: A pattern approach. Journal of Vocational Psychology 6, 163 188. Hart, S. L., & Quin, R. E. (1993). Roles executives play: CEO s, behavioral complexity, and firm performance. Human Relation, 46, 543 574. Hogan, R., Curphy, C. J., Hogan, J. (1994). What we know about leadership: Effectiveness and personality. American Psychologist, 49(6), 493 504. Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Holland, J. L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Jackson, D. N. (1989). Personality research form manual (3rd ed.). Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists Press. Kahn, R. L., & Byosiere, P. (1992). Stress in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed.; vol. 3; 571 650). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Katzell, R. A. (1994). Contemporary meta-trends in industrial and organizational psychology. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (3rd ed.; vol. 4; pp. 1 89). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Kilcullen, R. N. (1993). The development and use of rationally-keyed background data scales to predict leader effectiveness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, George Mason University. Lord, R. G., DeVader, C. L., & Alliger, G. M. (1986). A meta-analysis of the relationship between personality traits and leadership perceptions: An application of validity generalization procedures. Journal of Management, 14, 453 464. Magnusson, D. (1988). Individual development from an interaction perspective. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), Paths through life (pp. 1 18). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). Leadership motive pattern and long-term success in management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 737 743. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1985). The NEO personality inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Mumford. M. D., Marks, M. A., Connelly, M.S., & Zaccaro, S. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2000). Development of leadership skills: Experience and timing. Leadership Quarterly, 11(1), 87 114. Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Uhlman, C. E., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Doares, L. (1991). Process analytic models of creative thought. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91 122. Mumford, M. D., & Owens, W. A. (1984). Individuality in a developmental content: Some empirical and theoretical considerations. Human Development, 27, 84 108. Mumford, M. D., Snell, A. F., & Hein, M. B. (1993). Varieties of religious experience: Person influences on continuity and change in religious involvement. Journal of Personality, 61, 69 88. Mumford, M. D., Snell, A. F., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (1994). Personality and background data: Life history and self-concepts in an ecological system. In G. S. Stokes, M. D. Mumford, & W. A. Owens (Eds.), Biodata handbook: Theory, research, and the use of biographical information in selection and performance prediction (pp. 583 616). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Mumford, M. D., Stokes, G. S., & Owens, W. A. (1990). Patterns of life history: The ecology of human individuality. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Leader Types 133 Mumford. M. D., Zaccaro, S. J., Harding, F. D., Jacobs, T. O., & Fleishman, E. A. (2000). Leadership skills for a changing world: Solving complex social problems. Leadership Quarterly 11(1), 11 35. Owens, W. A., & Schoenfeldt, L. F. (1979). Toward a classification of persons. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 569 607. Schmidt, F. L. (1996). Statistical significance testing and cumulative knowledge in psychology: Implications for training of researchers. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 115 129. Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437 455. Schneider, B., Goldstein, H. W., & Smith, D. B. (1995). The ASA framework: An update. Personnel Psychology (Special Issue: Theory and Literature), 48(4), 747 773. Schneider, B., & Schneider, J. L. (1994). Biodata: An organizational focus. In G. S. Stokes, M. D. Mumford, & Owens, W. A. (Eds.), Biodata handbook: Theory, research, and use of biographical information in selection and performance prediction (pp. 423 450). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press. Snow, R. E. (1986). Individual differences and the design of educational programs. American Psychologist, 41, 1029 1034. Stokes, G. S., Mumford, M. D., & Owens, W. A. (1989). Life history prototypes in the study of human individuality. Journal of Personality, 49, 341 386. Ward, J., & Hook, M. E. (1963). Application of an hierarchical grouping procedure to the problem of grouping profiles. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 23, 69 81. Zaccaro, S. J. (1996). Models and theories of executive leadership: A conceptual/empirical review and integration (Rep. ARI050-GMU95). Alexandria, VA: U. S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. Zaccaro, S. J., Mumford, M. D., Connelly, M. S., Marks, M. A., & Gilbert, J. A. (2000). Assessment of leader problem-solving capabilities. Leadership Quarterly, 11(1), 37 64. Zaccaro, S. J., Mumford, M. D., Marks, M., Connelly, M. S., Threlfall, K. V., Gilbert, J., et al. (1996). Cognitive and temperament determinants of Army leadership (MRI Tech. Rep. 95-4). Bethesda, MD: Management Research Institute.