The impact of physical activity and fitness on academic achievement and cognitive performance in children



Similar documents
Research Digest. Physical Education, Physical Activity, and Academic Performance in Youth. Introduction. Physical Education and Academic Performance

Active Education Physical Education, Physical Activity and Academic Performance

Education and life long learning: summary

Physical Fitness and Academic Achievement in Third- and Fifth-Grade Students

International Educational E-Journal, {Quarterly}, ISSN , Volume-II, Issue-III, July-Aug-Sept 2013

ECONOMIC COSTS OF PHYSICAL INACTIVITY

The Effects of Moderate Aerobic Exercise on Memory Retention and Recall

The Association Between School-Based Physical Activity, Including Physical Education, and Academic Performance

What kind of work does Shape do?

How To Know Your Health

WHAT IS THE CORE RECOMMENDATION OF THE ACSM/AHA PHYSICAL ACTIVITY GUIDELINES?

PET 3463 Teaching Physical Education Final Exam

Why have new standards been developed?

Investing in Brain Health Pays Off

In structured sports programs, participation and enjoyment should be emphasized rather than competition and winning.

The impact of physical education and sport on education outcomes: a review of literature

Research Summary: Exploring the Link between Physical Activity, Fitness and Cognitive Function

Physical fitness and academic performance in middle school students

An Introduction to Exercise Science 1. Evolution of Exercise Science 5 Impetus for the Emergence of Modern Exercise Science 7

Breakfast and Cognition Review of the literature

The National Survey of Children s Health The Child

CURRICULUM OVERVIEW. Please contact your child s subject teacher or Head of Department for answers to subject related questions

Ethnic Minorities, Refugees and Migrant Communities: physical activity and health

JACK KARDYS. DIRECTOR Miami-Dade County Parks, Recreation and Open Spaces

Psychology. Academic Requirements. Academic Requirements. Career Opportunities. Minor. Major. Mount Mercy University 1

University of Michigan Dearborn Graduate Psychology Assessment Program

Physical Education Matters

Department of Veterans Affairs Health Services Research and Development - A Systematic Review

Healthy Schools Grant Application

Implications of physical activity for academic achievement. California physical education requirements. Physical Education and California Schools

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254

5TH GRADE FITNESS STUDY GUIDE

Measuring critical thinking, intelligence, and academic performance in psychology undergraduates

The Effects of Participation in Marching Band on Physical Activity and Physical Fitness in College Aged Men and Women

x 5 Africa Education Review x 6 African American Review x 7 Alberta Journal of Educational Research

Summary of health effects

Evidence-Informed Recommendations in Rehabilitation for Older Adults Aging with HIV: A Knowledge Synthesis

The Influence of Infant Health on Adult Chronic Disease

Physical Education is Critical to a Complete Education

CURRICULUM VITAE. Brendon S. Hale. 226 McCarthy Gymnasium Mississippi State, MS (662)

Kinesiology (M.S.) Kinesiology Admission. Mission. Goals

Physical Activity in the School Environment and the Community

Cognitive Behavior Group Therapy in Mathematics Anxiety

Evidence translation for effective early childhood intervention

UNLOCKING POTENTIAL: A MANIFESTO FOR PE AND SCHOOL SPORT

Education and the Brain: A Bridge Too Far John T. Bruer. Key Concept: the Human Brain and Learning

Prepared by:jane Healey ( 4 th year undergraduate occupational therapy student, University of Western Sydney

Written Example for Research Question: How is caffeine consumption associated with memory?

Sports Participation in Secondary Schools: Resources Available and Inequalities in Participation

Childhood Physical Activity Involvement in Active and Inactive Female Adults

Chapter 5: Analysis of The National Education Longitudinal Study (NELS:88)

The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment. Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households

Regence. Section: Mental Health Last Reviewed Date: January Policy No: 18 Effective Date: March 1, 2013

Curriculum Vita Molly E. K. McLaren, BS

Counseling psychologists School psychologists Industrial-organizational psychologists "Psychologist Overview"

Benefit of exercise for Parkinson s disease. Dr Florence Chang Movement disorder fellow

Social Care and Obesity

Executive Summary. Introduction. Purpose

Jill Malcolm, Karen Moir

The Adverse Health Effects of Cannabis

Fitness Testing for Sport and Exercise

Aurora University Master s Degree in Teacher Leadership Program for Life Science. A Summary Evaluation of Year Two. Prepared by Carolyn Kerkla

Improving School Sport and Physical Education in your School

RUNNING HEAD: TUTORING TO INCREASE STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT USING TUTORING TO INCREASE STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT ON END OF COURSE ASSESSMENTS. By KATHYRENE HAYES

Chronic Disease and Nursing:

Statement for the Record for the September 25, 2008, hearing entitled, Cell Phone Use and Tumors: What the Science Says

Get a move on The importance of school-based initiatives to increase children s physical activity

17/02/2015 Katie Williams Senior Industry Development Officer Exercise & Sports Science Australia Katie.williams@essa.org.au

Does Time-of-Day of Instruction Impact Class Achievement?

Requirements. Elective Courses (minimum 9 cr.) Psychology Major. Capstone Sequence (14 cr.) Required Courses (21 cr.)

Physical Education Is Critical to Educating the Whole Child

MRes Science Project Titles 2015/16 School of Health Sciences and Social Work, University of Portsmouth

Helping Children Get Started Right: The Benefits of Early Childhood Intervention

Research context and research gaps

The Association Between School-Based Physical Activity, Including Physical Education, and Academic Performance

SPORT PORTFOLIO GUIDANCE (E1) FOR PLACEMENT PROVIDERS OFFERING A WORK PLACEMENT

Planting Trees: Just What The Doctor Ordered

Make a Difference at Your School!

What are observational studies and how do they differ from clinical trials?

Exercise Adherence. Introduction to Exercise Adherence

Exercise Seen as Priming Pump for Students Academic Strides

Student Health & Wellness Sioux Falls School District Action Plan EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

The cost of physical inactivity

Master of Science. Obesity and Weight Management

Prevention Status Report 2013

9-12 Physical Education and Leisure Curriculum Framework. Revised 2011

Poster presentation School Physical Education: From the eyes of students

inflammation of the pancreas and damage to the an increased risk of hypertension, stroke and Table 7.1: Classification of alcohol consumption

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE. Corey A. Nunlist

Course description. Introductory Level Courses. EXSC1001. Foundations of exercise science (6 credits)

Cork Education and Training Board. Programme Module for. Child Psychology. leading to. Level 6 FETAC

CURRICULM VITAE AMY ROBERSON HAYES

ADHD and Math Disabilities: Cognitive Similarities and Instructional Interventions

Psychology. Undergraduate

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive

How to Develop a Sporting Habit for Life

Adult Physical Activity

APA Div. 16 Working Group Globalization of School Psychology

Physical Activity and Healthy Eating Among Children and Youth. Key Findings from the Study

BEYOND CITIUS, ALTIUS, FORTIUS A LEADERSHIP ROLE FOR THE SPORTS MEDICINE PHYSICIAN TO IMPLEMENT EXERCISE AS MEDICINE FOR POPULATION HEALTH

Transcription:

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology Vol. 2, No. 2, September 2009, 198214 The impact of physical activity and fitness on academic achievement and cognitive performance in children Thomas J.H. Keeley and Kenneth R. Fox* Department of Exercise, Nutrition and Health Sciences, The University of Bristol, UK (Received 9 March 2009; final version received 3 August 2009) The potential for physical activity and fitness to improve cognitive function, learning and academic achievement in children has received attention by researchers and policy makers. This paper reports a systematic approach to identification, analysis and review of published studies up to early 2009. A threestep search method was adopted to identify studies that used measures of physical activity or fitness to assess either degree of association with or effect on a) academic achievement and b) cognitive performance. A total of 18 studies including one randomised control trial, six quasi-experimental and 11 correlational studies were included for data extraction. No studies meeting criteria that examined the links between physical activity and cognitive function were found. Weak positive associations were found between both physical activity and fitness and academic achievement and fitness and elements of cognitive function, but this was not supported by intervention studies. There is insufficient evidence to conclude that additional physical education time increases academic achievement; however there is no evidence that it is detrimental. The quality and depth of the evidence base is limited. Further research with rigour beyond correlational studies is essential. Keywords: physical activity; physical fitness; children; young people; academic achievement; cognitive performance Introduction The physical health benefits of participating in regular physical activity and maintaining physical fitness are widely established (Department of Health, 2004, United States Department of Health and Human Services, 2008). It has been clearly demonstrated that physical activity decreases risk of developing cardiovascular disease (CVD), stroke, some cancers, obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus and is also effective in the treatment of several of these diseases. There has also been growing interest in the benefits of physical activity for mental health and a strong evidence base shows that regular activity and improved fitness increases psychological well-being (Biddle, Fox & Boutcher, 2001, Biddle & Mutrie, 2008). Exercise can help people feel better about themselves and their lives, reduce anxiety and improve mood. Evidence is also building to show that physical activity is associated with substantially reduced risks of mental illnesses and conditions such as *Corresponding author. Email: k.r.fox@bristol.ac.uk ISSN 1750-984X print/issn 1750-9858 online # 2009 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/17509840903233822 http://www.informaworld.com

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 199 depression, cognitive impairment and dementia (Fox & Mutrie, in press; Hamer & Chida, 2008). The benefits of physical activity in the treatment of depression (National Institute of Clinical Excellence, 2004) and improvement in select aspects of cognitive function in older adults are becoming increasingly well established (Angevaren, Aufdemkampe, Verhaar, Aleman, & Vanhees, 2008). Furthermore, acute bouts of well managed physical exercise may facilitate certain aspects of information processing in adults (Tomporowski, 2003). Some of these positive effects on mental health have also been shown in children and adolescents, although the evidence base is limited. Few studies have investigated the preventive or treatment effects of exercise on mental illness within this population, partly because incidence is low. However, reviews have indicated that exercise and/or sport involvement can have beneficial effects on psychological wellbeing. For example, exercise has been shown to improve physical self-perceptions and to a lesser extent self-esteem in children (Fox, 2001) although effects are inconsistent. In addition to the effects on psychological health, there has been substantial interest in the potential impact of improved fitness and exercise on cognitive function and learning in children. The notion that higher levels of activity or fitness may enhance thinking, concentration and subsequently academic performance is attractive to educators. Not only could it benefit children, it could improve the school s added value for academic achievement. For physical educators and sports coordinators, it could justify greater provision of physical activity in the school curriculum. Indeed, since the early 1990s schools have been adopting commercial programmes such as Brain Gym (www.braingym.org.uk), a system that utilises motor coordination exercises to enhance learning, despite evidence of its effectiveness. Other schemes such as Wake Up Shake Up (www.foundation-stage.info) and Energizers (www.ncpe4me.com/energizers) are also emerging in schools within the UK and the US respectively. Four literature reviews have been published on the links between physical activity and cognitive function or academic performance since 2003. In a review of 44 studies Sibley and Etnier (2003) examined the evidence for the influence of physical activity on cognition in children. 28 cross-sectional associational and 16 intervention studies, with children between the ages of four and 18 years were included. Eight different categories of cognitive assessment were identified and associations or effects of single bouts and regular participation in various forms of aerobic training, resistance training and physical education curriculum were summarised. A mean effect size of 0.32 in favour of activity was reported. This review was very inclusive with several studies being unpublished, outcome measures were diverse and some samples were children with learning difficulties where relationships might be quite different. The association between physical activity and school performance was reviewed by Taras (2005) in a paper reporting 14 studies, including an unstated number with abstract only, published between 1984 and 2004, involving participants between five and 18 years of age. In a narrative summation of the findings, showing weak or no correlation between activity level and academic performance, Taras concludes that the field requires further research to better understand the impact of activity levels upon student performance. Trudeau and Shephard (2008) presented a recent review of studies linking school time physical activity and academic performance. Nine cross-sectional studies and seven quasi-experimental studies assessing

200 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox academic performance by grade point averages (GPAs) and determinants of GPA (concentration, classroom behaviour etc), published between 1966 and 2007, were included. The review reported non-significant trends in studies and concluded that academic achievement is not affected by limiting the time allocated to PE instruction, school physical activity and sports programmes. A narrative summary of the research into the effects of physical activity on cognition in childhood was presented by Hillman, Erickson and Kramer (2008) as a short sub-section of a paper assessing the wider effects of physical activity on cognition across all age groups. The authors concluded that, from the limited amount of published research there was no indication that an increase in curriculum time physical activity is associated with a decrease in academic performance. A further narrative review is offered by Tomporowski, Davis, Miller, and Naglieri (2008), of studies of the effects of physical exercise on cognition and academic achievement. The conclusion reached by these authors is that exercise may be an important method of enhancing aspects of mental functioning that are central to cognitive development. Highlighted in this paper is the variability of the outcome of studies and poorly selected outcome measures. The authors suggest that this may be due to factors including researchers selecting populations that do not represent the general population, for example, children with mental or physical disabilities. Given the interest in the potential for physical activity in its various forms to enhance cognitive and school performance, we feel that a more systematic and rigorous approach to reviewing the literature is warranted that provides a robust and objective summary of the state of knowledge on this important topic. This is required in order to judge what is required to take the field forwards. Currently, the conclusions of existing reviews are equivocal and it is not possible to determine whether this arises from study or population selection, combining studies of different design or that address different research questions, or interpretation of findings. There are several study characteristics that require clearer segmentation including distinguishing between physical activity versus fitness effects, extra versus within curriculum activity, cognitive function versus academic achievement, and perhaps short term or acute versus long term effects. This review has attempted to address some of these issues by taking a carefully delineated approach to reporting the current published literature (until February 2009). Studies investigating academic performance and cognitive function as outcome variables are assessed separately, as are those taking physical activity and physical fitness as exposure variables. Furthermore cross sectional studies are separately assessed from intervention studies and greater precision in the definition of variables is attempted. Summaries are confined to outcomes that have achieved the researchers set levels of statistical significance. Results are discussed in the context of the potential for this area of research and the kinds of research questions and designs that would be needed to take the field forwards. Terms of reference The following definitions are provided to maintain clarity and consistency through the remainder of the review.

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 201 Exposure/independent variables Childhood physical activity is a complex mix of behaviours that take place in diverse social settings. Consideration could be given to categories such as break-time play, active travel, sport and physical education (both within and additional to the school curriculum), informal play and sports and dance clubs outside school. This review considers contexts as well as modes of activity which might include walking, running, cycling, swimming, vigorous sports, and dance. Furthermore, consideration is given to how each of these was quantified in terms of duration, frequency and level of intensity. In contrast to physical activity, physical fitness is a complex set of functional capacities and capabilities. In children, these are partly determined by genetic factors and stage of biological maturation as well as the amount of physical activity undertaken. Often a battery of tests are used to assess components such as cardiovascular fitness, muscular strength and endurance, sometimes body composition or degree of fatness, flexibility, agility, coordination, balance and reaction time. This review includes any aspect of physical fitness when it has been assessed using a standardised test or measure to score subjects. Outcome variables Academic achievement is the child s performance when assessed by standardised tests within a school, or educational setting. Often this is quantified as achievement in specific subjects such as mathematics or reading skill, grade point average (in the US) or through standard national assessment tests (SATs). This variable is dependent on the ability of the child, their home background and environment, as well as the quality and quantity of academic instruction that child receives. Cognitive performance refers to the child s performance when assessed using a recognised and validated test of cognitive function. Tests assess components of cognition such as reaction time, attention, working memory and stimulus response (collectively referred to as executive control). Cognitive and academic performance are thought to interrelate as aspects of cognition such as attention and working memory are vital for academic success. Method Search strategies A three-step search method was used to identify studies meeting inclusion criteria that investigated the relationship between physical activity or physical fitness and cognitive performance or academic achievement. The terms, and their combinations were searched in the databases of MEDLINE, PSYCHINFO, Cochrane data base, Google Scholar and ERIC: physical activity; habitual activity; physical education; physical fitness; physical education; cognition; cognitive function; cognitive performance; cognitive health; academia; academic performance; academic achievement and academic grades. The references lists of all identified studies were also searched for titles containing any of the above terms, and all relevant studies followed up. Three selected prominent authors in the field were then contacted by email and

202 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox provided with the list of studies meeting inclusion criteria and asked to identify further relevant papers. Inclusion and exclusion criteria For inclusion within this review studies had to meet all the following criteria:. Involved a study population of children and/or adolescents, without learning disorders or special needs, aged between four and 18 years;. used one of the following research designs: randomised control trial (RCT), quasi-experimental, cross-section or longitudinal correlational;. used a measure of physical activity (objective, self-report, or teacher rated) or physical fitness as an independent or exposure variable;. used a measure of academic achievement or cognitive performance as a dependent or outcome variable; and. was published or accepted for publication in a peer reviewed journal. Data extraction A standard data extraction template was used to extract the following data from all studies meeting criteria; primary and secondary research questions, location, sample size and characteristics, study design, exposure and outcome measures, adjustment variables, results with significance levels, and conclusions. These were then used as a basis of the summary tables. Results Search results 17 studies satisfied inclusion criteria. Of these, 11 were cross-sectional correlational studies. Five studies were of quasi-experimental design. One randomised controlled trials was identified. Five studies of correlational design (Table 1a) and five studies of quasi-experimental design (Table 1b) assessed physical activity and academic achievement. Four studies of correlational design assessed physical fitness and academic achievement (Table 2). Two studies of correlational design (Table 3a) and one study of experimental design (Table 3b) assessed physical fitness and cognitive function. No study examining the links between physical activity and cognitive function were found. Physical activity and academic achievement The five studies in Table 1a were all cross-sectional and were conducted in North America, Canada, Australia, Iceland or Hong Kong. Sample sizes varied from 333 to 7691, with ages ranging between kindergarten age and 15 years old. In four studies, total daily physical activity was assessed using a children s self-reported physical activity recall questionnaire and the other featured teacher-rated physical education curriculum time. Academic performance was assessed by test and examination results and in one case by an educational professional. In three of the studies

Table 1a. Physical activity and academic achievement: correlational studies Author, (date) Location Subjects Physical activity assessment Academic assessment Primary finding Carlson et al. (2008) Sigfusdottir et al. (2006) Yu et al. (2006) Dwyer et al. (2001) Tremblay et al. (2000) USA Iceland (nationwide) Hong Kong, China Australia New Brunswick, Canada 5316 students followed from Kindergarten to 5th grade. 5810 ninth and tenth grade students 333 students aged 812 years. 7691 students aged 715 years 6923 sixth grade students Teacher reported frequency and duration of PE lessons. Tertiles formed. Self-reported physical activity. Physical activity questionnaire for older children (PAQ-C). Experimenter administered questionnaire assessing frequency, duration and intensity of previous weeks PA and usual activity Four questions assessing participation in physical activity Mathematics and reading tests. Self-reported average grades for Icelandic, Maths, English and Danish Examination/test results and teacher reported conduct grades. School representative rated each students academic ability Maths and reading test scores Third tertile girls (70300 mins of PE per week) achieved slightly higher academic scores in kindergarten, first grade and fifth grade reading and first grade mathematics compared to first tertile girls (035 mins of PE per week). A very weak positive (r0.09) and significant (pb0.01) correlation between self-reported PA and self-reported grades. PA not associated with academic achievement in either boys (r0.067, non-sig) or girls (r0.068, non-sig). PA negatively associated with school conduct in girls (r0.124, pb0.01) Weekly PA associated with scholastic rating in 9, 10 and 11 year old girls ( r range0.11 to 0.14, p rangeb0.05 to B0.01) and 9, 10 and 12 year old boys (r range0.11 to 0.17, p rangeb0.05 to B0.001). Usual lunchtime activity positively (r range0.08 to 0.18 and significantly (p rangeb0.05 tob0.001) associated with scholastic rating in boys and girls. A weak negative relationship between PA and academic achievement. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 203

Table 1b. Physical activity and academic achievement: intervention studies Author, (date) Location Subjects Intervention Academic assessment Primary finding Ahamed et al. (2007) Coe et al. (2006) Sallis et al. (1999) Shephard et al. (1984) Dwyer et al. (1983) British Columbia, Canada 288 primary school students, aged 911 Michigan, USA 214 sixth grade students Southern California, USA Trois Rivieres, Quebec South Australia 654 fifth and sixth grade students 546 first to sixth grade students 500 fifth grade students AS! BC: 16 months of an additional 47 min/wk of school time PA One semester of one 55 min lesson of PE per day Project SPARK: 2 years of an additional 2742 min/wk of PE and an additional 2729 min/ wk of self-management classes 5 hours of PE per week for five years; control 40mins per week for same time period. 2 years of 75 min/day of PE focusing either on skill or fitness. Controls maintained three classes of 30 min/week Canadian Achievement Test (CAT-3) Academic achievement grades and standardised test score Metropolitan Achievement Test (MAT6/7) Standardised exam and teacher rating. ACER arithmetic test and GAP reading test Usual practice schools had significantly (p0.001) higher scores at baseline than intervention schools, but not at follow-up. Academic achievement not affected by the one semester of increased PE. Intervention group showed significantly (p range0.02 to 0.001) smaller declines in academic performance compared to control group. Improvement in performance in English, but not maths in standardised exam. Improved teacher ratings. No significant difference in either measure of academic performance. 204 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox

Table 2. Physical fitness and academic achievement: correlational studies Author, (date) Location Subjects Physical fitness assessment Academic assessment Primary finding Eveland-Sayers et al. (2009) Castelli et al. (2007) Grissom (2005) Tennessee, USA Illinois, USA California, USA 134 third, forth and fifth grade students 259 3rd and 5th grade students 884,715 5th, 7th and 9th grades students Dwyer et al. (2001) Australia 7691 students aged 715 years 1 mile run, sit-up, sit-and-reach and BMI. Mathematics and reading/language sections of Terra Nova test A significant negative correlation between 1-mile run times and mathematics (r0.28, pb0.01) and muscular fitness and mathematics (r0.20, pb0.05). When analysed by sex difference correlations were significant in girls but not in boys. The Fitnessgram Annual ISAT test Total (r0.42, pb0.01), Maths (r0.45, pb0.45) and Reading (r0.41, pb0.01) achievement positively and significantly associated with total fitness. Total (r0.48, pb0.48), Maths (r0.49, pb0.01) and Reading (r0.45, pb0.01) achievement positively and significantly associated with cardiovascular fitness. The Fitnessgram Numerous subjective measurements and PWC170 STAR, SAT/9 and CAT/ 6 tests School representative rated each students academic ability Consistent, positive and significant association between fitness scores and academic achievement scores Consistent, positive (r range0.11 to 0.18 sacled by increasing time on 1.6 km run) and significant (p rangeb0.05 to B0.001) association between cardiovascular fitness and academic achievement. Disparities of results between subjective and objective measurements. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 205

Table 3a. Physical fitness and cognitive performance: correlational studies Author, (date) Location Subjects Buck et al. (2007) Hillman et al. (2005) Illinois, USA Illinois, USA 74 children aged 712 years 24 children mean age 9.6 years Physical fitness assessment The Fitnessgram The Fitnessgram Cognitive assessment The Stroop colour-word task A visual oddball paradigm Primary finding Aerobic fitness was positively and significantly (p0.001) associated with the word, colour and incongruent colour-word conditions of the Stroop task. Results suggest aerobic fitness was associated with better cognitive functioning and executive control. High fit children had a significantly faster reaction time (pb0.01), greater P3 amplitude (pb0.001) and shorter P3 latency (pb0.001). Results suggest greater allocation of of attention and working memory in high fit children. 206 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox Table 3b. Physical fitness and cognitive performance: intervention studies Author, (date) Location Subjects Intervention Cognitive assessment Primary finding Davis et al. (2007) Augusta, Georgia, USA 94 children, aged 711 years, mean age 9.2 years. Low-dose (20 mins) vs High-dose (40 mins) of gymnasium based exercise eliciting a Heart Rates of 150BPM. 5 days per week for 15 weeks. CAS (based) on the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous and Successive (PASS) theory. Controls showed significantly lower post-test CAS Planning score than high-dose exercise group p.01. No significant difference between control and low-dose exercise groups or low and high-dose exercise groups.

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 207 associations were reported when controlled for socio-economic status, parental education and ethnicity of the child. Results from the studies in Table 1a were mixed. The Hong Kong study, which had the smallest sample, found no association between physical activity (as measured by the Physical Activity Questionnaire for Children (PAQ-C)) and examination/test scores. The study by Carlson et al. (2008) where physical education time tertiles were compared did show significantly higher mathematics and reading scores in the high versus the low PE tertile. This effect was weak but provides some indication that more time dedicated to PE is not detrimental to academic performance. In the remaining three correlational studies weak positive relationships, with r values ranging from 0.08 to 0.18, were found. There were no consistent patterns for age and gender. In summary, these cross-sectional associational studies provide limited evidence of a weak relationship between physical activity and academic achievement. No inference can be made on the causal nature of the relationships described in these correlational studies. Table 1b details five non-randomised controlled interventions that varied in duration from six months to five years. These studies were conducted in Canada, Australia and North America. All groups were of primary school age and cohort size varied between 214 and 654. The interventions were based on sustaining higher levels of physical activity through increased PE curriculum time and in one case (Sallis et al., 1999) additional self-management classes. Therefore, in all cases the increase in PA was at the expense of academic instruction, with control schools continuing their normal curriculum provision. Increases in PE time varied from 27 minutes per week to 75 minutes per day. The measurement of children s physical activity varied from direct observation to use of the PAQ-C and the calculation of MET values. Academic achievement was assessed using standardised national tests, grades and in one case teacher ratings were added. Two studies showed that an increase in school time physical activity was not associated with a change in academic performance. One study showed significantly smaller declines in four out of the eight measures of academic performance when compared to control schools. One showed an increase in performance in Mathematics but not in English and one showed a closing of a baseline difference in academic performance which was suggestive of a positive effect for higher levels of curriculum-based physical activity. None of the studies reported a significant detrimental effect of school time physical activity on academic performance. There was no obvious dose response relationship between the degree of the increase in physical activity or the length of intervention and academic performance in the school. In summary, these studies indicate that the introduction of more curriculum time physical activity, at the expense of time allocated to academic subjects, does not have a detrimental effect on childrens academic performance. Only one study reported a significant improvement in elements of academic achievement with increased school time physical activity. Physical fitness and academic achievement The four studies, three from North America and one from Australia, included in Table 2, examined the relationship between physical fitness and academic

208 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox performance. Ages ranged from 7 to 15 years with sample sizes ranging from of 134 to 884,715. Two studies used the US Fitnessgram which includes the following tests: PACER (cardiovascular endurance), push-ups and sit-ups (muscular endurance), sit and reach (flexibility) and body mass index. In the Eveland-Sayers et al. (2009) study all but the PACER test of the US Fitnessgram battery was used, replacing this with a one-mile run. The fourth study (Dwyer, Coonan, Leitch, Hetzel, & Baghurst, 2001) used a battery of tests including standing long jump (muscular power), sit ups and push ups (muscular endurance), sit and reach (hip flexion), skinfold thicknesses (fatness), lung function, 50 meter sprint (muscular power) and 1.6 kilometer run (cardiorespiratory endurance). Three studies used standardised tests to assess academic achievement while one study used a rating of academic ability on a fivepoint scale by an adult school representative (usually the school principal). Two studies (Castelli, Hillman, Buck, & Erwin, 2007; Dwyer et al., 2001) reported controlling for socioeconomic status and parental education. Consistent moderate positive correlations between physical fitness and academic performance were seen across the studies. The strongest correlations were seen with cardiovascular fitness, with r values ranging from 0.41 when using the PACER to 0.20 using the increasing time on 1-mile run. Associations were also seen between measures of muscular force/power and flexibility and academic scores. Dwyer et al. (2001), with 7691 children, also used a laboratory bicycle-based PWC 170 that measures physical capacity at the heart rate of 170 beats per minute per kilogram of lean body mass. In contrast to the field test of cardiovascular fitness, there was no relationship found with academic performance. This raises questions about the possible confounding of motivation, confidence, or cognitive skill factors when operating field tests of cardiovascular fitness. In summary, these four studies provide evidence of a relationship between mainly cardiovascular fitness and academic performance when field tests are used as estimates of fitness. However, this was not confirmed by the only study that used a more precise laboratory-based measure of cardiovascular fitness. Physical fitness and cognitive performance Our search strategy revealed two small correlational studies (Table 3a) and one intervention study (Table 3b) conducted in the US that assessed the association between physical fitness and cognitive performance. The Fitnessgram described in the previous section was used in both the correlational studies. In the study by Buck, Hillmann, & Castelli (2007), using a sample of 74 children between the ages of 7 and 12 years, the Stroop colour word task was used to assess selective attention, response inhibition, interference control and speed response (executive control). In the Hillman, Castelli, & Buck (2005) study of 24 children, with an average age of 9.6 years, a visual oddball paradigm was adopted to assess the ability to discriminate, time for task completion, as well as P3 latency and amplitude. The P3 is part of the event-related brain potentials occurring 300800ms after stimulus and occurs when the participant is attending to and discriminating between stimuli. In both studies higher levels of aerobic fitness were associated with significantly better performance on the cognitive task. These two studies provide initial evidence of a potential relationship between aspects of fitness (mainly aerobic) and cognitive performance.

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 209 The only intervention study was conducted recently by Davis et al. (2007). They used a pre and post intervention aerobic fitness treadmill test and the CAS test, based on the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous and Successive (PASS) theory of cognitive functioning. The intervention consisted of low and high-dose gymnasium based exercise programmes, focused around group games. Children in the low-dose group exercised for 20 minutes, five days per week for 15 weeks and the children in the high-dose group exercised for 40 minutes. Both groups aimed to achieve an average heart rate of 150 bpm for each session. The high-dose exercise group differed significantly from the no additional exercise control group on the Planning aspect of the CAS test. This well-designed study provides initial evidence for an effect of exercise upon at least one aspect of executive function. It is of note that low and high-dose intervention groups did not differ significantly on the treadmill post-test, suggesting that difference in fitness gains made by the two groups were small. Factors other than fitness change may have been responsible for the difference in cognitive function. Discussion During the past 10 years, interest has grown considerably in the social, mental and educational benefits of physical activity for young people. There appears to be great potential in this area, and indeed a belief in the value of sport and activity for social and mental benefits has already underpinned considerable investment in their promotion by local and national government. Particular attention has been paid to the potential for physical activity to improve learning and academic achievement. However, the small number of published reviews that have focused on the effects of physical activity on academic and cognitive performance in young people have not produced consistent results. This review attempted to take a systematic and rigorous approach to the identification, selection, and interpretation of this body of literature. The following general conclusions can be drawn about the state of the evidence base (until the end of February 2009): 1. There are few published studies. Only 17 were identified. The reasons for this paucity of research, especially given policy interest and indications of plausible mechanisms from related areas of research (discussed later) are not clear. 2. The majority of studies are cross-sectional and correlational in design. These at best have produced weak positive associations. There are several factors including level of motivation and aspirations, cognitive skills, clustering of abilities within individuals, parental encouragement and logistic support, that could provide alternative explanations for associations between engagement in activity and sport and mental performance. Controlled intervention studies are therefore necessary to isolate cause and effect. 3. Only six intervention studies were identified. Experimental studies have focused on the effect of additional school-time physical activity on academic performance. An exception is the recent work of Davis et al. (2007) that addressed aspects of executive function. No studies primarily addressing effects of physical activity in other contexts such as the active travel, breaktime play, informal play from the home, or participation in non-school based clubs and teams were located.

210 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox 4. Exposure variables have mainly been in school activity time or performance on a battery of physical fitness tests. 5. Outcome variables have been restricted to performance on standardised academic tests, grade point average and three studies provided scores on cognitive performance tasks. Based on the existing evidence, we feel we are able to make the following statements. 1. Based on five cross sectional studies, a weak relationship may exist between total daily physical activity and academic achievement. There are several plausible alternative explanations for this relationship and no intervention studies to support it. 2. There is no consistently convincing evidence to show that increasing curricular-based physical activity improves academic achievement. However, where physical activity has replaced academic time in the curriculum in primary schools, there is no evidence of a detrimental effect on academic achievement. Given the benefits of physical activity for children s healthy growth, weight management, and general health, this could be taken as supportive evidence for a greater amount of school time spent on physical activity. However, experimental studies that compare the effects of replacing decreased academic time with physical activity or sedentary non-academic time are required before firm conclusions can be drawn. 3. Based on four cross-sectional studies, a weak relationship exists between aspects of physical fitness, primarily aerobic fitness as measured by field running tests, on elements of academic achievement. It should be noted that the only study to use a laboratory-based test of aerobic fitness did not find a relationship, and field tests are susceptible to confounding by motivational factors (Fox & Biddle, 1988). There is no experimental evidence to indicate that improving fitness will increase academic achievement. 4. Based on two cross-sectional studies, a weak relationship exists between aspects of physical fitness, primarily aerobic fitness and executive control elements of cognitive performance. This is supported by one recent welldesigned randomised controlled trial showing positive effects for a programme of additional daily exercise. As the literature stands, it is not possible to determine whether cognitive performance or academic achievement are improved by physical activity or physical fitness. Although there is weak cross-sectional evidence, there are too many other possible and plausible explanations for this relationship. Parents who support and encourage their children s academic activities are also likely to encourage them to be active. Similarly, children are likely to emulate their parents values which might include both sport and academic achievement. Further, there may be some degree of clustering of academic and athletic abilities in children. As explained earlier, academically more capable or driven children may be more motivated in particular to do well on tests of physical fitness, particularly where running for speed and distance is the measure. Children with behavioural problems are less likely to be involved or survive in school sport. Children who are unfit and inactive may reflect greater illness and school absence, and miss out on school work.

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 211 In conclusion, there is only a small amount of research published, that features sufficiently rigorous measurement, and adequate study design. Only one randomised controlled trial was located. Clearly, there has been very limited investment in research in this area. This is surprising because of policy interest in the area, and the intuitive and grass roots belief that physical activity is good for the brain. Furthermore, there are indications from other research approaches, particularly in psychobiology and neuroscience, most of it based at this stage on animal models, that there may be some important underpinning mechanisms to explain effects of exercise on cognitive function. Neurogenesis is the growth of new nerve cells in the nervous system, and provides a potential mediating mechanism by which physical activity and fitness could improve cognitive efficiency. In mice, voluntary exercise and running have been shown to stimulate hippocampal neurogenesis (van Praag et al., 2005, Brown et al., 2003). Similarly, exercise induces angiogenesis (the growth of new vascularity and hypertrophy of existing blood vessels) within the cerebral cortex of rats (Kleim, Cooper, & VandenBerg, 2002). It has been postulated that this may benefit cognitive function by allowing greater perfusion of blood through this region. It has been suggested that mechanisms such as these underpin the growing evidence base that indicates reduction in the region of 30% in subsequent risk of premature cognitive decline and dementia in older adults who have been and remain active (Fox & Mutrie, in press, Hamer & Chida, 2009). The notion that exercise helps maintain the hard wiring and blood supply in the brain seems plausible as metabolic turnover with exercise increases dramatically from rest. It seems equally plausible that exercise might stimulate neural growth and efficiency during the period of biological maturation in children. However, there is little current evidence to support this. Perhaps the most convincing evidence is provided by Winter et al. (2007) who showed through a randomised cross-over design that vigorous activity can improve brain-derived neurotropic factor, dopamine and epinephrine and these improve postexercise capacity for aspects of short term and medium term cognitive performance. However, this study was conducted with young male adults. Future research Certainly, these diverse sources of evidence suggest that high quality research should be undertaken to investigate the impact of both acute and regular physical activity on the cognitive function of children and young people, particularly during important phases of growth. There is also a case for investigating the impact of exercise on the cognitive function of those who have cerebral impairment. Our view of the work to date is that intervention designs are needed. Because of the early stage of this research, many small scale studies focusing on potential mechanisms and feasibility studies are required before more expensive trials are attempted. There is insufficient knowledge at the descriptive level to identify definitive mechanisms and interventions at this point. It would seem helpful to pay more attention to the path of potential effect. For example, before impact on academic achievement is addressed, it would seem appropriate to have some notion of the potential mechanisms by which physical activity or fitness would take effect. This may be through higher levels of specific cognitive performance such as concentration, memory, decision making, alertness and thinking speed. It may be

212 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox a result of psycho-physiological shifts caused by exercise or improved fitness on cerebral function so future research should be ambitious and consider endocrinological changes and functional fmri techniques. This will require exercise researchers to team with neuroscientists. Social psychologists on the other hand may wish to investigate the effect of individual differences in motivation and factors in the activity setting as contributors to effect. Other considerations are important. Objective measures of physical activity such as accelerometry and stronger measures of physical fitness are likely to provide greater insight into individual differences and help increase precision of estimates. It is notoriously inaccurate to assess activity through self-report in children. Greater attention should be paid to the context in which activity takes place, and the mode, frequency and intensity of the activity. Different effects are likely to be seen as these factors vary. Similarly, different factors might dominate with developmental stage of the child, so careful thought needs to be given to choice of age group of the sample. Future research needs to take into account current weaknesses but the quality of future research will be dependent on adequate funding. Currently, given the paucity of research, no strong tradition is established for work in this area. Although it will take time, if the research question or objectives are clearly stated, if a convincing case is provided for the need to address the research question, and there is high quality in the research method, measures and analytical tools proposed, then there will be increasing chance of the work being funded. References Ahamed, Y., MacDonald, H., Reed, K., Naylor, P-J., Lui-Ambrose, T., & McKay, H. (2007). School based physical activity does not compromise children s academic performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 39(2), 371377. Angevaren, M., Aufdemkampe, G., Verhaar, H., Aleman, A., & Vanhees L. (2008). Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2, Biddle, S.J.H., Fox, K.R., & Boutcher, S. (2001). Physical Activity and Psychological Wellbeing. London & New York: Routledge. Biddle, S.J.H., & Mutrie, N. (2008). Psychology of physical activity: determinants, well-being, and interventions (2nd ed.). London: Routledge. Brown, J., Cooper-Kuhn, C.M., Kempermann, G., van Praag, H., Winkler, J., Gage, F.H., & Kuhn, H.G. (2003). Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 20422046. Buck, S.M., Hillman, C.H., & Castelli, D.M. (2007). The relation of aerobic fitness to the stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 40(1), 166173. Carlson, S.A., Fulton, J.E., Lee, S.M., Maynard, M., Brown, D.R., Kohl, H.W., et al. (2008). Physical education and academic achievement in elementary school: data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. American Journal of Public Health, 98(4), 721727. Castelli, D.M., Hillman, C.H., Buck, S.M., & Erwin, H.E. (2007). Physical fitness and academic achievement in third-and fifth-grade students. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29, 239252. Coe, D.P., Pivarnik, J.M., Womack, C.J., Reeves, M.J., & Malina, R.M. (2006). Effects of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 38(8), 15151520. Davis, C.L., Tomporowski, P.D., Boyle, C.A., Waller, J.L., Miller, P.H., & Naglieri, J.A. (2007). Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children s cognitive functioning: A randomized controlled trial. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(5), 510519.

International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 213 Department of Health Physical Activity Health Improvement and Prevention. (2004). At least five a week. London: Department of Health Physical Activity, Health Improvement and Prevention. Dwyer, T., Coonan, W.E., Leitch, D.R., Hetzel, B.S., & Baghurst, R.A. (1983). An investigation of the effects of daily physical activity on the health of primary school students in South Australia. International Journal of Epidemiology, 12(3), 308313. Dwyer, T., Sallis, J.F., Blizzard, L., Lazarus, R., & Dean, K. (2001). Relation of academic performance to physical activity and fitness in children. Pediatric Exercise Science, 13, 225237. Etnier, J.L., Nowell, P.M., Landers, D.M., & Sibley, B.A. (2006). A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Research Reviews, 52, 119130. Eveland-Sayers, B.M., Farley, R.S., Fuller, D.K., Morgan, D.W., & Caputo, J.L. (2009). Physical fitness and academic achievement in elementary school. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 6(1), 99104. Fox, K.R., & Biddle, S.J.H. (1988). Educational and psychological considerations in the use of fitness tests. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 59(2), 4753. Fox, K.R. (2001). The future health professional: multidisciplinary and multiskilled to address nutrition and physical activity. Nutrition Bulletin, 26, 163164. Fox, K.R., & Mutrie, N. (in press). Physical activity and the prevention of mental illness, dysfunction and cognitive deterioration. In G. O Donovan (Ed.) BASES Guidelines on Physical Activity in the Prevention of Chronic Disease. Human Kinetics. Grissom, J.B. (2005). Physical fitness and academic achievement. Journal of Exercise Physiology online, 8(1), 1125. Hamer, M., & Chida, Y. (2009). Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychological Medicine, 39, 311. Hillman, C.H., Castelli, D.M., & Buck, S.M. (2005). Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 37(11), 19671975. Hillman, C.H., Erickson, K.I., & Kramer, A.F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 5865. Jin, J., Jing, H., Choi, G., Oh, M.S., Ryu, J.H., Jeong, J-W., Huh, Y., & Park, C. (2008). Voluntary exercise increases the new cell formation in the hippocampus of ovariectomized mice. Neuroscience Letters, 439, 260263. Kleim, J.A., Cooper, N.R., & VandenBerg, P.M. (2002). Exercise induces angiogenesis but does not alter movement representations with rat motor cortex. Brain Research, 934, 16. Lewis, M.H. (2004). Environmental complexity and central nervous system development and function. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 9195. National Institute of Clinical Excellence. (2004). Depression: Management of depression in primary and secondary care. Leicester: British Psychological Society. Parfitt, G., & Eston, R.G. (2005). The relationship between children s habitual activity level and psychological well being. Acta Peadiatrica, 94, 17911797. Sallis, J.F., McKenzie, T.L., Kolody, B., Lewis, M., Marshall, S., & Rosengard, P. (1999). Effects of health-related physical education on academic achievement: Project SPARK. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70(2), 127134. Sibley, B.A., & Etnier, J.L. (2003). The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science, 15, 243256. Sigfusdottir, D.I., Kristjansson, A.L., & Allegrante, J.P. (2007). Health behaviour and academic achievement in Icelandic school children. Health Education Research, 22(1), 7080. Sing-Manoux, A., Hillsdon, M., Brunner, E., & Marmont, M. (2005). Effects of physical activity on cognitive functioning in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II Prospective Cohort Study. American Journal of Public Health, 95(12), 22522258. Swain, R.A., Harris, A.B., Wiener, E.C., Dutka, M.V., Morris, H.D., Theien, B.E., et al. (2003). Prolonged exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience, 117, 10371046.

214 T.J.H. Keeley and K.R. Fox Taras, H. (2005). Physical activity and student performance at school. Journal of School Health, 75(6), 214218. Tomporowski, P.D. (2003). Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychologica, 112(3), 297324. Tomporowski, P.D., Davis, C.L., Miller, P.H., & Naglieri, J.A. (2008). Exercise and children s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educational Psychology Review, 20(2), 111131. Tremblay, M.S., Imman, J.W., & Willms, J.D. (2000). The relationship between physical activity, self-esteem, and academic achievement in 12-year-old children. Pediatric Exercise Science, 12, 312323. Trudeau, F., & Shephard, R.J. (2008). Physical education, school physical activity, school sports and academic performance. International Journal of Behavioural Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5(10). doi: 10.1186/1479 Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F.C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A., et al. (2007). High impact running improves learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 87, 597609. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2008). Physical activity guidelines advisory committee report, 2008. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. van Praag, H, Shuber, T, Zhao, C., Gage, F.H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(38), 86808685. Yu, C.C.W., Chan, S., Cheng, F., Sung, R.Y.T., & Hau, K-T. (2006). Are physical activity and academic performance compatible? Academic achievement, conduct, physical activity and self-esteem of Hong Kong Chinese primary school children. Educational Studies, 32(4), 311341.