The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys



Similar documents
Which Letter Worked Best?

Community Life Survey Summary of web experiment findings November 2013

SOCIETY OF ACTUARIES THE AMERICAN ACADEMY OF ACTUARIES RETIREMENT PLAN PREFERENCES SURVEY REPORT OF FINDINGS. January 2004

The Effect of Pre-Notification on Mail Survey Response Rates: An Experimental Result

An Evaluation of Cinema Advertising Effectiveness

Selecting Research Participants

INTERNATIONAL SOCIAL SURVEY PROGRAMME (ISSP) WORKING PRINCIPLES

in nigerian companies.

71% of online adults now use video sharing sites

Americans and text messaging

Employment-Based Health Insurance: 2010

HMRC Tax Credits Error and Fraud Additional Capacity Trial. Customer Experience Survey Report on Findings. HM Revenue and Customs Research Report 306

Using Surveys for Data Collection in Continuous Improvement

24% of internet users have made phone calls online

2003 National Survey of College Graduates Nonresponse Bias Analysis 1

Can Equity Release Mechanisms fund long term care costs? Desmond Le Grys

MAT 155. Chapter 1 Introduction to Statistics. Key Concept. Basics of Collecting Data. 155S1.5_3 Collecting Sample Data.

Randomization in Clinical Trials

Descriptive Methods Ch. 6 and 7

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF UBC FACULTY SALARIES: INVESTIGATION OF

Paediatric Outpatient Survey 2011

Tablet Ownership 2013

CHAPTER 4 DATA ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION

Drawings Of A Typical Psychologist

Does Charity Begin at Home or Overseas?

Paper Bills and Statements A Real Necessity In A Digital World

2014 Washington State Health Care Authority Adult Medicaid Health Plan CAHPS Report

The examination of value system in the consumer market in hungary

Patient survey report Category C Ambulance Service User Survey 2008 North East Ambulance Service NHS Trust

Discussion Paper: Conceptual difficulties in measuring retirement

Visual Design, Order Effects, and Respondent Characteristics in a Self-Administered Survey

Quitline Tax Increase. Survey NEW ZEALAND POLICE CITIZENS SATISFACTION RESEARCH (TN/10/19) Six Month Follow Up. Contents

A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF WORKFORCE DIVERSITY IN SERVICE AND MANUFACTURING SECTORS IN INDIA

17% of cell phone owners do most of their online browsing on their phone, rather than a computer or other device

Brand Loyalty in Insurance Companies

The Rise in Download Rate of Search & Use by Adults

Abstraction in Computer Science & Software Engineering: A Pedagogical Perspective

Online Product Research

Survey Research. Classifying surveys on the basis of their scope and their focus gives four categories:

Equality Analysis Report Template (2014)

Street Smart: Demographics and Trends in Motor Vehicle Accident Mortality In British Columbia, 1988 to 2000

Outpatient Survey 2011

What does $50,000 buy in a population survey?

The impact of Discussion Classes on ODL Learners in Basic Statistics Ms S Muchengetwa and Mr R Ssekuma muches@unisa.ac.za, ssekur@unisa.ac.

The Interaction of Workforce Development Programs and Unemployment Compensation by Individuals with Disabilities in Washington State

Papers presented at the ICES-III, June 18-21, 2007, Montreal, Quebec, Canada

Chapter 2. Sociological Investigation

Running head: SAMPLE FOR STUDENTS 1. Sample APA Paper for Students Interested in Learning APA Style 6th Edition. Jeffrey H. Kahn

A survey of public attitudes towards conveyancing services, conducted on behalf of:

The Relative Effectiveness of Sound and Animation in Web Banner Advertisements

Ofcom Illegal Filesharing Pilot - Peer Review

Statistics 2014 Scoring Guidelines

Same-sex Couples Consistency in Reports of Marital Status. Housing and Household Economic Statistics Division

Association Between Variables

Table of Contents. Excutive Summary

Problems for Chapter 9: Producing data: Experiments. STAT Fall 2015.

Council of Ambulance Authorities

Math 58. Rumbos Fall Solutions to Review Problems for Exam 2

THE NO MÁS STUDY: Domestic Violence and Sexual Assault in the Community METHODOLOGY LIMITATIONS

Dual Frame Estimation in the 2003 National Survey of College Graduates

Physics Bachelors with Master s Degrees

Cell Phone Activities 2013

Eye-contact in Multipoint Videoconferencing

Beyond 2011: Population Coverage Survey Field Test 2013 December 2013

The effect of covering letter personalisation in mail surveys

Old-Age and Survivors Insurance: Insured Workers and Their Representation in Claims

Writing the Empirical Social Science Research Paper: A Guide for the Perplexed. Josh Pasek. University of Michigan.

So that's how they drive! Tracking driver behaviour and attitudes in Scotland Chris Eynon, TNS-BMRB. Road Safety Scotland Seminar, October 2011

Fairfield Public Schools

interpretation and implication of Keogh, Barnes, Joiner, and Littleton s paper Gender,

Jon A. Krosnick and LinChiat Chang, Ohio State University. April, Introduction

Attitudes towards Financial Planners. Factuality research

Executive Summary. Process or Communicate? Agency or Direct? Efficiency or Effectiveness?

Video calling and video chat

Shared Parental Leave: Nine Months and Counting Understanding employee attitudes to shared parental leave

INCLINATION OF ENTREPRENEURSHIP AMONG MBA WOMEN STUDENTS IN NAMAKKAL DISTRICT, TAMIL NADU

APPENDIX V METHODOLOGY OF 2011 MONTANA HEALTH INSURANCE SURVEYS

Danny R. Childers and Howard Hogan, Bureau of the,census

Pricing the Critical Illness Risk: The Continuous Challenge.

Survey Analysis Guidelines Sample Survey Analysis Plan. Survey Analysis. What will I find in this section of the toolkit?

RECRUITERS PRIORITIES IN PLACING MBA FRESHER: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

Student Quality Perceptions and Preferences for MBA Delivery Formats: Implications for Graduate Programs in Business and Economics

Attitudes to Independent Dental Hygiene Practice: Dentists and Dental Hygienists in Ontario. Tracey L. Adams, PhD

Inequality, Mobility and Income Distribution Comparisons

The Demographics of Social Media Users 2012

Virtual Teaching in Higher Education: The New Intellectual Superhighway or Just Another Traffic Jam?

THE IMPORTANCE OF BRAND AWARENESS IN CONSUMERS BUYING DECISION AND PERCEIVED RISK ASSESSMENT

Effects of Different Response Types on Iranian EFL Test Takers Performance

Earnings in private jobs after participation to post-doctoral programs : an assessment using a treatment effect model. Isabelle Recotillet

Testing Hypotheses About Proportions

New! Internet Courses

High School Psychology and its Impact on University Psychology Performance: Some Early Data

Journal of Technology Education Vol. 13 No.1, Fall Comparing Computer Usage by Students in Education Programs to Technology Education Majors

OCR Business Studies for AS

Bivariate Statistics Session 2: Measuring Associations Chi-Square Test

Elementary Statistics

Air Travel Market Segments A New England Case Study

CONTENTS OF DAY 2. II. Why Random Sampling is Important 9 A myth, an urban legend, and the real reason NOTES FOR SUMMER STATISTICS INSTITUTE COURSE

An Analysis of the Health Insurance Coverage of Young Adults

Introduction to Hypothesis Testing

Transcription:

The Effect of Questionnaire Cover Design in Mail Surveys Philip Gendall It has been suggested that the response rate for a self administered questionnaire will be enhanced if the cover of the questionnaire contains a picture, and, furthermore, that the more distinctive and complex the cover design created, the stronger this effect is likely to be. This paper reports the results of a study designed to test these hypotheses by comparing responses to six different questionnaire cover designs, varying in terms of complexity of graphic design and the presence or absence of images. These results suggest that, while the use of pictures or photos may marginally increase response rates, there is little evidence that a more complex design is better than a simple one. Keywords: mail survey, response rates, questionnaire, cover design Introduction In his seminal work on survey research, Dillman (1978) suggested that prominent graphical designs on questionnaire covers encourages their recipients to respond. This view was supported by Jenkins & Ciochetto (1993) in a small exploratory study from which they concluded that subjects were overwhelmingly drawn to a cover page containing a picture (in this case an icon of an apple sitting on some books). A study by Nederhof (1988) also found that a questionnaire with a largely black contrastive front cover yielded an 12.5% higher response rate than the same cover design that was white spaced and hardly contrastive. Nederhof's explanation for this result was that because the black questionnaire was more distinctive and visually complex it was more memorable. Hence the longer-term cognitive accessibility of the black questionnaire was enhanced; this increased the likelihood that it would be attended to once again after being put aside, and thus improved the chance that it would be completed and returned. The significance of this enhanced cognitive accessibility was demonstrated by the fact that the white covers performed at least as well as the black covers early in the survey, but did significantly worse after the fourth wave when reminder techniques (postcards and telephone calls), which did not include a copy of the questionnaire, were used. Thus it appears that the response rate for a self administered questionnaire may be enhanced if the cover of the questionnaire contains a picture and, furthermore, that the more distinctive and complex the cover design created, the stronger this affect is likely to be. However, attempts by Dillman & Dillman (1995) to replicate Nederhof's research using different cover designs were unsuccessful. Dillman & Dillman tested a range of cover designs including simple text versions, white dominant and black dominant graphic designs (to emulate Nederhof's experiment), and four-colour versions of the same graphic designs. But their results provided little or no support for Nederhof's theory. The black cover produced a higher response in one experiment, while the white cover was more effective in the other, though neither of the differences was statistically significant. Similarly, there were no differences in the effectiveness of text or colour designs, casting further doubt on the hypothesis that questionnaire cover design influences survey completion rates. Page 1 of 7

Nevertheless, Dillman & Dillman conceded the possibility that their conceptual replication of Nederhof's covers was sufficiently different to somehow affect the memorability of the covers tested in their experiments. Furthermore, the effect observed in Nederhof's study was very strong. Consequently it may be premature to reject Nederhof's conclusions simply because Dillman & Dillman found no support for them. This paper reports the findings of another study designed to examine the effect of questionnaire cover design on the response to a mail survey, and, specifically, to test the effect of complexity of graphic design and the presence or absence of images. Method The vehicle for this research was the 1994 International Social Survey Programme (ISSP) survey on the family and changing gender roles 1. The ISSP is a group of 22 countries which conducts an annual survey of economic and social policy issues using the same questionnaire in each country. In New Zealand, the ISSP surveys are conducted by mail. In September 1994, a questionnaire, covering letter on University letterhead, and reply-paid envelope were sent to 1762 New Zealanders aged 18 and over. The sample was selected systematically from the New Zealand Electoral Rolls, with the number of names selected from each electorate proportional to the size of the electorate. The sample was randomly allocated to six groups, each containing approximately 280 potential respondents. Each of these groups received a questionnaire with a different cover. The covers tested varied in terms of complexity of graphic design, the use of colour, and the presence or absence of images, but all were printed on the same beige card. The experimental design involved three "pairs" of covers. The first pair consisted of one cover with only a simple graphic design in black lettering, and the same cover, but with a picture included. The second and third pairs of covers consisted of more complex, but different graphic designs in black and red lettering, with one cover in each pair incorporating photographs. This experimental design is illustrated in Table 1 and the questionnaire cover designs are shown in Figure 1. The questionnaire itself comprised 20 pages and 67 questions (though the total number of individual question items totalled 113). It asked respondents their opinions, behaviour and knowledge on a range of issues concerning the roles of men and women in marriage, at work, and in society in general. It also included an extensive demographic section. A reminder letter was sent to all non-respondents four weeks after the initial mailing. A second reminder was sent to all remaining non-respondents four weeks later. Questionnaires were not included with either reminder. After a further four weeks a total of 1236 questionnaires had been returned. Eighty two respondents refused to take part in the survey, 26 had died or were otherwise ineligible, and 152 questionnaires were returned "Gone no address". This left 976 valid responses, representing a response rate of [976/(1762-178)]x100 = 65.3%. Page 2 of 7

Table 1. Experimental design Cover A1 Simple graphic design Black lettering only No pictures or photos Cover B1 More complex design No picture or photos Cover C1 Different complex design No picture or photos Cover A2 Simple graphic design Black lettering only Picture included Cover B2 More complex design Photos included Cover C2 Different complex design Photos included Results At the end of 12 weeks, response rates for the six treatment groups ranged from 62% to 67% (see Table 2). Table 2. Response rates for six cover designs Design Complexity Response Rate 1 No picture or Photos (1) Picture or Photos Included (2) N % n % Simple A Complex B Complex C 168 165 166 65.9 64.5 66.4 161 169 147 67.4 66.3 61.5 Note: 1. Response rates adjusted for "Gone-no-address" and ineligible. There is little or no evidence in these results that the inclusion of a picture or photos in a questionnaire cover increases the response to a mail survey. Though in two pairs of questionnaires the version including a picture or a photo had a higher response, the increases were less than 2% and were not significant, and for the other pair of questionnaires the cover with no picture or photo achieved the highest response. Overall, the response rate for the three questionnaires without images was 65.6%, compared to 65.1% for those which included either a picture or photos. Similarly, there is little evidence that increased complexity or distinctiveness increases mail survey response rates. Overall, the response rate for the simple design was 66.6%, compared to 65.4% and 64.0% for the two more complex designs. Furthermore, the response achieved by the very simplest design was virtually the same as that for the most successful of the more complex designs, and better than for two of them. Page 3 of 7

However, Nederhof's (1988) theory of the cognitive accessibility of questionnaires suggests that more distinctive and visually complex questionnaires should produce higher response rates in the later phases of a mail survey, particularly if reminders are not accompanied by another questionnaire. The response rates for the three waves of this survey are shown in Table 3. If Nederhof's theory were correct, we would expect the more complex cover designs to produce higher response rates in the second and third waves. This is not what happened. In fact, the simplest (and by assumption, the least memorable) covers had the highest response to both the first and second reminder letters. Table 3. Response rates by wave Response Rate 1 % Cover Design Initial Posting First Reminder Second Reminder n % n % n % Simple graphic (A1) 123 43.0 43 31.9 12 13.5 Simple graphic plus picture (A2) 111 44.4 37 29.4 13 16.5 Complex graphic (B1) 114 42.9 39 27.9 12 13.0 Complex graphic plus photos (B2) 126 47.6 35 27.6 8 9.1 Complex graphic (C1) 118 45.7 41 32.0 7 8.8 Complex graphic plus photos (C2) 110 43.8 26 20.8 11 11.3 Total 692 44.6 221 28.3 63 12.3 Note: 1. Adjusted for "Gone-no-address" and ineligible and based on number of letters sent in each wave. However, examination of first-wave responses alone shows that the addition of a picture to the simple graphic design increased the response rate by 1.4%, and the inclusion of photos increased the response to one of the complex designs by 4.7% (in the third pair of covers, the effect of photos was to decrease the response rate by just under 2%.) This provides some support for Jenkins & Ciochetto's conclusions. But the three questionnaires without images performed better in the second and third waves, so that by the end of the survey the positive differences in favour of images observed at the end of the first wave had virtually disappeared and the other difference had increased. Despite these results, it is possible that the cover designs tested had differential effects on the responses of men and women or of those in different age groups. For example, a particular cover design could have achieved a high response rate among younger people and a low response rate among older people, but these effects could have been disguised in the overall response rate. However, as Table 4 shows, there was no relationship between age or sex and response to the alternative cover designs. In an attempt to gain some insight into respondents' reactions to the covers tested, three focus groups were conducted with convenience samples of potential respondents. Participants were 13 men and 15 women between the ages of 18 and 50, recruited from local community organisations. Page 4 of 7

Table 4. Sex and age distributions by cover design Cover Design Sex Age Group Male Female Under 30 30-39 40-49 50-65 Over 65 % % % % % % % A1 41.6 58.4 19.4 20.6 21.3 24.4 14.4 A2 38.0 62.0 15.8 24.1 19.6 25.9 14.6 B1 41.7 58.3 21.0 23.5 17.9 19.8 17.9 B2 40.4 59.6 17.5 19.9 26.5 23.5 12.7 C1 43.5 56.5 17.5 20.6 21.9 27.5 12.5 C2 39.7 60.3 18.5 24.0 18.5 24.7 14.4 Total 40.8 59.2 18.4 22.1 21.0 24.3 14.4 Note: 1. For cover design by sex, X 2 = 1.18, d.f. = 5, p =.95. 2. For cover design by age group, X 2 = 10.92, d.f. = 20, p =.95. For each group, participants were first asked to read a copy of the covering letter used in the survey. Then they were each given a complete set of the six alternative covers and asked to select the cover they considered would be most effective and least effective in motivating them to complete and return a questionnaire on the topic concerned, and to explain why. Table 5 shows the outcome of this process. Ironically, the cover judged to be most effective by the highest number of focus group participants (cover C2) was the least effective in practice. Conversely, the simplest cover (A1) was clearly rated as least effective in the focus groups but performed as well as any of the other alternatives in the field. These results are, of course, based only on a small convenience sample, but they do suggest that either potential respondents are not good judges of what constitutes an effective questionnaire cover design or that what is on the cover of a questionnaire is not particularly important (provided the rest of the survey package is well designed and presented). Table 5. Most effective and least effective covers Cover Design Most Effective Cover Least Effective Cover Simple graphic (A1) 3 16 Simple graphic plus picture (A2) 6 0 Complex graphic (B1) 5 3 Complex graphic plus photos (B2) 3 4 Complex graphic (C1) 2 1 Complex graphic plus photos (C2) 9 4 Page 5 of 7

What the focus groups did confirm, however, was that the cover designs which were assumed to be more distinctive and complex were in fact perceived in this way by potential respondents. This could be deduced by the comments made about the alternative covers. For example, cover A1 was described as "Simple and to the point"; "Straightforward, no frills"; and "Nice and plain"; whereas comments about covers B2 and C2 included references to them being "Very busy", "Cluttered" and "Eyecatching". (Though two of these latter three comments are negative, they support the contention that these cover designs were perceived as more distinctive and complex than the others tested.) Discussion This study was designed to test the hypotheses that the response rate for a mail survey will be increased if the cover of the questionnaire contains a picture, and that the more distinctive and complex the cover design created, the stronger this effect will be. The results suggest that, while the use of photos or pictures may marginally increase response rates, the effect is not guaranteed and may even be negative. Similarly, there is little or no evidence in this study to support Nederhof's theory that a more distinctive, complex questionnaire cover design is better than a simple one. However, as Dillman & Dillman (1995) point out, "retrievability", and hence the effect of questionnaire cover design, may be different for different populations. Nederhof (1988) surveyed biotechnologists at work, whereas those surveyed in both Dillman & Dillman's study and in this study were members of the general public, contacted at home. Perhaps memorability of questionnaire cover design is more important in situations where respondents are dealing with considerable paperwork, as would be the case for Nederhof's biotechnologists. Even if this is so, it does not necessarily mean that questionnaire cover design is unimportant in surveys of the general public. The difference in response rates between the most successful and least successful of the six covers tested was nearly 6%, a non-trivial difference. Unfortunately, such a difference cannot be achieved simply by including a picture on the cover of a questionnaire or by increasing the distinctiveness or complexity of the cover design. Such measures may increase the response rate, but their effect is not predictable. Intuitively it seems logical that the cover design of a mail survey questionnaire should affect the response to it, and, further, that the "right" design should increase the response rate achieved. The problem is how to determine the "right" design. Talking to potential respondents about cover design may not be particularly helpful; in this study the cover which potential respondents preferred was the least effective in practice. And, without a wellsupported theory of what constitutes an effective cover design, we can only guess at what will work best. From a practical point of view, this study suggests that a clear, simple, well balanced cover design is likely to be as effective as a more elaborate one, at least for surveys of the general public. The addition of a relevant graphic appears unlikely to decrease the effectiveness of a questionnaire and may increase it, so there seems little to lose from doing this, if it can be done easily, and potentially something to gain. The wisdom of using complex cover designs and designs which include photos seems more doubtful. Photos, in particular, have the Page 6 of 7

potential to create unpredictable and sometimes undesirable images in the minds of respondents, and, for this reason, I suggest it is better to avoid them. Overall, it appears that any effect of questionnaire cover design on response rate will be marginal in a well-conducted mail survey. Nevertheless, the appearance of the questionnaire inevitably contributes to the impression created by any survey package, consequently it would be wrong to ignore the issue of questionnaire cover design simply because we have not yet been able to determine what constitutes a better questionnaire cover. References Dillman DA (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: The Total Design method. New York: Wiley-Interscience. Dillman JJ & Dillman DA (1995). The influence of questionnaire cover design on response to mail surveys. Proceedings of an International Conference on Measurement and Process Quality. Bristol, England. (In press.) Jenkins C & Ciochetto S (1993). Results of cognitive research on the multiplicity question from the 1991 Schools and Staffing Survey student records questionnaire. A report submitted to the National Center for Education Statistics, Census Bureau Memorandum, February 10. (p.12) Nederhof AJ (1988). Effects of a final telephone reminder and questionnaire cover design in mail surveys. Social Science Research, 17, 353-361. Philip Gendall is Professor of Marketing and Head of the Department of Marketing at Massey University. Footnote. 1. Though the official title of the ISSP module was "The family and changing gender roles", in New Zealand the title used on the survey questionnaires was "The roles of men and women in society". The latter was considered a more appropriate title for a survey of the general public. Page 7 of 7