CELL PHONE USE AND GRADE POINT AVERAGE AMONG UNDERGRADUATE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS



Similar documents
Impact of attendance policies on course attendance among college students

CHARACTERIZING THE EFFECT OF VIDEOPHONE CONVERSATIONS ON INTERSECTION DRIVING PERFORMANCE. Urbana, Illinois, USA

The effect of multitasking on the grade performance of business students

Asynchronous Learning Networks and Student Outcomes: The Utility of Online Learning Components in Hyhrid Courses

TEACHING PRINCIPLES OF ECONOMICS: INTERNET VS. TRADITIONAL CLASSROOM INSTRUCTION

The Effects of Cell Phone Distraction on Cognitive Tasks. Yu Lee, Chanda Atkinson, Danielle Hritsko, Kobe Acquaah

SCIENTIST-PRACTITIONER INTEREST CHANGES AND COURSE PERFORMANCE IN AN UNDERGRADUATE RESEARCH METHODS PSYCHOLOGY COURSE

Cell Phoning and Texting While Driving: Multitasking Ourselves to Death

Running head: SAMPLE FOR STUDENTS 1. Sample APA Paper for Students Interested in Learning APA Style 6th Edition. Jeffrey H. Kahn

Distracted Driving is Impaired Driving

CRITICAL THINKING ASSESSMENT

Laptops, Cell Phones and Facebook- Distraction or Course Tool?

The relationship between goals, metacognition, and academic success

When reviewing the literature on

Skills versus Concepts: Attendance and Grades in Information Technology Courses

The Effects of Alcohol Use on Academic Performance Among College Students

A Hands-On Exercise Improves Understanding of the Standard Error. of the Mean. Robert S. Ryan. Kutztown University

RUNNING HEAD: TUTORING TO INCREASE STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT USING TUTORING TO INCREASE STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT ON END OF COURSE ASSESSMENTS. By KATHYRENE HAYES

Driver Distraction is Greater with Cell Phone Conversation than with Passenger Conversation -- A Social Cybernetic Interpretation

Running head: EFFECTS OF CELL PHONE USE 1

Perceptions of College Students in Long Distance Relationships

Assessing Blackboard: Improving Online Instructional Delivery

UNDERGRADUATE PREPARATION FOR GRADUATE TRAINING IN FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

Applied Data Analysis. Fall 2015

Test Anxiety, Student Preferences and Performance on Different Exam Types in Introductory Psychology

CELL PHONE INDUCED PERCEPTUAL IMPAIRMENTS DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

Assessment Findings and Curricular Improvements Department of Psychology Undergraduate Program. Assessment Measures

Scientific Method. 2. Design Study. 1. Ask Question. Questionnaire. Descriptive Research Study. 6: Share Findings. 1: Ask Question.

SIS Data Importer Guide for Administrators 1.0 (Beta) Contents

UNIT: PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

SENIOR EXIT SURVEY. 6. If you changed majors to psychology, when did you switch? 9. Did you obtain a certificate or minor from another department?

Follow-up Audit of Medical School Admissions

THE ROCHESTER INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION. The Effect of Unsafe Driving Behaviors on Attitudes Toward Texting While Driving

Rapid Communication. Who Visits Online Dating Sites? Exploring Some Characteristics of Online Daters

Important Steps to Becoming a Psychiatrist. Courses to take as an undergraduate, besides typical PSY courses (Intro, Research Methods, Stats):

Chapter 6: Experiences of Transfer Students

Psychology. Academic Requirements. Academic Requirements. Career Opportunities. Minor. Major. Mount Mercy University 1

Cell Phone Distracted Driving is Impaired Driving. John Ulczycki National Safety Council

REFLECTIONS ON ONLINE COURSE DESIGN - QUALITY MATTERS EVALUATION AND STUDENT FEEDBACK: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

The Psychology Lab Checklist By: Danielle Sclafani 08 and Stephanie Anglin 10; Updated by: Rebecca Behrens 11

Investigating Gender Stereotypes in Elementary Education

OBJECTS-FIRST VS. STRUCTURES-FIRST APPROACHES TO OO PROGRAMMING EDUCATION: AN EMPIRICAL STUDY

How To Study The Academic Performance Of An Mba

This definition of special education comes from the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), Public Law

Maximizing Learning in Online Training Courses: Meta-Analytic Evidence. Traci Sitzmann

Prevalence of Fully Online Courses in Dietetics Education Programs

NAME Street City State Zip Application to Programs School of Business Administration and Economics College at Brockport, State University of New York

Student budgeting and spending behaviors: a comparative study

The Use and Abuse of Cell Phones and Text Messaging in the Classroom: A Survey of College Students

PROPOSED PENNSYLVANIA STATEWIDE PROGRAM-TO-PROGRAM ARTICULATION AGREEMENT IN PSYCHOLOGY

COLLEGE OF EDUCATION TECHNICAL STANDARDS/ FOUNDATIONAL COMPETENCIES POLICY 1 (May 2004; revised August 2005; title revised August 2007)

Achievement and Satisfaction in a Computer-assisted Versus a Traditional Lecturing of an Introductory Statistics Course

Chi Square Distribution

DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS

5 Information Technology in Courses

Defiance College FIELD PLACEMENT APPLICATION. Name Student ID # Last First Middle. Local address

AP * Statistics Review. Designing a Study

The Growing Epidemic of Cell Phone Use While Driving. Participant Guide

Attitudes Toward Science of Students Enrolled in Introductory Level Science Courses at UW-La Crosse

Contemporary Issues In Education Research First Quarter 2012 Volume 5, Number 1

College of Education. Bachelor of Science Psychology Major and Comprehensive Psychology Program

Video Games and Academic Performance. Ronny Khadra. Cody Hackshaw. Leslie Mccollum. College of Coastal Georgia

Undergraduate Degree Program Assessment Progress Report Cover Sheet

Engaging the Disengaged: Factors Associated with Undergraduate Campus Leadership Involvement

Do Supplemental Online Recorded Lectures Help Students Learn Microeconomics?*

CONSUMERS OF ONLINE INSTRUCTION

Teaching college microeconomics: Online vs. traditional classroom instruction

The Success and Satisfaction of Community College Transfer Students at UC Berkeley

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN CLASSROOM MANAGEMENT STYLES OF TEACHERS AND THE OCCURRENCE OF DISCIPLINE PROBLEMS IN INTERNATIONAL SCHOOLS BANGKOK, THAILAND

Analysis of the Effectiveness of Online Learning in a Graduate Engineering Math Course

WR305, Writing for the Web Usability Test Documents

S. E. Kruck is Assistant Professor, Information Technology and Management Science Program, James Madison University, Harrisonburg, Virginia.

Program Level Assessment Report for

AP CS Principles Pilot at University of California, San Diego

Preparing for Graduate School I: Options and Time Lines Dr. Steffen Wilson, Dr. Laura Koppes, Dr. Meredith Wells Eastern Kentucky University

Motivators for Obtaining Advanced Degrees in Today s Society. Caroline Mulhall and Cassandra Rehmel. Hanover College

For more information, visit our web site at or contact:

The McLean Scholarship for Nursing and Physician Assistant Majors

Psychology G4470. Psychology and Neuropsychology of Language. Spring 2013.

Virtual Teaching in Higher Education: The New Intellectual Superhighway or Just Another Traffic Jam?

Appendix B: Accelerated Program Guidelines

Aggie R-r-ring Program Use Survey Fall 2005 Results

Your Questions from Chapter 1. General Psychology PSYC 200. Your Questions from Chapter 1. Your Questions from Chapter 1. Science is a Method.

Examining Science and Engineering Students Attitudes Toward Computer Science

STUDENTS PERCEPTIONS OF TEACHER CREDIBILITY AND LEARNING EXPECTATIONS IN CLASSROOM COURSES WITH WEBSITES

Issues in Information Systems Volume 13, Issue 2, pp , 2012

Helping psychology students improve their statistics performance: A test of the Exo-St@ts web-based training environment

A Comparison Between Online and Face-to-Face Instruction of an Applied Curriculum in Behavioral Intervention in Autism (BIA)

Department of Psychology

T r i t o n C o l l e g e Assessment Planning and Reporting Tool ACADEMIC PROGRAMS

Running head: SHIFTING GENRE PAPER: QUANTITATIVE TO QUALITATIVE 1. Shifting Genre Paper: Quantitative Analysis of At-Risk Youth and Locus of Control

A Modest Experiment Comparing HBSE Graduate Social Work Classes, On Campus and at a. Distance

Undergraduate Degree Program Assessment Progress Report Cover Sheet

Comparison of Student Performance in an Online with traditional Based Entry Level Engineering Course

Estimating the effect of projected changes in the driving population on collision claim frequency

Angelo State University. PSY 6347 Life-Span Development Psychology. fall, James Forbes, PhD

ACT CAAP Critical Thinking Test Results

Scientific Methods in Psychology

Transcription:

CELL PHONE USE AND GRADE POINT AVERAGE AMONG UNDERGRADUATE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS BRITTANY A. H ARM AN ToRu SATO Department of Psychology, Shippensburg University Shippensburg, Pennsylvania, USA The present study examined whether cell phone use frequency is correlated with academic performance as measured by grade point average among undergraduate university students. Participants completed a survey about their cell phone use and also reported their grade point average. Results revealed that text messaging frequency was negatively correlated with grade point average and variables such as academic level while cell phone call frequency was not. The results of this study suggest that the more an individual sends or receives text messages, the lower his or her grade point average typically is. Surprisingly, individuals with higher grade point averages are more comfortable text messaging in class. No significant results were found in regards to individuals placing phone calls on cell phones. Key words: Cell Phone Use Frequency, Text Messages, Cell Phone Calls, Grade Point Average (GPA) Introduction Since the commercialization of cellular phone technology by Motorola in the early 1980s, the use of this communication device has rapidly increased. Studies on the impact of using a cell phone while driving or simultaneously engaging in other motor activities of a similar nature are common (Strayer, Drews, & Johnston, 2003; Drews, Pasupathi, & Sti-ayer, 2008; Charlton, 2009; Hyman, Boss, Wise, McKenzie, & Caggiano, 2010), but research investigating the influence of cell phone use in other domains is surprisingly sparse. One area of particular interest which seems underrepresented in the literature regarding cell phone use is the impact of this communication device in academic settings. Because teens aged 13-17 and young adults aged 18-24 are overwhelmingly the most prolific groups of cell phone users (NielsenWire, 2010), it is plausible that their level of cell phone use may have a significant effect on their academic performance. An analysis of data gathered recently by the Nielsen Company revealed that young adults aged 14-18 exchange an average of 1,630 text messages per month, or approximately 54 messages per day (NielsenWire, 2010). Additionally, in regards to cell phone use in educational settings, the Pew Research Center found that in spite of the fact that many schools prohibit the use of cell phones, 65% of students who attend schools that ban cell phones still bring them every day and 43% of students reported that they text message in class at least once per day (Pew Intemet, 2010). These findings led us to question whether or not a student's overall frequency of cell phone use impacts his or her overall academic 544

Cell Phone Use and Grade Point Average... / 545 performance, as measured by a student's grade point average (GPA). Past research has suggested that cell phone use frequency may have a negative effect on academic performance (Ellis, Daniels, & Jauregui, 2010; Sánchez- Martínez & Otero, 2009). For instance, one study found that frequent cell phone use was related to school failure among high school students (Sánchez-Martínez & Otero, 2009). In another study, an experiment was conducted in a university class to reveal that text messaging on cell phones during class causes lower academic test scores among undergraduate students (Ellis, Daniels, & Jauregui, 2010). Finally, a study by End, Worthman, Mathews, & Wetterau (2010) found that students who experience a cell phone intmsion are often unable to identify the lecture material discussed during the dismption and suggested that these intrusions could potentially impair the leaming process. Although these studies provide important insights, the present study examined how frequency of cell phone use is related to overall academic performance as measured by grade point average (GPA) among undergraduate university students. Based on previous findings examining cell phone use and academic performance mentioned above, we hypothesized that cell phone use frequency and the degree of comfort using cell phones in class would be negatively correlated with academic performance. Method Participants A total of 118 undergraduate university students (38 men, 80 women) participated in the study in exchange for partial course credit. The mean age of the sample was 20.01 years of age, with an age range of 18-39 years. Survey Material and Procedure Participants reported to a lab and were asked with sign a participant consent form. They were then asked complete an anonymous survey consisting of a variety of questions regarding cell phone use, academic standing, and demographic information. For the questions regarding academic year and GPA, participants were asked to circle one of four options. For academic year, the options were Freshman, Sophomore, Junior, & Senior. The responses were coded from 1-4: Freshmen to Seniors. For GPA the options were 0-1.99,2.00-2.99,3.00-3.49,3.50-4.00. The responses were coded from 1-4 ranging from the lowest to highest GPA category. Participants were asked to estimate a number by filling in a blank for questions regarding the average number of cell phone calls and text messages made/sent and received as well as the average number of times their cell phone is checked for messages and the average number of people called or texted in a day. We asked participants to exclude text messages that are considered to be spam and cell-phone calls that are not connected from all of their estimates. Participants were also asked whether they feel comfortable text mes-

546 / College Student Journal Table 1. Descriptives and correlations Variable Mean SD Correlations with Age AcaYear GPA Average number of people text messaged per day Average number of text messages sent per day Average number of text messages received per day Average number of people spoken to on cell phone per day Average number of calls made per day Average number of calls received per day Average number of times the cell phone is checked Comfort text messaging in class iv=118 *p<0.05 **p<0.01 7.04 102.89 112.59 3.45 4.17 3.90 60.14 1.42 5.89 141.06 211.58 2.27 3.55 3.86 85.98.50 -.203* -.219* -.091 -.108 -.110 -.091 -.188.150 -.167 -.263** -.209*.013 -.062 -.074 -.203* -.032 -.018 -.211* -.212* -.160 -.056 -.115 -.045.258** saging in class. The response format for this question was simply yes or no and coded as 1 and 0, respectively. After completing the survey, participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation. Results The means, standard deviations, and correlations regarding all of the variables examined are presented in Table 1. These results revealed negative correlations between GPA and the number of texts an individual sends, r(112)= -.211, p=.026 as well as GPA and the number of texts an individual receives, r(l 11)= -.212,p-.025. Contrary to our expectations, GPA was positively correlated with being comfortable text messaging in class, r(l 18)=.258, p=.005. Furthermore, academic year was negatively correlated with the number of text messages sent, r(l 12)= -.263, p=.005, the number of texts received, r(117)= -.209, p=.028, and the average number of times per day an individual checks his or her cell phone for text messages, r(102)= -.203, p=.041. In addition, the variable of age was negatively correlated with the average number of people an individual text messages per day, r(il3)=-.203,p=.031. Age was also negatively correlated with the number of text messages sent per day, K112)=-.219,p=.020. We also examined the average number of phone calls participants make or receive and the average number of different people participants call in an average day. These variables did not significantly correlate with GPA, academic year, or age. Finally i-tests examining gender differences on all variables revealed no significant effects.

Cell Phone Use and Grade Point Average... /547 Discussion Correlations As is evident from the results reported above, some of these findings support our hypothesis stating that cell phone use frequency may be negatively correlated with academic performance, while others did not. For example, the negative correlation that was observed between age and academic year and the number of text messages that were sent, received, and the number of times he or she checked the phone for messages was unsurprising, given that individuals in the 18-24 year age group who comprised most of our population have been found to send or receive the second highest number of text messages per month, second only to individuals in the 13-17 year age group (NielsenWire, 2010). GPA was negatively correlated with the number of text messages that were sent and received. This result was also unsurprising given that past research has found that a high frequency of cell phone use is correlated with lower academic test scores (Ellis & Jauregui, 2010), an inability to remember dismpted lecture material (End, Worthman, Mathews, & Wetterau, 2010), and school failure (Sánchez-Martínez & Otero, 2009). Additionally, the association between lower grade point averages and sending or receiving a higher number of text messages can be explained by the fact that compared to individuals with lower GPAs, those with higher GPAs are more likely to spend more time on academics and therefore they may have less time to send and receive text messages. Higher GPA could also be influenced by the fact that not being constantly preoccupied with a cell phone allows the individual to have more time to reflect on information presented to him or her throughout the day, make meaningful associations between what they are leaming and their own experiences, and participate in more advanced communication with others about academic material than what is typically communicated through text messages. Individuals with higher GPAs may also be more acutely aware of their own cognitive limitations, acknowledging the fact that sending text messages or having a ringing cell phone interrupts their concentration and thus, may turn their phones off when studying. As a result of findings from past research and our own assumptions, we were surprised to find that GPA was positively correlated with comfort text messaging in class. It seems as though the higher the GPA that an individual has, the more comfortable he or she feels sending or receiving text messages in the classroom. This could potentially be explained by the fact that individuals with higher GPAs may be more comfortable with their own ability to leam the material outside of class and thus, paying attention to the material presented in class may not be viewed as essential to achieving a high grade in the course. Additionally, because part of receiving a good grade is understanding what a particular instructor is requiring for each assignment, people with higher GPA's may be more sensitive to the needs and demands of the instmctor. This type of sensitivity might also contribute to the student's ability to discriminate

548 / College Student Journal between instructors and class circumstances that will or will not influence their grade if they use their cell phones. Finally, in contrast to the significant correlations with text messaging, no correlations were observed between age, academic year, or GPA and the number of phone calls that were made, received, or the number of people an individual called. It is possible that there was not enough variation with the variables on phone calls to have significant correlations because the averages for these variables were much lower in comparison to the text-messaging variables. For example, the average number of cell phone calls made (M= 4.17), received (M= 3.90), and the number of people called (M=3.45) per day was very low compared to the number of text messages sent (M= 102.89), received (M= 112.59), and the number of people an individual communicates with via text messaging on average per day (M= 7.04). This is unsurprising, as research by the Nielsen Company suggests that 78% of teens consider text messaging more convenient than making calls on their cell phones (NielsenWire, 2010) and less than a quarter of teens have made or received a call during class (Pew Intemet, 2010). Limitations and Conclusions The present study which investigated the correlation between the frequency of cell phone use and overall academic performance demonstrated a number of significant findings. Most importantly, this study revealed that the more an individual text messages, the lower his or her GPA is likely to be. A multitude of speculations can be made to explain these findings, and more in depth research needs to be conducted to analyze the validity of these conjectures. Our study had a number of limitations that should be accounted for in future research on the topic. First, our study was correlational in nature and thus, causality between the variables cannot be determined. For example, there is no way of knowing whether doing poorly in school makes us devalue academics and utilize more recreational networking to protect our self-esteem, which would lead us to text message more or if there might be no causal relafionship between academic and texting variables. If the latter was the case, a third variable, such as self control, might be causing the changes in both the academic and the texting variables. Additionally, our study did not take into account whether students who are more comfortable text messaging in class actually engage in this behavior or what factors contribute to their decision to text message or refrain from doing so. Furthermore, it would be interesting to investigate if the lower grade point averages of students who text message more frequently are lower as a result of texting in class, while studying, or because of some other unknown variable. Further research needs to be conducted to determine the more specific ways in which the high frequency use of cell phones or other technologies impacts the academic success of students. It is our hope that the present research study will serve as an important contribufion to the literature aimed at developing a more complete understanding of the

Cell Phone Use and Grade Point Average... / 549 impact of cell phone use within academic settings. Perhaps this study will enable students to be more aware of the long term impacts of using a cell phone while in class or studying on their overall grade point averages. Furthermore, we believe that our research can help educators and school policy makers formulate well informed and appropriate decisions regarding the possession and use of these communication devices in the classroom. Correspondence conceming this paper should be addressed to: Brittany A. Harman. Department of Psychology, Shippensburg University, 1871 Old Main Drive, Shippensburg, PA 17257 References Charlton, S. G. (2009). Driving while conversing: Cell phones that distract and passengers who react. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41(1), 160-173. Drews, F. A., Pasupathi, M., Strayer, D. L. (2008). Passenger and cell phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(4), 392-400. End, C. M., Worthman, S., Mathews, M. B., Wetterau, K. (2010). Costly Cell Phones: The Impact of Cell Phone Rings on Academic Performance. Teaching of Psychology, 37(1), 55-57. Ellis, Y., Daniels, B., & Jauregui, A. (2010). The effect of multitasking on the grade performance of business students. Research in Higher Education Journal, 8, 1-10. Hyman, 1. E., Boss, S. M., Wise, B. M., McKenzie, K. E., Caggiano, J. M. (2010). Did you see the unicycling clown? Inattentional blindness while walking and talking on a cell phone. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 597-607. Sánchez-Martínez, M., & Otero, A. (2009). Factors associated with cell phone use in adolescents in the community of Madrid (Spain). Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12, 131-137. Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., Johnston, W. A. (2003). Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9(1), 23-32. Teens and Mobile Phones. Retrieved September 16, 2010 from the Pew Research Center Web site: http://www.pewintemet.org/reports/2010 /Teens-and-Mobile-Phones/Summary-of-fmdings.aspx U.S. Teen Mobile Report: Calling Yesterday, Texting Today, Using Apps Tomorrow. Retrieved December 2, 2010 from the NielsenWire Web site: http://blog.nielsen.coni/nielsenwire/onlin e_mobile/u-s-teen-mobile-report-calling-yesterday-texting-today-using-apps-tomorrow/

Copyright of College Student Journal is the property of Project Innovation, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.