Learner and Information Characteristics in the Design of Powerful Learning Environments



Similar documents
Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments

Cognitive Load Theory: Instructional Implications of the Interaction between Information Structures and Cognitive Architecture

Instructional Design for Advanced Learners: Establishing Connections Between the Theoretical Frameworks of Cognitive Load and Deliberate Practice

Expertise Reversal Effect of Animation Segmentation

Visualisation and Instructional Design. John Sweller School of Education University of New South Wales Sydney NSW 2052 Australia

Comparison of different approaches on example based learning for novice and proficient learners

Using Cognitive Load Theory to Tailor Instruction to Levels of Accounting Students Expertise

Cognitive load theory, educational research, and instructional design: some food for thought

Taking the Load Off a Learner s Mind: Instructional Design for Complex Learning

A critical analysis of the conceptual and mathematical properties of instructional efficiency

Structuring the Transition From Example Study to Problem Solving in Cognitive Skill Acquisition: A Cognitive Load Perspective

Cognitive Load Theory: Instruction-based Research with Applications for Designing Tests

An Online Resource for the Design of Instructional Videos and Animations

Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning

Multimedia Learning Theories and Online Instruction

The Ideal Learning Management System for Multimedia Learning

THE EFFECT OF COGNITIVE AGING ON MULTIMEDIA LEARNING

The Four-Component Instructional Design Model: Multimedia Principles in Environments for Complex Learning

Interactive and Dynamic Visualizations in Teaching and Learning of Anatomy: A Cognitive Load Perspective

Aids to computer-based multimedia learning

the cross-cutting edge

Unique contributions of eye-tracking research to the study of learning with graphics

Direct Measurement of Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning

Metacognitive Load Useful, or Extraneous Concept? Metacognitive and Self- Regulatory Demands in Computer-Based Learning

A SIMPLIFIED METHOD FOR TEACHING CASH FLOW STATEMENT CONSTRUCTION TO ENTREPRENEURSHIP AND NON-ACCOUNTING MAJORS

Eye tracking as a tool to study and enhance multimedia learning

Defining core competencies of an instructional technologist

Animation as an Aid to Multimedia Learning

Cognitive Load Based Adaptive Assistive Technology Design for Reconfigured Mobile Android Phone

This historical document is derived from a 1990 APA presidential task force (revised in 1997).

How To Design A Computer Game

1. Cognitive load theory and user interface design: Making software easy to learn and use (Part 1)

Interactive and non-interactive pictures in multimedia learning environments: Effects on learning outcomes and learning efficiency

The Impact of Applied Cognitive Learning Theory on Engagement with elearning Courseware

The promise of multimedia learning: using the same instructional design methods across different media

The promise of multimedia learning: using the same instructional design methods across different media

Learning in High-Tech and Multimedia Environments Roxana Moreno

Introduction to Multimedia Learning

Using Inventor Analysis and Simulation to Enhance Students Comprehension of Stress Analysis Theories in a Design of Machine Elements Course.

WHAT IS COGNITIVE INTERVENTION/REHABILITATION?

An Introduction to Online Teaching and Learning. Good teaching is good teaching, no matter how it s done.

Research Basis for Catchup Math

ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY FACTORS FOR COMPUTING PROGRAMMING LANGUAGES LEARNING (CPLL)

Implications of Designing Instructional Video Using Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning

APPLYING COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY TO THE DESIGN OF ONLINE LEARNING. Kate M. Erland Burkes, B.A., M.A. Dissertation Prepared for the Degree of

Dmitrii Paniukov Jongpil Cheon Steven M. Crooks Texas Tech University. Abstract. Introduction. Global Lunar Patterns

Overloading on Slides: Cognitive Load Theory and Microsoft s Slide Program PowerPoint

The subject of this chapter, ten steps to complex

Learning the triage procedure. Serious gaming based on guided discovery learning versus studying worked examples. V.

KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION

How To Test The Theory Of The Worked Example

A Proposed Framework for Integration of Visualization Techniques and Active Learning Strategy in Wireless Communication Network

COMPREHENSION OF GRAPHS SUPPORTED BY SUPPLANTATION OF POINT-TO-OBJECT OPERATIONS

Software maintenance. Software Maintenance. Fundamental problems. Maintenance activities. Lehman/Belady model of evolution. Topics

Chapter LXVI Instructional Game Design Using Cognitive Load Theory

The Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning

Overview of Multimedia Instruction

The Four-Component Instructional Design Model: Multimedia Principles in Environments for Complex Learning. Jeroen J. G.

REFLECTIONS ON LEARNING THEORIES IN INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN 1

An Application of Contemporary Learning Theory to Online Course Textbook Selection

Measuring effectiveness of graph visualizations: A cognitive load perspective

EDT 503: Instructional Media Design

Development and Design: E- Portfolio on Project-based Learning with Assessment System

Training and Development (T & D): Introduction and Overview

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

A Cognitive Approach to Instructional Design for Multimedia Learning

Working Memory and Learning

Using the Multi-Display Teaching System to Lower Cognitive Load

Paas, F., Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Van Gog, T. (in press). Designing instruction

Cognitive Psychology

Mastery approaches to mathematics and the new national curriculum

Areas of Processing Deficit and Their Link to Areas of Academic Achievement

When Is It Best to Learn with All Worked Examples?

A STATISTICS COURSE FOR ELEMENTARY AND MIDDLE SCHOOL TEACHERS. Gary Kader and Mike Perry Appalachian State University USA

The Effects of Still Images and Animated Images on. Motion-Related and Non-Motion Related Learning Tasks in

SIMULATION-BASED MEDICAL EDUCATION IN INTERNAL MEDICINE NING-ZI SUN MD MHPE DIVISION OF GENERAL INTERNAL MEDICINE DEPARTMENT OF MEDICINE

LEARNING TO PROGRAM USING PART-COMPLETE SOLUTIONS

An Analytics-Based Approach to Managing Cognitive Load by Using Log Data of Learning Management Systems and Footprints of Social Media

Don t Forget to Warm-Up Your Brain! Caroline Liddy. Before beginning a race, do you jump right into it and run as fast as you can,

Supporting Online Material for

Picture Memory Improves with Longer On Time and Off Time

Concept-Mapping Software: How effective is the learning tool in an online learning environment?

On a Deeper Understanding of the Constructivist Learning Principles and Constructivist Instructional Design

Research into competency models in arts education

Research & Innovation Network, Pearson; 2U of San Diego Mobile Technology Learning Center; 3National Network of State Teachers of the Year

(Advanced Preparation)

Applying the Science of Learning: Evidence- Based Principles for the Design of Multimedia Instruction

MODULE - I Foundations of Psychology

Introduction to 30th Anniversary Perspectives on Cognitive Science: Past, Present, and Future


Three Genres of Game Research. Richard E. Mayer University of California, Santa Barbara

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY. effectiveness of, the psychology of teaching, and the social psychology of schools as

E-Learning at Kyongju University in Seoul, Korea: the Present and the Future

Improving learning outcomes for first year introductory programming students

Designing Socio-Technical Systems to Support Guided Discovery-Based Learning in Students: The Case of the Globaloria Game Design Initiative

Program Visualization for Programming Education Case of Jeliot 3

The Effect of Static Visual Instruction on Students Online Learning: A Pilot Study

Skill acquisition. Skill acquisition: Closed loop theory Feedback guides learning a motor skill. Problems. Motor learning practice


Factors for the Acceptance of Enterprise Resource Planning (ERP) Systems and Financial Performance

Transcription:

APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 20: 281 285 (2006) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1244 Learner and Information Characteristics in the Design of Powerful Learning Environments FRED PAAS 1,2 * and LIESBETH KESTER 1 1 Educational Technology Expertise Center, Open University of the Netherlands 2 Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands SUMMARY This themed issue aims to present some current directions in cognitive load research. The contributions to this issue represent a compilation of symposia contributions to the 11th European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction (EARLI), 2005, in Nicosia, Cyprus. These cognitive load symposia were focused on empirical and theoretical perspectives on designing powerful learning environments by aligning learner characteristics, information characteristics, or both with the knowledge structures underlying the cognitive architecture. This article provides an introduction to cognitive load theory and the instructional design consequences of these characteristics, and a short overview of the contributions to this issue. Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. The central notion of cognitive load theory (CLT: Paas, Renkl, & Sweller, 2003, 2004; Sweller, 1999, 2004) is that if individuals are to learn effectively in a learning environment, the architecture of their cognitive system, the learning environment, and interactions between both must be understood, accommodated and aligned. The theory focuses on complex cognitive tasks, in which instructional control of cognitive load is critically important to meaningful learning. To realize this control, CLT uses current knowledge about the human cognitive architecture to generate instructional techniques. This architecture consists of an effectively unlimited long-term memory (LTM), which interacts with a working memory (WM) that is very limited in both capacity and duration; for new, yet to be learned information, the processing capacity is limited to only 4 plus or minus 1 element, and if not rehearsed, the information is lost within 30 seconds (Cowan, 2001). LTM contains cognitive schemas that are used to store and organize knowledge by incorporating multiple elements of information into a single element with a specific function. Skilled performance develops through the building of increasing numbers of ever more complex schemas by combining elements consisting of lower level schemas into higher level schemas. If the learning process has occurred over a long period of time, the schema may incorporate a huge amount of information. Because a schema can be treated by working memory as a single element, the limitations of WM disappear for more knowledgeable learners when dealing with previously learned information stored in LTM. *Correspondence to: Fred Paas, Educational Technology Expertise Center, Open University of the Netherlands, P.O. Box 2960, 6401 DL Heerlen, The Netherlands. E-mail: Fred.Paas@ou.nl Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

282 F. Paas and L. Kester The design of powerful learning environments, in which instructional conditions are aligned with the cognitive architecture, requires understanding of the learner characteristics that affect the underlying knowledge structures and their interactions with the learning task. Not surprisingly, learner expertise has been identified by cognitive load researchers as a key characteristic to consider in the design of instructional techniques (for an overview see, Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003). Kalyuga et al. (2003) have described a phenomenon called the expertise reversal effect, indicating that the effectiveness of instructional techniques depends very much on levels of learner expertise. Instructional techniques that are effective with inexperienced learners can lose their effectiveness and even have negative consequences when used with more experienced learners. So, as novice learners gain expertise, their requirements in learning materials change in accordance with their capacity for cognitive load. Another learner characteristic that is known to impact the knowledge structures underlying the cognitive architecture is learner age. This characteristic is particularly important in the context of the topical focus on lifelong learning. One of the central findings of cognitive ageing research is that the efficiency of cognitive operations declines with age in adults. A general and robust effect of cognitive ageing research is that agerelated declines in cognitive performance are most likely to emerge in complex cognitive tasks requiring effortful processing (Perfect & Maylor, 2000). The most prevalent explanations for the age-related cognitive declines are based on reduced WM capacity (e.g. Salthouse, Mitchell, Skovronek, & Babcock, 1989), slowed processing speed (e.g. Salthouse, 1996), difficulties inhibiting responses to irrelevant information (e.g. Hartman & Hasher, 1991), and deficits in integrative aspects of WM (e.g. Mayr, Kliegl, & Krampe, 1996). In general, the challenge of cognitive load researchers is to find instructional techniques that reduce the performance gap between young and old learners engaged in complex tasks (see e.g. Paas, Camp, & Rikers, 2001; Van Gerven, Paas, Van Merriënboer, & Schmidt, 2002). To that end, Paas, Van Gerven, and Tabbers (2005) have systematically mapped age-related cognitive declines on the potentially compensatory strategies that are offered by CLT. CLT argues that the interactions between learner and information characteristics can manifest as intrinsic or extrinsic cognitive load (for an overview see, Paas, Tuovinen, Tabbers, & Van Gerven, 2003). Intrinsic cognitive load is determined by the level of element interactivity of the information presented by a learning task, that is, the number of elements a learner has to attend to simultaneously to understand the learning material. With increasing expertise learners are able to deal with configurations of interacting elements as single units, thereby decreasing the intrinsic load of a task. So, intrinsic load is supposed to be determined by an interaction between the nature of the material being learned and the expertise of the learners. It cannot be directly influenced by instructional manipulations; only a simpler learning task that omits some interacting elements can be chosen to reduce this type of load. Although the omission of essential, interacting elements will compromise sophisticated understanding, it is considered unavoidable with very complex tasks that present high element interactivity information. Sophisticated understanding can only commence if, after subsequent additions of omitted elements, eventually, the learner is able to process all essential elements simultaneously. Besides the task-related intrinsic load, the manner in which the task information is presented to learners and the learning activities required of learners impose an instructional design-related extrinsic cognitive load. Where that load is unnecessary and so interferes with schema construction and automation, it is referred to as ineffective or extraneous. Where the

Learner and information characteristics 283 extrinsic load fosters schema construction and automation, it is referred to as effective or germane. Intrinsic and extrinsic cognitive load are additive in that together, the total load cannot exceed the working memory resources available if learning is to occur. That is why extraneous load is primarily important when intrinsic load is high because intrinsic and extrinsic load are additive. If intrinsic load is low, levels of extraneous load may be less important because total cognitive load may not exceed working memory capacity, and learners may be able to compensate for poor instructional design. As a consequence, instructional designs intended to manage cognitive load are primarily effective with complex cognitive tasks that present high element interactivity information. Extraneous and germane load can work in tandem in that, for example, a reduction in extraneous cognitive load can free capacity for an increase in germane cognitive load. Whereas, instructional strategies to lower extraneous load are well documented (see Sweller, Van Merriënboer, & Paas, 1998), strategies to induce germane load, and particularly, strategies to manage intrinsic load have only recently come to the attention of cognitive load researchers. With regard to intrinsic load, the general opinion up to now is that it should only be reduced if, even after removal of all sources of extraneous cognitive load, the element interactivity of the complex task-related information is still too high for efficient learning. In practice, this means that CLT recommends instructional designers or teachers to use germane load inducing methods only in combination with relatively simple tasks, in which the simultaneous processing of all interactive information elements leaves some spare cognitive capacity. However, there is hardly any information available about how intrinsic cognitive load can be reduced effectively. An exception is a theoretical framework presented by Van Merriënboer, Kirschner, and Kester (2003), which identifies simple-to-complex versions of the whole task as an approach to decrease intrinsic cognitive load. It is becoming clearer that the cognitive load effects found so far are highly dependent on characteristics of the learner and the information presented by the task. In fact, it appears that the different types of cognitive load can only be reliably categorized with knowledge of these characteristics. That is why we believe that research efforts to advance CLT can only be successful if they consistently take these characteristics into consideration. The contributions to this themed issue either deal with learner characteristics, characteristics of the information presented in learning tasks, or both, and as such can be considered timely in the sense that they try to advance our understanding about the mediating effects of learner and information characteristics. This themed issue begins with an article by Ayres, titled Impact of reducing intrinsic cognitive load on learning in a mathematical domain, which is one of the first studies investigating the effects of different approaches to reduce intrinsic load on learning. Two experiments focusing on the interaction between characteristics of the learner and the task information were conducted in the domain of mathematics. More specifically, groups of low and high prior knowledge were confronted with either part-tasks isolating the constituent elements, whole tasks fully integrating all elements, or mixed tasks progressing from part-tasks to whole tasks. The Olina, Reiser, Huang, Lim, and Park article Problem format and presentation sequence: Effects on learning and mental effort among US high school students, reports an experiment that examined the interaction between format and presentation sequence of practice problems for lower and higher achieving students on learning of comma rules in a real classroom setting. They compared the learning outcomes of lower and higher achieving students who were presented (1) cued problems that reduce extraneous load

284 F. Paas and L. Kester or conventional problems in (2) a random problem presentation order that increases germane load or a blocked problem presentation order. The article by Van Gerven, Paas, Van Merriënboer, and Schmidt, titled Modality and variability as factors in training the elderly, presents an investigation into the combined learning effects on a puzzle task of instructional modality and variability for young and elderly learners. Particularly, using both the visual and auditory modality was expected to reduce extraneous load and high-variability practice was expected to increase germane load, which leads to the hypothesis that this specific combination of instructional modality and variability would lead to the best learning results, especially for the elderly. The Seufert and Brünken article Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation focuses on the alignment of task information characteristics with the cognitive architecture by comparing the combined effects of two types of help and two types of support. The authors hypothesized that surface level help in the form of hyperlinks in combination with deep structure level help explaining the relations of corresponding structures would be the best instructional aid for constructing coherent mental representations. The article by Kalyuga, titled Assessment of learners organized knowledge structures in adaptive learning environments, focuses on tailoring instruction to levels of learner expertise by dynamically aligning learner and task information characteristics with the knowledge structures underlying the cognitive architecture. Two rapid assessment techniques that either adapted the learning tasks to the learner s performance or to the learner s performance in combination with the associated cognitive load were compared to a non-adaptive control condition. The Van Merriënboer, Kester, and Paas article, Teaching complex rather than simple tasks: Balancing intrinsic and germane load to enhance transfer of learning, argues that in contrast to the general opinion that germane load should only be induced if the intrinsic load leaves enough spare capacity to profit from it, learning tasks should always be combined with methods that induce germane cognitive load. They argue that the element interactivity of learning tasks should be limited early in training to decrease intrinsic load, so that germane-load inducing methods might be used right from the start of the training programme. Finally, the discussion articles by Sweller and Rikers close this themed issue. We hope that the collection of articles in this themed issue makes cognitive load researchers realize that powerful learning environments can only be designed by using a CLT that provides guidelines for (dynamically) aligning characteristics of the learner and the information presented by the task with the cognitive architecture. REFERENCES Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 87. Hartman, M., & Hasher, L. (1991). Aging and suppression: memory for previously relevant information. Psychology and Aging, 6, 587 594. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23 31. Mayr, U., Kliegl, R., & Krampe, R. T. (1996). Sequential and coordinative processing dynamics in figural transformations across the life span. Cognition, 59, 61 90.

Learner and information characteristics 285 Paas, F., Camp, G., & Rikers, R. (2001). Instructional compensation for age-related cognitive declines: effects of goal specificity in maze learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 181 186. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1 4. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 1 8. Paas, F., Tuovinen, J., Tabbers, H., & Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist, 38, 63 71. Paas, F., Van Gerven, P. W. M., & Tabbers, H. K. (2005). The cognitive aging principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 339 354). New York: Cambridge University Press. Perfect, T. J., & Maylor, E. A. (2000). Rejecting the dull hypothesis: the relation between method and theory in cognitive aging research. In T. J. Perfect, & E. A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 1 18). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403 428. Salthouse, T. A., Mitchell, D. R. D., Skovronek, E., & Babcock, R. L. (1989). Effects of adult age and working memory on reasoning and spatial abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 507 516. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257 285. Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. Camberwell, Australia: ACER Press. Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 9 31. Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251 296. Van Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2002). Cognitive load theory and aging: effects of worked examples on training efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 38, 87 107. Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load of a learners mind: instructional design for complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 5 13.