Rational bias in forensic science

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Rational bias in forensic science"

Transcription

1 Law, Probability and Risk (2010) 9, Advance Access publication on January 18, 2010 doi: /lpr/mgp028 Rational bias in forensic science GLEN WHITMAN Associate Professor of Economics, Department of Economics, California State University, Northridge, Nordhoff Street, Northridge, CA , USA AND ROGER KOPPL Professor of Economics and Finance, Department of Economics and Finance and Institute for Forensic Science Administration, Silberman School of Business, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Madison, NJ 07940, USA [Received on 26 May 2009; revised on 1 October 2009] The current organization of forensic science induces biases in the conduct of forensic science even if forensic scientists are perfectly rational. Assuming forensic examiners are flawless Bayesian statisticians helps us to identify structural sources of error that we might otherwise have undervalued or missed altogether. Specifically, forensic examiners conclusions are affected not just by objective test results but also by two subjective factors: their prior beliefs about a suspect s likely guilt or innocence and the relative importance they attach to convicting the guilty rather than the innocent. The authorities police and prosecutors implicitly convey information to forensic examiners by their very decision to submit samples for testing. This information induces the examiners to update their prior beliefs in a manner that results in a greater tendency to provide testimony that incriminates the defendant. Forensic results are in a sense contaminated by the prosecution and thus do not provide jurors with an independent source of information. Structural reforms to address such problems of rational bias include independence from law enforcement, blind proficiency testing and separation of test from interpretation. Keywords: forensic science; bias; Bayesian; NAS report; organization. 1. Introduction The report of the National Academy of Sciences (NAS), Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward (NAS Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic Sciences Community, 2009), notes that dependence on law enforcement creates the risk of bias. Forensic scientists who sit administratively in law enforcement agencies or prosecutors offices, or who are hired by those units, are subject to a general risk of bias (p. 6 2). We will examine this and other sources of bias in forensic science as it is currently organized in the USA. The potential biases we examine are largely attributable to the institutional structure of forensic science rather than the cognitive limits of individual forensic scientists. To draw out the consequences of factors that do not glen.whitman@gmail.com koppl@fdu.edu c The Author [2010]. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.

2 70 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL depend on cognitive limits, we will assume that forensic scientists are free of them and in this sense rational. The NAS report identifies three key features of the institutional structure of forensic science in the USA today, namely, fragmentation, lack of oversight and dependence on law enforcement. The report neglects, however, the important fact of monopoly. Forensic science today is characterized by a twofold monopoly. First, evidence is typically examined by one crime lab only. In this sense, the crime lab receiving a bit of evidence has a monopoly on examination of that evidence. Second, that same lab will normally be the only one to offer an interpretation of the results of the examination it performs. No other experts in forensic science will be asked to judge what the evidence means. Our analysis implicitly recognizes that dependence on law enforcement is frequently combined with a monopoly in interpretation for the crime lab. We ignore, however, the monopoly in examination. Thus, our analysis is not meant to cover all ways in which current institutions may induce bias in forensic examinations. The NAS report relies on and contributes to a substantial literature identifying problems in the conduct of forensic science. Much of this literature identifies biases in the work of forensic scientists. Such biases are often regarded as irrational. Risinger et al. (1989), e.g. describe handwriting identification as irrational evidence (p. 779, note 213). Much of the most recent literature draws on standard cognitive science results showing that humans are not rational decision makers in the strict sense laid down by modern statistics and decision theory. 1 Dror and Charlton (2006) point to a class of errors by experts that represent epistemological problems that derive from the mechanisms of human cognition and the workings of the mind (p. 602). They view such errors as deviations from objectivity (p. 614). Dror et al. (2005) show that emotionally charged information, such as a grisly photograph of a murder victim, increases the probability of falsely matching fingerprints; in traditional rational choice models, such affective priming would not alter performance on cognitive tasks. The important article of Risinger et al. (2002) discusses biasing by domain-irrelevant information. While Risinger et al. do not label such induced biases irrational, they do not acknowledge that some induced biases may be consistent with rational choice. In this article, we argue that the current organization of forensic science induces biases in the conduct of forensic science even if forensic scientists are perfectly rational. In particular, we will model them as rational Bayesian statisticians. We recognize, indeed affirm, that they may not be and that irrationalities of human cognition may induce other biases or strengthen the biases we identify. Assuming that forensic examiners are flawless Bayesian statisticians, however, helps us to identify structural sources of error that we might otherwise have undervalued or missed altogether. Specifically, forensic examiners conclusions are affected not just by objective test results but also by two subjective factors: their prior beliefs about a suspect s likely guilt or innocence and the relative importance they attach to convicting the guilty rather than the innocent. The authorities police and prosecutors implicitly convey information to forensic examiners by their very decision to submit samples for testing. This information induces the examiners to update their prior beliefs in a manner that results in a greater tendency to provide testimony that incriminates the defendant. It may seem unnatural or inappropriate to model forensic scientists as rational in the rather narrow sense of flawless Bayesian statistician, although there is precedent for it (Phillips et al., 2001). In doing so, however, we are not saying that forensic scientists always understand and consciously apply statistical reasoning to their jobs. Many of our fellow economists would defend our model as 1 Savage (1972), originally published in 1954, is a classic statement of this conception of rationality.

3 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 71 capturing factors that influence forensic scientists in more or less the way the model suggests, regardless of how those influences are experienced subjectively by the forensic scientist. This idea is a bit like saying that a motorist implicitly calculates a coefficient of friction when deciding whether to pass another motorist in the rain. There is something to this notion and we have supported a highly qualified version of it in the past (Koppl and Whitman, 2004). In this paper, however, we are making an even weaker claim. We are saying that the biases we identify are hard to get rid of. We are saying the biases we identify would exist even if forensic scientists were perfectly rational in the sense we have identified. We are saying that bias is a larger and more difficult problem than we might already have recognized in the context of the flawed mechanisms of human cognition noted by Dror and Charlton (2006) and others. 2 We suggest several changes to the organization of forensic science that would help to reduce the influence of such extrinsic factors. We endorse the NAS call for independence of crime labs from law enforcement (p. 6 7). We endorse the call of Koppl et al. (2008) for sequential unmasking, which requires sequencing the laboratory workflow such that evidentiary samples are interpreted, and the interpretation is fully documented, before reference samples are compared (Krane et al., 2008, p. 1006). In other words, examiners are exposed to potentially biasing information only after making decisions that might be biased by such information. We support blind proficiency testing. We make a qualified defense of evidence line-ups of the sort Miller (1987) describes. Finally, we follow Koppl (2005) in calling for a separation of test and interpretation. Test results would be forwarded for interpretation to forensic experts for both defense and prosecution. 2. Subjective judgement and categorical testimony in forensic science Forensic science evidence is often ambiguous, and the analysis is generally subjective. In spite of such ambiguity and subjective judgement, forensic science testimony is generally categorical, essentially conveying a match or no match decision. The tension exists within an institutional structure that frequently puts crime labs under the administration of law enforcement agencies. 2.1 Institutional dependence and subjective judgement Today, most forensic science work in our criminal justice system is conducted in government crime labs, and most of these labs are organized as a part of a police agency. The NAS report says, The majority of forensic science laboratories are administered by law enforcement agencies, such as police departments, where the laboratory administrator reports to the head of the agency (p. 6 1). The current process does not commonly produce multiple examinations by the defense experts or independent experts (Thompson, 1995, p. 167). Noting this fact, the NAS report says, prosecutors usually have an advantage over most defendants in offering expert testimony in criminal cases (p. S-8). Giannelli (2004) finds that the defense right to expert assistance recognized by the Supreme Court in the 1985 case Ake v. Oklahoma has not been effectively implemented (p. 1419). The NAS report makes repeated references to the extensive role of subjective judgement in forensic science. Forensic examination involves subjective judgement and interpretation at almost every step. Subjective judgements can go one way or the other. Thus, they may depend on factors extrinsic to the evidence. Importantly, for this paper, they allow the match decision to depend on the examiner s preferences and prior beliefs as well as the evidence. 2 Even the question of whether such mechanisms are flawed is contentious (Cosmides and Tooby, 1994).

4 72 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL The NAS report underplays the importance of subjective judgement in DNA analysis. Even DNA profiling, however, often entails subjective judgement. The NAS report says that several historical facts combined to make DNA typing less subjective than it had been at its inception (p. 5 5). DNA test results are often unambiguous. Nevertheless, DNA tests sometimes produce ambiguous results that are subject to multiple interpretations and [w]hen interpreting ambiguous results... human analysts rely heavily on subjective judgments to distinguish signal from noise, explain anomalies, and account for discrepancies (Thompson and Cole, 2007, p. 34). Figure 1 illustrates an unambiguous case. The biological sample is prepared and run through a machine called a genetic analyser, which produces data that are represented as an electropherogram, which is then interpreted by a forensic scientist. As Fig. 1 reveals, an electropherogram is a squiggly line. The forensic scientist looks at the squiggly line to see where the peaks are. The pattern of peaks is the person s genetic profile. Because each peak represents an allele, the electropherogram will show one or two peaks at each locus depending on whether each parent contributed distinct or identical alleles at that locus. 3 The eletropherogram of Fig. 1 is unambiguous; there is no substantial question about how many peaks there are and where they appear. Figure 2 illustrates some of the ambiguities that can enter DNA profiling. When DNA is degraded or the original sample contains a small quantity of DNA, it can be difficult to distinguish between signal and noise. Is the spot labelled 12 in the figure high enough to be a peak? Perhaps, but it might be noise or a technical artifact, i.e. a random blip with no meaning. The spot labelled OL Allele would be recognized by DNA analysts as a blob (caused by some random impurity) and not a peak. But we do not know whether the blob hides a peak. With small or degraded samples, allelic dropout may cause some peaks to fall away for no particular reason and allelic drop in (contamination) may cause spurious peaks to appear. Thus, many profiles are consistent with the evidence represented in Fig When dealing with small or degraded samples, it can be hard to know which squiggles of the electropherogram are meaningful peaks and which squiggles are meaningless noise. Thompson (2009) develops this and other examples at length. Under the protocols of most crime labs in the USA, he says, Whether the comparison of profiles results in a finding of inclusion, exclusion or inconclusive is an entirely subjective determination. Mixed samples are another source of ambiguity and subjective judgement. They are particularly challenging when potential contributors have several alleles in common..., when stochastic variations in peak heights occur, or when technical artifacts such as stutter, allelic dropout, and degradation/inhibition occur (Paoletti et al., 2005, p. 1). DNA analysts typically rely on subjective judgement to disentangle the threads and identify the genetic profiles of the separate DNA contributors. To help them correctly reconstruct the separate DNA profiles of the separate contributors to the biological sample, they generally have a suspect s DNA profile before them. In other words, they go into the test with a cheat sheet. Koppl et al. (2008) object to this practice. 3 Each of the marked intervals on the electropherogram corresponds to a locus, a particular stretch of DNA. Half of a person s DNA (ignoring mitochondrial DNA) comes from the person s mother and half from the father. An allele is the pattern found at a locus on one of those halves. Thus, at each locus, there will be one allele contributed by the mother and one possibly identical allele contributed by the father. A particular allele at a locus determines where a peak will appear within the interval of the electropherogram that corresponds to the DNA. Thus, a person who is heterozygotic (or heterozygous ) at a given locus will have two distinct alleles and an electropherogram with two peaks in the relevant interval. A person who is homozygotic (or homozygous ) at a given locus will have two copies of the same allele at that locus and, therefore, an electropherogram with one peak in the relevant interval. 4 Figures 1, 2 show only a three-locus segment of the full electropherogram.

5 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 73 FIG. 1. An unambiguous electropherogram. Figure 1 reflects the fact that DNA analysts may interpret the crime scene evidence with the genetic profiles of competing suspects before them. This practice makes the DNA examination a multiple-choice test. We do not have data on the relative frequency of ambiguous and unambiguous DNA evidence in criminal casework in the USA. Such data would be difficult to gather in part because case files can be difficult to access (Gold, 2008) and the underlying evidence is sometimes destroyed (Greene and Moffiet, 2007). Several considerations suggest that DNA evidence may be ambiguous in a non-trivial fraction of cases, however. First, as Thompson (2009) notes, evidentiary samples are often mixtures

6 74 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL FIG. 2. An ambiguous electropherogram. and mixtures are often ambiguous. Second, the use of partial samples is increasing (Moore, 2009) and such samples, when arising from degraded evidence, may produce the sort of ambiguity represented by Fig. 2. Finally, there is some evidence that some crime labs may be using very small samples, which gives rise to Low Copy Number or low template analysis (California v. Hector Espino, no. NA07660). The New York City Office of the Chief Medical Examiner issued a letter in November 2006 saying that the lab has been performing Low Copy DNA testing in criminal casework since January Overall, it seems fair to say, as we have, that DNA evidence may be ambiguous in a non-trivial fraction of cases. Thompson (2009) provides further evidence supporting our view. According to the NAS report, subjective judgement also enters other forensic disciplines, including fingerprint examinations (p. 5 9), firearms identification, which is often called ballistics (p. 5 21), shoeprints and tire tracks (pp. 5 15, 5 20), handwriting comparisons (p. 5 29) and bloodstain pattern analysis (p. 5 29). Dror and Rosenthal (2008), Phillips et al. (2001) and Schwartz (2004, 2005) provide further evidence and analysis. The NAS report notes that the extreme disaggregation of forensic science training, practice and organization has produced different professional cultures and standards for performance, which raises the worrisome prospect that the quality of evidence presented in court, and its interpretation, can vary unpredictably according to jurisdiction (NAS Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic Sciences Community, 2009, p. S 11). Phillips et al. (2001, p. 299) make a similar argument. When forensic labs are operated in conjunction with police and prosecutors, forensic examiners know that most or all of their samples will come from the authorities. We will demonstrate later that this fact alone generates a bias in the interpretation of evidence. But we should also note that typically the authorities explicitly inform the forensic examiners about the facts of the case and their theory of the crime (Thompson, 1995, especially pp ). 2.2 The match or no match assumption The model to be presented in Section 2 assumes that the forensic lab makes a binary decision after performing an examination: declare either a match or a no match. The NAS report notes, Many 5 A pdf scan of the letter is available from Koppl.

7 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 75 terms are used by forensic scientists in scientific reports and in court testimony that describe findings, conclusions, and degrees of association between evidentiary material (e.g. hairs, fingerprints, fibers) and particular people or objects. Such terms include, but are not limited to match, consistent with, identical, similar in all respects tested, and cannot be excluded as the source of (p. S-15). Because such terms may imply different degrees of association, our assumption that forensic scientists indicate either match or no match is a simplification. Nevertheless, our assumption reasonably approximates the reality of forensic testimony in American courts today. DNA identifications, e.g. are typically accompanied by testimony about the random match probability, which may be as low as one in several quadrillion, essentially zero. Over-claiming tends to produce match or no match testimony. Cole clarifies Friedman (2003, pp ) by defining over-claiming as exaggerating the probative value of knowledge claims (2007, p. 817). It has been identified as a serious problem in forensic science (Garrett and Neufeld, 2009; Cole, 2007, especially pp ; Friedman, 2003). Vague language such as probable or very likely is not necessarily overclaiming, but it easily slips into it and, in any event, often amounts to match or no match testimony. Match or no match testimony is the standard in latent print testimony (Cole, 2007, pp ; Phillips et al., 2001, p. 298) and may be the norm in toolmark testimony Cole (2007, p. 821) and bite mark testimony (Cole, 2007, pp ; see also Nelson, 2005; McRoberts, 2004) Forensic techniques often lack the supporting research necessary to make statements of confidence levels (Phillips et al., 2001, p. 299). At present, forensic testimony in our courts seems to be characterized by exaggerated claims of certainty, minute random match probabilities and oracular pronouncement. Such testimony leaves the jury able to understand only whether or not there has, supposedly, been a match. For this reason, we consider our assumption that the forensic scientist s testimony consists in declaring either match or no match a reasonable approximation of actual practice. 3. The model We will now present a model of forensic decision making that shows how decisions by the authorities to submit samples affect the forensic lab s decisions about how to report test results. Arkes and Mellers (2002) have applied a similar model to juries. The main elements in our model were anticipated in Phillips et al. (2002), some of whose conclusions are similar to some of ours (2002, especially p. 299). Our paper has a different set of purposes than Phillips et al. articulate (2002, p. 294), however, and draws out a different set of implications. The literatures on likelihood ratios and Bayesian approaches to forensic evidence are large (Berry, 1991; Koehler, 1996; Aitken, 2000; Champod, 2000; Biedermann et al., 2008, 2009; Bolck et al., 2009). These literatures are devoted to whether and how different statistical methods should be self-consciously applied by forensic scientists, whereas we are using our model in a semi-descriptive manner to build our even if argument. 3.1 The forensic lab s decision process We assume that the authorities provide the forensic lab with a sample from a suspect on which the lab will run a test. Based on the test result, the lab will report either a match or a no match to a sample from the crime scene.

8 76 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL Let δ be the lab s prior probability of the suspect being guilty and (1 δ) the prior probability of innocence. For an imaginary lab whose personnel always think that the police suspect just has to be guilty, δ would be close to 1. For an imaginary lab whose personnel always think that the police suspect just has to be innocent, δ would be close to 0. Presumably, the truth typically lies between these imaginary extremes. The value of δ depends, perhaps, not only on some overall assessment of the quality of local policing but also on factors such as the perceived overall rate of criminality in the relevant area and the profile of the suspect as perceived by lab personnel. Guilty and innocent suspects will generate different distributions of test results. Let r be a continuous variable corresponding to the result of the test. For illustrative purposes, it may be helpful to think of r as peak height on an electropherogram and to imagine that a sufficiently small peak height such as zero implies no match and a sufficiently large peak height implies match. In a more realistic example, r would be a vector somehow reflecting the differences between the suspect and crime scene electropherograms. For guilty suspects, test results are characterized by the probability density function f g (r) and for innocent suspects, f n (r). Assume that the mean test result for guilty suspects is strictly greater than that for innocent suspects. Thus, f g (r) tells us which values of r are more likely under the hypothesis that the suspect is guilty and f n (r) tells us which values of r are more likely under the hypothesis that the suspect is innocent. It is a simplification to assume that the probability density function for r depends on whether or not the police suspect is guilty. Someone other than the defendant might be the source of an evidentiary sample. For example, a test may be performed to determine whether DNA found on an alleged rape victim belongs to a consensual partner. To discuss source rather that guilt, however, would require us to enter into the complicated relationships that may exist between source and guilt. As important as such complexities may be in an individual case, they do not seem to affect the logic or inferences we develop in this paper. Given a test result r, the lab must update its belief about the likelihood of guilt. The updated probability will be as follows: Pr(guilty r) = It follows that the updated probability of innocence is: Pr(innocent r) = δ f g (r) δ f g (r) + (1 δ) f n (r). (1 δ) f n (r) δ f g (r) + (1 δ) f n (r). These expressions reflect the idea that guilt will seem to lab personnel the more probable hypothesis (relative to innocence) the higher δ and the higher f g (r) relative to f n (r). If there are some values of r that can only be generated by guilty suspects, and some values of r that can only be generated by innocent suspects, then for those values of r the updated probability of guilt will be either zero or one. But for any r that could in principle be generated by either an innocent or a guilty suspect, the lab needs to decide whether to report the result as a match or not. (Think of the spot labelled 12 in Fig. 2.) We suspect that this is the more typical case in many forensic disciplines. As r gets larger, it becomes more likely that the suspect is guilty, but no r is so high as to be absolutely definitive. Thus, the lab must decide on a threshold value of r above which the lab will report a match (and below which it will report no match). This is the interpretive role of the forensic scientist.

9 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 77 To find the match threshold, we need to know the values the lab places on conviction. Let u be the utility of convicting a guilty suspect, and let d be the disutility of convicting an innocent. 6 We will refer to u and d together as conviction utilities. It is a simplification to interpret u and d as utilities relating to conviction rather than judgements of source. As with our probability density functions discussed earlier, this simplification allows us to ignore the complexities that can arise in the relationship between source and guilt. Moreover, when forensic scientists have outcome preferences other than truth, they seem to be related mostly to questions of guilt and innocence rather than source (Kelly and Wearne, 1998, pp ; Thompson, 1995, p. 154). The conviction utilities describe something implicit in the analyst s choices and not necessarily something experienced consciously as known preferences or as feelings of pleasure and pain. In the psychological process of choice, these utility values might be experienced or rationalized ex post as differences in other parameters. For example, an analyst with a low disutility of convicting the innocent may experience low d as the belief that false-positive errors are highly unlikely. This reassuring thought might support two rather different beliefs. On Thursday, it might support the analyst s sincere belief that a spot such as the one labelled 12 in Fig. 2 is a peak reflecting the corresponding peak on the suspect s profile. The following Tuesday, it might support the same analyst s equally sincere conviction that an entirely similar spot is a meaningless artifact consistent with the absence of a peak at the corresponding point on the profile of that day s suspect. 7 As we shall note again later, a rational decision maker may not be consciously aware of all determinants of his or her choices. For simplicity, let the lab assume that its finding will translate directly into a verdict. Then, the lab will report a match if and only if the expected value of a conviction is greater than zero: [ which simplifies to: δ f g (r) δ f g (r) + (1 δ) f n (r) ] u [ (1 δ) f n (r) δ f g (r) + (1 δ) f n (r) f g (r) f n (r) > 1 δ d δ u. ] d > 0, This inequality defines the match threshold. Three results are immediately apparent: 1. The threshold depends on the prior likelihood of guilt. The larger is δ, the lower is the righthand side, and thus, the more likely the threshold is to be met. This means that it is important to consider the source of the lab s prior. We will address this question in Section The threshold depends on the ratio of the disutility of convicting an innocent to the utility of convicting the guilty. The smaller is this ratio (i.e. the more the lab cares about convicting the guilty relative to not convicting the innocent), the more likely is the lab to announce a match. 3. The threshold depends on the relative likelihood of the test result having derived from an innocent versus a guilty person. If we assume, as seems sensible, that increasing r results in a greater relative likelihood of guilt (i.e. the ratio f g (r)/f n (r) is increasing in r), then a higher r will make the lab more likely to announce a match. 6 In other words, u is the utility gain from convicting rather than not convicting a guilty suspect and d is the utility loss from convicting an innocent rather than not convicting an innocent person. 7 Thompson (2009), gives several forms of evidence tending to show that forensic DNA analysts sometimes shift their criteria for a match based on the DNA profile of the suspect. Dror and Charlton (2006) and Dror et al. (2006) provide examples of the sort of reversal described in the text.

10 78 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL (There are some special cases in which that assumption is not correct. If r is uniformly distributed, it is possible that an interval will exist in which the ratio f g (r)/f n (r) is not increasing in r, and thus, higher r does not make the lab more likely to announce a match. If r is discrete rather than continuous the lab s prior (δ) can be so high or so low that the lab report will be invariant to the test result.) 3.2 How the authorities influence the lab s decision process In the preceding section, the prior δ was taken as given. But it actually results from the updating of a prior prior, based on implicit information provided by the authorities i.e. the police or prosecutors who submitted the sample to the lab. When the authorities have a potential suspect, they will send a sample to the lab some fraction a of the time for guilty suspects and some fraction b of the time for innocent suspects. We assume a > b; i.e. the authorities ask for the lab s input when there is already independent reason to believe that the suspect is guilty and guilty parties are more likely to have generated such independent reasons. Some evidence supports our expectation that a > b. Risinger et al. (2002) report that Peterson et al. (1984) found that, on average, fewer than 10% of all reports disassociated a suspect from the crime scene or from connection to the victim (pp ). Risinger et al. note that, This high rate of inculpation comes from the fact that each piece of evidence connected with any suspect has a heightened likelihood of being inculpatory, since investigators do not select suspects or evidence at random, but only those they have some reason to think were connected to the crime. Thus, forensic scientists have a continuing expectation that the evidence before them is inculpatory (p. 48). Let γ be the lab s prior probability that someone chosen at random from the general population is guilty. (The argument would be the same if we considered relatively broad subpopulations such as black males or short white women fitting a victim s description or other relevant information.) Thus, γ is the prior for someone without a sample from that person having been submitted. The table below summarizes the distribution of cases as perceived by the lab: Case sent to lab Case not sent to lab Guilty person γ a γ (1 a) Innocent person (1 γ )b (1 γ )(1 b) Given this distribution, the lab will update its beliefs about as-yet-untested suspects like so: δ = Pr(guilt y submitted) = 1 δ = Pr(innocent submitted) = γ a γ a + (1 γ )b, γ a γ a + (1 γ )b. Thus, the lab s prior belief δ is actually an updated belief which takes into account the implicit information provided by the authorities decision to submit a sample at all. This updating occurs before forensic testing begins. It is straightforward to prove that δ, and thus δ/(1 δ), is increasing in a and decreasing in b. If the authorities were just as likely to send samples from innocent suspects as they were to submit

11 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 79 samples from guilty suspects (i.e. if a and b were equal), then we would have δ/(1 δ) = γ /(1 γ ). But as a increases relative to b, the value of δ/(1 δ) rises, thereby making the lab s match threshold more likely to be satisfied. In short, the very choice to submit a suspect s sample to the lab makes the lab more inclined (than it would be otherwise) to announce a match, indicating that the suspect is guilty. 4. Information bleed Forensic science should reduce the error rates that would otherwise exist in the criminal justice system. If even the best forensic evidence were shown to increase error rates, then judges might be tempted to exclude it as prejudicial. To reduce errors in the criminal justice system, forensic scientists must bring new and independent scientific information to the process. As our model shows, however, the current organization of forensic science encourages forensic scientists to discount the scientific information they generate. This can be seen by expressing the match threshold as f n(r) f g (r) 1 δ δ d u < 1. The scientific information is f n(r) f g (r) 1 δ. It is discounted by the factor δ d u, which is likely to be less than one. In this sense, the organization of forensic science creates an information bleed. The organization of forensic science causes valuable scientific information to bleed out of the system, thus reducing the contribution forensic science makes to the criminal justice system. Information bleed turns a relatively objective scientific result into a relatively subjective personal judgement that depends on non-scientific information in the case file. As discussed in Section 1, forensic experts typically convey their conclusions to juries, rather than conveying evidence revealed in the testing process. In terms of our model, they tell the jury whether there was a match rather than telling the jury the ratio f n(r) f g (r). Forensic testimony incorporates some information provided by the authorities, which means that forensic testimony does not provide an independent source of information to the jury. As Bikhchandani et al. (1992, p. 1009) observe in the similar context of information cascades, the benefit of diverse information sources is attenuated when decision makers discount their information relative to that of others. When people infer information from the actions of prior decision makers, their own actions convey less information to future decision makers. Banerjee (1992, p. 798) rightly says,... the very act of trying to use the information contained in the decisions made by others makes each person s decision less responsive to her own information and hence less informative to others. In this case, the lab may ignore or give too little weight to the ratio f n(r) f g (r). Labs will sometimes engage in a verification procedure in which samples deemed a match by one examiner will be submitted to a second or even a third examiner to confirm the match. This is the case, for instance, with fingerprint matching. Remarkably, a fingerprint verifier is usually informed of the first examiner s conclusion. According to an Office of the Inspector General (OIG) Report,... although the verifier was aware of the fact that the first examiner had made an identification, the verifier would not know which features in the print were relied upon by the initial examiner in reaching his conclusion (OIG, 2006, p. 115). Applying the same logic as in our basic model, we predict that the verifier s updated assessment of the likelihood of guilt (δ) will be even larger than the first examiner s. He will therefore be more likely to announce a match than if he thought he was performing a first-round test. Thus, it is no surprise that a refused verification was as [sic] an extremely unusual event (OIG, 2006, p. 115).

12 80 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL An underappreciated aspect of the ACE-V method of fingerprint identification encourages further information bleed (ACE-V stands for Analysis, Comparison, Evaluation and Verification. ). As the official guidelines of the Scientific Working Group on Friction Ridge Analysis, Study and Technology (SWGFAST) are currently written, they do not prohibit verification shopping, whereby a failure of verification may be ignored and another verification sought. SWGFAST (2006) guidelines say, When examiners have conflicting conclusions, a quality review shall be conducted. It is the responsibility of the agency to determine whether corrective action is appropriate. The required quality review must be documented, and such documentation must include A review of case documentation. The case file has all case documentation. Thus, the documentation of conflicting conclusions seems not to be intended for the case file, nor do the SWGFAST guidelines require that such documentation or even the fact of conflicting conclusions be included in the case file. While the SWGFAST guidelines sonorously pronounce certain types of conflicting conclusions to imply serious error, they require corrective action only as deemed appropriate (SWGFAST, 2006, p. 6). We might view the failure of SWGFAST guidelines to prohibit verification shopping as an inconsequential omission if we had reason to believe that practices and procedures in local agencies effectively prevented verification shopping. It seems clear that verification shopping is generally recognized to be inappropriate, even unethical. 8 Nevertheless, there are at least two documented cases in which verification shopping seems to have been tolerated as a matter of policy. A Seminole County fingerprint scandal erupted in Spring 2007 when latent print examiner Tara Williamson issued a memo accusing her co-worker Donna Birks of misbehavior and incompetence (Stutzman, 2007a,b; Williamson, 2007). One of her specific charges regarded shopping verifications. Birks could not get verification for a particular print from two persons she approached in the lab. The print was then sent to a retired [fingerprint] examiner [from the same office] who one year earlier medically retired early and had admittedly lost his eye for latent prints. This examiner should not have been deemed competent and not allow to verify the print for such reasons (see SWGFAST Quality Assurance Guidelines for Latent Print Examiners. p ). (The underlining and grammatical error were both in the original memo.) Note that the problem was seeking verification from someone who was not competent. The problem was not shopping the verification. The second example comes from an official report on the case of Brandon Mayfield, whom the FBI mistakenly identified as the source of a print left at the scene of the Madrid train bombing. The [FBI s Latent Print Unit (LPU)] Quality Assurance Manual provided that if the second examiner reached a different conclusion, the matter must be referred to the supervisor and/or the Unit Chief for resolution. No formal statistics regarding the frequency of this occurrence have been maintained by the LPU, but LPU witnesses interviewed by the OIG stated that a refused verification was as an extremely unusual event. One option available to the supervisor was to select another verifier if the first verifier declined to confirm the identification. In that instance, there was no policy requiring that the first verifier s disagreement be documented in the case file. (OIG, 2006, p. 115) The report does not suggest that there was verification shopping in the Mayfield case. But it does reveal that it was considered just fine to shop your verifications. 8 See, for comments suggesting that most fingerprint examiners recognize that verification shopping is inappropriate, but that some instances of it are thought to be legitimate cases of conflict resolution.

13 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 81 Verification shopping increases information bleed and increases the importance of subjective factors. In terms of the mathematical symbols of our model, verification shopping causes the ratio f n (r) δ f g (r) to depend in part on the ratios 1 δ and d u. Verification shopping is more likely the more ambiguous the underlying evidence is, the higher the prior probability is guilt is and higher the utility of convicting a guilty suspect relative to the disutility of convicting an innocent subject. 5. Applications & extensions The threshold condition for announcing a match suggests numerous applications and extensions. To summarize again, the threshold condition indicates (a) that a match is more likely to be announced when the prior probability of guilt is larger; (b) that a match is more likely to be announced when the ratio of disutility (from convicting an innocent) to utility (from convicting the guilty) is lower and (c) that a match is more likely to be announced when the probability of the test result for an guilty person over the probability of the test result for an innocent person is higher. 5.1 Nature of the crime Forensic conclusions can be affected by the perceived heinousness of the crime. Dror et al. (2005), in a controlled experiment on fingerprint examinations, showed that fingerprint examiners were more likely to find a match when they had been given a description of a crime involving physical harm to a person, such as murder or assault, and they were less likely to find a match when given a description of a crime involving no harm to a person, such as bicycle theft or burglary. 9 As mentioned earlier, these results could be interpreted as a form of irrationality (affective priming) interfering with performance of a cognitive task. But they also can be understood as rational in light of the threshold condition. If the threat of physical violence increases the utility of conviction (u) more than the disutility of conviction (d), which seems likely since violent acts inspire fear and greater desire to prevent future occurrences, then the d/u ratio should be lower for violent crimes, making a match more likely. Simply put, forensic examiners may have different preferences about different types of crime, and they rationally follow their preferences. Dror et al. (2005) also find that the greater tendency to find matches for violent crimes only occurred in the context of ambiguous comparisons (such as when fingerprints were smudged or smeared) and not in the context of unambiguous comparisons (when the prints were nearly identical or clearly different by experimental design). This again fits with the present model as an unambiguous match occurs when either f n (r) or f n (r) is zero; as a result, the threshold condition is automatically met or not met, regardless of the value of d/u. 5.2 Racial prejudice and stereotyping Racial prejudice and stereotyping can affect the forensic examination process in two distinct ways. First, some groups may correctly or not be regarded as more likely to commit offenses in the first place. If examiners are made aware of a suspect s membership in such a group, the prior probability of guilt will be revised upward, making a match finding more likely. 9 This study, unlike Dror and Charlton (2006), used naïve subjects rather than working fingerprint examiners.

14 82 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL Second, the utility and disutility attached to conviction of guilty and innocent suspects may differ across groups. A white examiner might, consciously or unconsciously, care less about convicting an innocent member of a racial minority especially if he harbors the suspicion that the suspect probably did something wrong anyway, even if not the crime in question. He might also attach greater utility to convicting guilty members of a racial minority if he thinks that group s members are more likely to commit crimes in the future. As a result, d/u will tend to be lower for suspects in disfavored minorities, thereby raising the likelihood of a match being found. By contrast, groups looked upon more favorably and considered less likely to commit crimes than others such as Koreans, Jews and women will tend to have fewer matches found. If ex-post rates of conviction affect the formation of priors about different groups propensities to commit crimes, then we face the disturbing prospect of a feedback effect in which larger priors lead to higher convictions which lead to yet larger priors. Small or even non-existent differences between groups could lead to disproportionate differences in conviction outcomes. Bunzel and Marcoul (2008) reach such a result for arrest rates arising from traffic stops, although their model assumes an irrational overconfidence among police to motivate the initial misperception of relative rates of criminal behavior. 5.3 Pro-prosecution bias If forensic examiners identify with the prosecution more than the defense, perhaps because they are directly employed by the authorities or consider themselves part of that team, then the utility and disutility they attach to convictions will tend to be similar to those of the prosecution i.e. a smaller d/u ratio. As a result, we should expect more matches to be announced. This form of pro-prosecution bias, it should be noted, is not a cognitive error; again, it is simply a difference in preferences that leads to different results. Furthermore, the more confidence forensic examiners have in the competence of the authorities, the more likely they will be to announce a match. This follows from the contribution of the parameters a and b to the lab s updated prior. Greater confidence in the authorities competence corresponds to larger a (greater chance of submitting a sample from a guilty person) and smaller b (lower chance of submitting a sample from an innocent person), both of which will increase the lab s prior δ. Just as with violent-versus-non-violent crimes, the pro-prosecution bias will naturally have its greatest impact when test results are more ambiguous. When test results are unambiguous, the match threshold is automatically satisfied or falsified, and thus, the ratio of conviction utilities is irrelevant. At the other extreme, when the evidence is thoroughly ambiguous offering no further evidence of the suspect s guilt or innocence the lab s prior and conviction utilities will fully determine the outcome. This effect is best illustrated by the case of uniform distributions, where all ambiguous outcomes are interpreted as matches if the lab s prior is large enough. The variety of pro-prosecution bias described here can also help explain forensic malfeasance, i.e. cases in which a forensic examiner actually falsifies or misreports his test results. As shown in the case of discrete distributions, a lab s prior and conviction utilities can lead it to report a match despite a negative test result (so long as there is any chance of a false negative). This result might describe what happened in the case of Texas v. George Rodriguez, as summarized in an expert review of the case We do not know whether Bolding s apparent error was willful or not. We are claiming only that the hypothesis that it was willful might be explained in part by a pro-prosecution bias.

15 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 83 [Houston pathologist] Jim Bolding s trial testimony... contains egregious misstatements of conventional serology. These statements reveal that either the witness lacked a fundamental understanding of the most basic principles of blood typing analysis or he knowingly gave false testimony to support the State s case against George Rodriguez. His testimony is completely contrary to generally accepted scientific principles. (Blake et al., 2004, p. 5) Pro-prosecution bias as a source of malfeasance also seems an accurate explanation for rogue forensic experts such as Steven Hayne and Michael West, both of whom have helped prosecutors convict numerous defendants on the basis of flimsy forensic evidence (Balko, 2007a,b). As Radley Balko reports in an investigative exposé of bad forensics, During the last two decades, there have been more than a dozen high-profile cases in which dubious forensic witnesses conned state and federal courts, sometimes for many years and in hundreds of cases (Balko, 2007b, p. 38). 5.4 Bogus techniques Some techniques arguably have little or no scientific validity. In the extreme, the distributions f n (r) or f n (r) will be identical, and thus, the matching threshold will depend entirely on prior beliefs and conviction utilities. For example, the FBI long attributed a greater epistemic value to compositional analysis of bullet lead ( bullet-lead analysis ) than now seems justified (National Research Council 2004). Solomon (2007) suggests that there may be hundreds of American prisoners today who were wrongly convicted on such evidence. Other questionable techniques include lip-print analysis and ear-print analysis (Moenssens et al., 1995; McRoberts and Possley, 2006; State v. Kunze). Nowdiscredited techniques of fire investigation contributed to the wrongful execution of Cameron Todd Willingham (Mills and Possley, 2004, Grann, 2009). One explanation for the testimony in these cases is simply that forensic experts mistakenly believed in false or unproven theories. But we suggest a complementary explanation: that some forensic analysts apply the techniques and then reach conclusions driven largely or entirely by their priors and conviction utilities. Their decision to behave in this way might be motivated in part by the aforementioned pro-prosecution bias. Our alternative explanation is complementary because (as we noted earlier) a rational decision maker may not be consciously aware of all determinants of his or her choices. 5.5 Battlefield forensics and the war on terror The NAS report notes the role of forensic science in homeland security and the extensive forensicscience capabilities of the U.S. Department of Defense (pp to 11-6). As part of the ongoing war on terror, the U.S. military now employs a set of techniques referred to as battlefield forensics. The training program for battlefield forensics deals with the proper handling, collection, and processing of combat related forensics. This training provides an organic capability to provide competent evidentiary exploitation in a variety of situations (Dudkiewicz, 2006). The goal, as described by Anh Duong of the U.S. Navy, is to [r]apidly process battlefield evidence in-situ to support judicial, tactical & strategic operations (Duong, 2007, p. 13). The results of battlefield forensics can be used, among other things, to decide which persons to capture and detain as terrorists or enemy combatants.

16 84 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL Fear of terrorism should be expected to drive down the d/u ratio. This result is especially likely when an invading army deals with the local population, and even more so when suspected terrorists are mostly drawn from a different race or nationality, as is the case in Iraq, Afghanistan and other locations in the Middle East. Placing a very high value on American lives and low value on foreign lives (and liberties) naturally leads to a low d/u ratio. In addition, distrust of the local population also will tend to produce stronger initial priors about the likelihood of guilt, and trust in one s military compatriots will cause the updated prior to be yet larger (via the same mechanism described under pro-prosecution bias, above). As Koppl (2005, p. 262) argues, the institutional context can give rise to a kind of motivated reasoning among forensic workers. Lodge and Taber s (2000, p. 185) simple model of motivated political reasoning lists several factors that tend produce biased judgements, including (1) weak consequences from being wrong, (2) complex judgemental tasks, (3) ambiguous evidence and (4) time pressure. All four of these factors would naturally figure prominently in military forensics. No irrationality is necessary to explain such motivated reasoning in the case of battlefield forensics; instead, we need only look to the prior beliefs and utilities of the participants. The result is a very low evidentiary threshold for detaining suspected terrorists. 6. Potential remedies We suggest several possible changes in the way forensic science is performed. The primary goal was to make forensic testimony a better source of independent information to be considered by juries. Koppl (2005) reviews the literature and suggests a suite of reforms. Our discussion is restricted to reforms suggested by our model. 6.1 Independence Crime labs are typically organized under the police. As we have seen, this mode of organization tends to raise the lab s prior, δ, and reduce its d/u ratio, i.e. the relative cost of a type I error. Organizing crime labs under an independent body such as the court or the state or local health department would tend to combat these two effects. Giannelli (1997) argues for the creation of such independence by organizing crime labs under the office of the medical examiner. The NAS report endorses and recommends independence (p. 6-7). Problems of forensic medical examination in Mississippi (Balko, 2007a,b), however, suggest that such independence is no panacea. As Koppl notes, The Ryan Commission Report (State of Illinois, 2002) and Giannelli (1997) call for independence of forensic labs from law-enforcement agencies. The tendency of independence to reduce bias has been questioned in a minority opinion expressed in the Ryan report. The reality is that no matter how independent this separate state agency is, the bulk of its work will still be for police agencies and prosecutors (Illinois, p. 53). The value of independence depends on other simultaneous factors, such as how forensic labors are divided and whether labs are subject to competitive pressure. In the least favorable conditions, the minority opinion in the Ryan report is probably right. But in more favorable conditions, independence may reduce bias.

17 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE 85 Independence is not a stand-alone measure. When combined with other reforms, however, it should help to reduce inappropriate biases in crime labs. 6.2 Sequential unmasking and blind proficiency tests Under the current system, forensic examiners typically know that the authorities are more likely to submit a sample from someone who is guilty, i.e. a > b. The larger is the difference between a and b, the greater is the impact on the lab s prior and thus the greater is the chance of the lab announcing a match. Sequential unmasking and blind proficiency testing would reduce this form of bias. Sequential unmasking helps to reduce inappropriate biases by sequencing the laboratory workflow such that evidentiary samples are interpreted, and the interpretation is fully documented, before reference samples are compared (Krane et al., 2008, p. 1006). The reference sample is not, somehow, a secret to be hidden from the examiner. Rather, in the case of DNA typing, procedure should require the examiner to characterize the evidentiary sample ( call the alleles ) before viewing the reference sample. Information unmasked in a sequential manner is not permanently masked or hidden from the examiner. For this reason, Krane et al. (2008) coined the term sequential unmasking rather than using terms such as masking or blinding that inappropriately suggests that secrets must be kept from untrustworthy forensic scientists. Sequential unmasking would help in part by preventing the authorities from telling the forensic examiners their theory of the case. But this measure may not go far enough because the very act of submitting a sample conveys information. Blind proficiency tests would reduce the information content of such submissions. In a blind proficiency test, the lab taking the test thinks that it is (or at least may be) getting real crime scene evidence. In reality, however, the evidence has been constructed by the testing authority, which knows, therefore, what the right answer is. A blind proficiency test might be called a placebo sample. A medical placebo looks like medicine, but it contains no active ingredients. A placebo sample in forensic science looks like crime scene evidence, but there is no underlying crime because the placebo is constructed to test the lab s performance. Thus, we know the correct outcome for any forensic examination of a placebo sample. If the lab were periodically given blind proficiency tests, the value of b would rise relative to the value of a. Consequently, the submission of samples by the authorities would not increase the lab s prior by as much, and the match threshold would not be satisfied as easily. 6.3 Evidence line-ups Risinger et al. (2002) call for evidence line-ups to combat the almost built-in expectation that tested evidence will inculpate (p. 47). In terms of our model, they believe evidence line-ups would reduce the difference between a and b. An evidence line-up consists of one sample from the suspect plus several others. The lab would be kept ignorant about which sample came from the suspect. Instead of comparing the unknown hair found at the crime scene to the known hair from our suspect, e.g. the lab would compare the unknown hair to five known hairs, only one of which is from the suspect. As with blind proficiency tests (placebo samples), the predicted result is to reduce the impact of the authorities submission decision on the lab s prior by shrinking the difference between a and b. Forensic scientist Larry Miller (1987) conducted an experimental study of evidence line-ups for microscopic hair analysis. The results were dramatic: false positives fell from 30.4% under the

18 86 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL traditional approach to 3.8% using line-ups (Miller 1987, p. 160). This result is consistent with our model s predictions. It is important to properly design the implementation of this reform, as illustrated by the similar case of police line-ups for eyewitnesses (Schacter et al., 2007). A properly designed system would ensure, e.g. that samples not taken from the suspect are appropriately similar to the suspect s sample. If the suspect s sample is too dissimilar from the others, the forensic science may mistakenly declare a match to what is merely the most salient sample. 6.4 Separation of test and interpretation A great deal of subjective interpretation happens in the process of performing a test and drawing conclusions from it. As we have seen, the decisions of crime labs are affected by factors unrelated to the physical or chemical outcome of the test. We suggest that, at least with some types of forensic analysis, it would be possible to separate the test from its interpretation. A lab could perform DNA testing in a case, for instance, without interpreting the electropherogram. Any conclusions would be drawn by two forensic consultants. (Giannelli, 2004, reviews legal arguments supporting a defense right to forensic expertise.) One forensic consultant would work (and possibly testify) for the prosecution, the other for the defense. Each could, and probably would, be biased but this is a virtue as the adversarial process would force them to reveal the assumptions on which their conclusions are based. Froeb and Kobayashi (1996) argue that, in the context of civil trials, the adversarial process helps to eliminate juror biases and produce full information decisions, so long as both sides may present evidence (see also Milgrom and Roberts, 1986.) Koppl and Cowan (2010) argue that a similar result holds in the criminal context. But even if the forensic consultants competing biases would not promote true judgements by jurors, we should not fool ourselves into thinking that no bias affects the conclusions of forensic examiners in the status quo. In terms of this paper s model, we are essentially suggesting that forensic examiners should f n (r) f g (r) reveal the ratio,, to the jury, rather announcing match or no match. The adversarial process will help to expose ambiguities in the evidence and subject them to open dispute. This approach leaves jurors free to apply their own priors and conviction utilities rather than implicitly relying on those of the crime lab. In today s system, by contrast, priors, conviction utilities and test results are all packed into a single Delphic affirmation of match or no match by a forensic scientist. This practice strips the jurors of their role as triers of fact and hands that function over to the crime lab. We recognize, of course, that much forensic analysis is highly technical in nature, and juries may have difficulty in understanding the claims made. Jurors could be confused by presentations of, e.g. electropherograms and discussion of peak heights, blobs and so on. The current system, however, dodges this problem only by shrouding the evidence in the perceived (but illusory) objectivity of the forensic expert. Koppl and Cowan (2010), citing Froeb and Kobayashi (1996) and Milgrom and Roberts (1986), argue that the competition between strongly opposed forensic consultants would tend to induce the full information decision by the naïve decision makers of the jury Creating a defense right to forensic experts may be necessary to ensure the effectiveness of other reforms. The beneficial effects of any reform tend to fade as the affected parties learn compensating behaviors. But because a body of defense experts similar to and allied with public defenders would act to ensure their own continued existence, the good effects of this reform are less likely to fade. The existence at trial of strongly opposed forensic consultants would tend to apply upstream pressure that would help upstream reforms retain their salutary effects.

19 RATIONAL BIAS IN FORENSIC SCIENCE Conclusions Forensic science aspires to objectivity, but objectivity is a chimera. Subjective interpretation is unavoidable in the process of performing forensic work and reporting the results. The fact that forensic labs are generally part of the police organization and receive their samples almost exclusively from the authorities induces forensic examiners to alter their beliefs and thus behavior: they become more likely to announce incriminating matches than they would be otherwise. Consequently, juries as triers of fact are robbed of a potential source of independent information. This result occurs even if forensic examiners are perfectly rational Bayesians; the result could be yet worse if they are subject to cognitive errors and biases. We recommend reforms that reduce the influence of the conviction utilities and Bayesian priors of forensic scientists, thereby making them better sources of information for the criminal justice system. 8. Acknowledgements For helpful comments, the authors would like to thank David Croson, Pierre Garrouste, Keith Inman, Jay Koehler, Dan Krane, Michael Risinger, Mario Rizzo, Norah Rudin, Michael Saks, two anonymous referees, participants in the Colloquium on Market Institutions and Economic Processes of New York University, the 2007 meeting of the Southern Economic Association and the 2008 Forensic Bioinformatics Conference, The Science of DNA Profiling: A National Expert Forum. We thank William Thompson for providing Figs 1 and 2. We thank Dan Krane and Jason Gilder of Forensic Bioinformatics for special help in understanding DNA typing. REFERENCES AITKEN, C. G. G. (2000) Statistical Interpretation of Evidence/Bayesian Analysis, in Siegel, Jay, Geoffrey Knupfer, and Pekka Saukko editors, Encyclopedia of Forensic Sciences, New York: Academic Press, pp ARKES, H. R. & MELLERS, B. A. (2002) Do Juries Meet Our Expectation? 26(6) Law and Human Behavior BALKO, R. (2007a) Indeed, and Without a Doubt. Reason Online, 2 August Downloaded from BALKO, R. (2007b) CSI: Mississippi. Reason Magazine, November 2007, BANERJEE, A. V. (1992) A Simple Model of Herd Behavior. 107(3) Quarterly Journal of Economics BERNSTEIN, D. E. (2008) Expert Witnesses, Adversarial Bias, and the (Partial) Failure of the Daubert Revolution. 93(2) Iowa Law Review BERRY, D. A. (1991) Inferences Using DNA Profiling in Forensic Identification and Paternity Cases. 6(2) Statistical Science BIEDERMANN, A., BOZZA, S. & TARONI, F. (2008) Decision Theoretic Properties of Forensic Identification: Underlying Logic and Argumentative Principles. 177 Forensic Science International BIEDERMANN, A., BOZZA, S. & TARONI, F. (2009) Probabilistic Evidential Assessment of Gunshot Residue Particle Evidence (Part I): Likelihood Ratio Calculation and Case Pre-assessment Using Bayesian Networks. 191 Forensic Science International BIKHCHANDANI, S., HIRSHLEIFER, D. & WELCH, I. (1992) A Theory of Fads, Fashion, Custom, and Cultural Change as Informational Cascades. 100(5) Journal of Political Economy

20 88 G. WHITMAN AND R. KOPPL BLAKE, E. T., NEWALL, P., SENSABAUGH, G., SHALER, R., SINGER, R. L. & STOLOROW, M. D. (2004) Peer Review Report Texas v. George Rodriguez. Government Printing Office, Washington, DC. Available from the authors (Whitman & Koppl) on request. BOLCK, A., WEYERMANN, C., DUJOURDY, L., ESSEIVA, P. & VAN DEN BERG, J. (2009) Different Likelihood Ratio Approaches to Evaluate the Strength of Evidence of MDMA Tablet Comparisons. 191 Forensic Science International BUNZEL, H. & MARCOUL, P. (2008) Can Racially Unbiased Police Perpetuate Long-run Discrimination? 68 Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization CHAMPOD, C. (2000) Overview and Meaning of ID, in Siegel, Jay, Geoffrey Knupfer, and Pekka Saukko editors, Encyclopedia of Forensic Sciences, New York: Academic Press, pp COLE, S. (2003) Fingerprinting: The First Junk Science? 28(1) Oklahoma City University Law Review COLE, S. (2007) Where the Rubber Meets the Road: Thinking About Expert Evidence as Expert Testimony. 803 Villanova Law Review COSMIDES, L. & TOOBY, J. (1994) Better than Rational: Evolutionary Psychology and the Invisible Hand. 84(2) American Economic Review DROR, I. E. & CHARLTON, D. (2006) Why Experts Make Errors. 56(4) Journal of Forensic Identification DROR, I., CHARLTON, D. & PÉRON, A. E. (2006) Contextual Information Renders Experts Vulnerable to Making Erroneous Identifications. 156 Forensic Science International DROR, I. E., PÉRON, A. E., HIND, S.-L. & CHARLTON, D. (2005) When Emotions Get the Better of Us: The Effects of Contextual Top-down Processing on Matching Fingerprints. 19 Applied Cognitive Psychology DROR, I. E. & ROSENTHAL, R. (2008) Meta-analytically Quantifying the Reliability and Biasability of Forensic Experts. 53(4) Journal of Forensic Sciences DUDKIEWICZ, S. (2006) Battlefield Forensics Training. Army G2 Information Technology: Note to the Field, September Downloaded from articleid=540&issueid=40. DUONG, A. N. (2007) U.S. Navy Biometrics: An Overview. Downloaded 27 October 2007 from Thu Sep 13/Session I/13 Duong DOD.pdf. FRIEDMAN, R. (2003) Squeezing Daubert Out of the Picture. 33 Seton Hall Law Review FROEB, L. M. & KOBAYASHI, B. H. (1996) Naive, Biased, yet Bayesian: Can Juries Interpret Selectively Produced Evidence? 12 Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization GARRETT, B. L. & NEUFELD, P. J. (2009) Invalid Forensic Science Testimony and Wrongful Convictions. 95(1) Virginia Law Review GIANNELLI, P. C. (1997) The Abuse of Scientific Evidence in Criminal Cases: The Need for Independent Crime Laboratories. 4 Virginia Journal of Social Policy and the Law GIANNELLI, P. C. (2004) Ake v. Oklahoma: The Right to Expert Assistance in a Post-Daubert, Post-DNA World. 89(6) Cornell Law Review GOLD, M. (2008) Va. DNA Project Is In Uncharted Territory: Legal Experts Seek More Transparency. Washington Post, 17 August Downloaded 20 August 2008 from GRANN, D. (2009) Trial by Fire. New Yorker, 7 September Downloaded 7 September from fact grann?currentpage=all. GREENE, S. & MOFFIET, M. (2007) Bad Faith Difficult to Prove. The Denver Post, 22 July Downloaded 28 January 2009 from KELLY, J. F. & WEARNE, P. (1998) Tainting Evidence: Inside the Scandals at the FBI Crime Lab, New York: The Free Press.

Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward

Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward This PDF is available from The National Academies Press at Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward ISBN 978-0-309-13135-3 Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic

More information

STATEMENT OF PAUL C. GIANNELLI

STATEMENT OF PAUL C. GIANNELLI STATEMENT OF PAUL C. GIANNELLI WEATHERHEAD PROFESSOR OF LAW CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY BEFORE THE COMMITTEE ON THE JUDICIARY UNITED STATES SENATE HEARING ON THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES REPORT:

More information

NATIONAL COMMISSION ON FORENSIC SCIENCE. Ensuring That Forensic Analysis is Based Upon Task-Relevant Information

NATIONAL COMMISSION ON FORENSIC SCIENCE. Ensuring That Forensic Analysis is Based Upon Task-Relevant Information NATIONAL COMMISSION ON FORENSIC SCIENCE Ensuring That Forensic Analysis is Based Upon Task-Relevant Information Type of Work Product: Views Document Issued by Human Factors Subcommittee Statement of the

More information

The NAS Report and Foundations of Good Science

The NAS Report and Foundations of Good Science The NAS Report and Foundations of Good Science March 29, 2012 NACDL/Innocence Network Kansas City, MO Andrea Roth, UC Berkeley School of Law aroth@law.berkeley.edu Goals for the Next Hour Learn what the

More information

Legal view of digital evidence

Legal view of digital evidence Chapter 2 Legal view of digital evidence Before developing a model or a theory, it is important to understand the requirements of the domain in which the model or the theory is going to be used. The ultimate

More information

Decades of Successful Sex Crimes Defense Contact the Innocence Legal Team Now

Decades of Successful Sex Crimes Defense Contact the Innocence Legal Team Now Criminal Court Felonies The U.S. has the highest rate of felony conviction and imprisonment of any industrialized nation. A felony crime is more serious than a misdemeanor, but the same offense can be

More information

How To Decide A Case In The Uk

How To Decide A Case In The Uk 1 THE COURT: You have been selected and sworn to determine the facts and render a verdict in the case of the Commonwealth / 1 of Pennsylvania versus Robert Greene, who is charged with one count of robbery,

More information

Overview of the NAS Report on Forensic Science in the U.S.

Overview of the NAS Report on Forensic Science in the U.S. Overview of the NAS Report on Forensic Science in the U.S. Forensic Science in Iowa: Impact of the National Academy of Sciences Report October 29, 2010 Brian Farrell Overview Issues with forensic science

More information

DNA for Defense Attorneys. Chapter 6

DNA for Defense Attorneys. Chapter 6 DNA for Defense Attorneys Chapter 6 Section 1: With Your Expert s Guidance, Interview the Lab Analyst Case File Curriculum Vitae Laboratory Protocols Understanding the information provided Section 2: Interpretation

More information

Forensic Science Testing: The Forensic Filler-Control Method for Controlling Contextual Bias, Estimating Error Rates, and Calibrating Analysts Reports

Forensic Science Testing: The Forensic Filler-Control Method for Controlling Contextual Bias, Estimating Error Rates, and Calibrating Analysts Reports Forensic Science Testing: The Forensic Filler-Control Method for Controlling Contextual Bias, Estimating Error Rates, and Calibrating Analysts Reports Gary L. Wells 1 Miko M. Wilford 1 Laura Smalarz 1

More information

RECOMMENDATION. Issues Covered During Hearings and Deliberations

RECOMMENDATION. Issues Covered During Hearings and Deliberations NAS REPORT Strengthening Forensic Science in the US: A Path Forward Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic Science Community Committee on Science, Technology, and Law Policy and Global Affairs

More information

Introduction to Forensic Science and the Law. FBI Building Washington, DC

Introduction to Forensic Science and the Law. FBI Building Washington, DC FBI Building Washington, DC 1 Objectives You will understand: How crime labs in the United States are organized and what services they provide. The growth and development of forensic science through history.

More information

Genetic Witness: Science, Law, and Controversy in the Making of DNA Profiling

Genetic Witness: Science, Law, and Controversy in the Making of DNA Profiling Genetic Witness: Science, Law, and Controversy in the Making of DNA Profiling By Jay D. Aronson Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Press, 2007, ISBN 978-0-8135-4187-7 (Price $23.95), pp. 211. Reviewed

More information

ASA Board Policy Statement on Forensic Science Reform

ASA Board Policy Statement on Forensic Science Reform ASA Board Policy Statement on Forensic Science Reform April 17, 2010 The 2009 National Academies report, Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward, 1 identified many serious deficiencies

More information

Contextual information renders experts vulnerable to making erroneous identifications

Contextual information renders experts vulnerable to making erroneous identifications Forensic Science International 156 (2006) 74 78 Preliminary communication Contextual information renders experts vulnerable to making erroneous identifications Itiel E. Dror *, David Charlton, Ailsa E.

More information

Opening Statement Prof. Constantine Gatsonis Co-Chair, Forensic Science Committee

Opening Statement Prof. Constantine Gatsonis Co-Chair, Forensic Science Committee Opening Statement Prof. Constantine Gatsonis Co-Chair, Forensic Science Committee Good Afternoon Ladies and Gentlemen. I will begin with a heartfelt note of thanks to the staff of this committee of the

More information

Confirmation bias in biometric and forensic identification. Tim Valentine

Confirmation bias in biometric and forensic identification. Tim Valentine Confirmation bias in biometric and forensic identification Tim Valentine Face Comparison tasks Biometric Review of candidate faces from an automatic facial recognition system (e.g biometric security system

More information

Glossary. To seize a person under authority of the law. Police officers can make arrests

Glossary. To seize a person under authority of the law. Police officers can make arrests Criminal Law Glossary Arrest Charge Convicted Court Crime/Offence Crown Attorney or Prosecutor Criminal Custody Guilty Illegal Innocent Lawyer To seize a person under authority of the law. Police officers

More information

Managing Contextual Bias in Forensic Science

Managing Contextual Bias in Forensic Science Managing Contextual Bias in Forensic Science William C. Thompson University of California, Irvine SAMSI Forensics Workshop, September 1, 2015 NAS Report on Forensic Science (2009) Forensic science experts

More information

The Legal System in the United States

The Legal System in the United States The Legal System in the United States At the conclusion of this chapter, students will be able to: 1. Understand how the legal system works; 2. Explain why laws are necessary; 3. Discuss how cases proceed

More information

How to Use the California Identity Theft Registry

How to Use the California Identity Theft Registry How to Use the California Identity Theft Registry A Guide for Victims of Criminal Identity Theft Tips for Consumers Consumer Information Sheet 8 June 2014 What Is Criminal Identity Theft?...1 How You May

More information

CHAPTER 5 FORENSIC SCIENCE

CHAPTER 5 FORENSIC SCIENCE 48 CHAPTER 5 FORENSIC SCIENCE CHAPTER 5 FORENSIC SCIENCE 49 THE ISSUE Unvalidated or improperly applied forensic science contributed to approximately half of the 265 wrongful convictions overturned by

More information

Massachusetts Major City Chiefs. Best Practices in Eyewitness Identification and the Recording of Suspect Interviews

Massachusetts Major City Chiefs. Best Practices in Eyewitness Identification and the Recording of Suspect Interviews Massachusetts Major City Chiefs Best Practices in Eyewitness Identification and the Recording of Suspect Interviews September 2010 Introduction In December 2009 the Boston Bar Association published Getting

More information

TOP TEN TIPS FOR WINNING YOUR CASE IN JURY SELECTION

TOP TEN TIPS FOR WINNING YOUR CASE IN JURY SELECTION TOP TEN TIPS FOR WINNING YOUR CASE IN JURY SELECTION PRESENTED BY JEFF KEARNEY KEARNEY & WESTFALL 2501 PARKVIEW STREET, SUITE 300 FORT WORTH, TEXAS 76102 (817) 336-5600 LUBBOCK CRIMINAL DEFENSE LAWYERS

More information

INTRODUCTION FORENSIC SCIENCE AND THE LAW CHAPTER 1 OBJECTIVES

INTRODUCTION FORENSIC SCIENCE AND THE LAW CHAPTER 1 OBJECTIVES INTRODUCTION TO FORENSIC SCIENCE AND THE LAW CHAPTER 1 OBJECTIVES Describe the organization of crime labs in the US and the services they provide Describe the Federal rules of evidence Frye standard and

More information

In Your Blood Forensic DNA Databases

In Your Blood Forensic DNA Databases Irish Council for Bioethics In Your Blood Forensic DNA Databases DAVID NICHOLLS / SCIENCE PHOTO LIBRARY Q1 What is DNA? Q4 What is a DNA database? DNA stands for deoxyribonucleic acid. It is a chemical

More information

10 Victims and the law 57

10 Victims and the law 57 10 Victims and the law 57 10: Victims and the law This section gives a summary of the law in relation to victims of crime. Introduction The court may call a victim as a witness in a criminal case. However,

More information

THE SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY SPEAKS: The NAS Report: A Year Later. Robert Epstein Assistant Federal Defender Defender Association of Philadelphia

THE SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY SPEAKS: The NAS Report: A Year Later. Robert Epstein Assistant Federal Defender Defender Association of Philadelphia THE SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY SPEAKS: The NAS Report: A Year Later Robert Epstein Assistant Federal Defender Defender Association of Philadelphia THE BAD: UNITED STATES V. ROSE, 672 F.Supp.2d 723 (D. Md.

More information

Computer Forensics: an approach to evidence in cyberspace

Computer Forensics: an approach to evidence in cyberspace Computer Forensics: an approach to evidence in cyberspace Abstract This paper defines the term computer forensics, discusses how digital media relates to the legal requirements for admissibility of paper-based

More information

STATE OF ARIZONA, Appellee, ROY MATTHEW SOVINE, Appellant. No. 1 CA-CR 14-0094

STATE OF ARIZONA, Appellee, ROY MATTHEW SOVINE, Appellant. No. 1 CA-CR 14-0094 NOTICE: NOT FOR PUBLICATION. UNDER ARIZONA RULE OF THE SUPREME COURT 111(c), THIS DECISION DOES NOT CREATE LEGAL PRECEDENT AND MAY NOT BE CITED EXCEPT AS AUTHORIZED. IN THE ARIZONA COURT OF APPEALS DIVISION

More information

Lesson 4. Preventing and Policing White-Collar Crime

Lesson 4. Preventing and Policing White-Collar Crime Preventing and Policing ASSIGNMENT 11 Read this introduction and then read pages 260 294 in White- Collar Crime: The Essentials. White-collar crime is clearly complex and multifaceted. No single theory

More information

A Victim s Guide to Understanding the Criminal Justice System

A Victim s Guide to Understanding the Criminal Justice System A Victim s Guide to Understanding the Criminal Justice System The Bartholomew County Prosecutor s Office Victim Assistance Program Prosecutor: William Nash 234 Washington Street Columbus, IN 47201 Telephone:

More information

Stages in a Capital Case from http://deathpenaltyinfo.msu.edu/

Stages in a Capital Case from http://deathpenaltyinfo.msu.edu/ Stages in a Capital Case from http://deathpenaltyinfo.msu.edu/ Note that not every case goes through all of the steps outlined here. Some states have different procedures. I. Pre-Trial Crimes that would

More information

Conviction Integrity Unit Best Practices October 15, 2015

Conviction Integrity Unit Best Practices October 15, 2015 Conviction Integrity Unit Best Practices October 15, 2015 District Attorney s offices are increasingly creating Conviction Integrity Units (CIUs) to re examine questionable convictions and guard against

More information

DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIMS NEED TO KNOW

DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIMS NEED TO KNOW DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIMS NEED TO KNOW DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: What Victims Need to Know The increasing use of DNA evidence in criminal cases gives victims of crime new hope that offenders will be

More information

How To Find Out If Watching Ctv Makes A Person Guilty Of A Crime Without Scientific Evidence

How To Find Out If Watching Ctv Makes A Person Guilty Of A Crime Without Scientific Evidence www.all-about-forensic-science.com Presents The CSI Effect: Does It Really Exist? By Donald E. Shelton 2008 (Originally Published by The National Institute of Justice) Crime and courtroom proceedings have

More information

Executive Summary. Summary - 1

Executive Summary. Summary - 1 Executive Summary For as long as human beings have deceived one another, people have tried to develop techniques for detecting deception and finding truth. Lie detection took on aspects of modern science

More information

Introduction to. Hypothesis Testing CHAPTER LEARNING OBJECTIVES. 1 Identify the four steps of hypothesis testing.

Introduction to. Hypothesis Testing CHAPTER LEARNING OBJECTIVES. 1 Identify the four steps of hypothesis testing. Introduction to Hypothesis Testing CHAPTER 8 LEARNING OBJECTIVES After reading this chapter, you should be able to: 1 Identify the four steps of hypothesis testing. 2 Define null hypothesis, alternative

More information

VOIR DIRE 2/11/2015 STATE OF TEXAS VS JANE DOE 1. CONVERSATION - ONLY TIME YOU CAN ASK THE LAWYERS QUESTIONS 2. NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWER

VOIR DIRE 2/11/2015 STATE OF TEXAS VS JANE DOE 1. CONVERSATION - ONLY TIME YOU CAN ASK THE LAWYERS QUESTIONS 2. NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWER STATE OF TEXAS VS JANE DOE VOIR DIRE 1. CONVERSATION - ONLY TIME YOU CAN ASK THE LAWYERS QUESTIONS 2. NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWER 3. DESELECTION (TO MAKE THE JURY = SIT THERE & BE QUIET) 4. SOME QUESTIONS

More information

Forensic Science - A Path Forward Committee on Finding the Perfect Career

Forensic Science - A Path Forward Committee on Finding the Perfect Career Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward Committee on Identifying the Needs of the Forensic Sciences Community, National Academy of Sciences, 2009 Position Summary The Scientific

More information

In 2009, the nation s 411 publicly funded crime

In 2009, the nation s 411 publicly funded crime U.S. Department of Justice Office of Justice Programs Bureau of Justice Statistics Census of Publicly Funded Forensic Crime Laboratories, Matthew R. Durose, BJS Statistician Kelly A. Walsh, Research Associate,

More information

ARREST! What Happens Now?

ARREST! What Happens Now? Personal Injury Wrongful Death Slip & Fall Automobile Accidents Trucking Accidents Motorcycle Accidents Medical Malpractice Criminal Defense You re Under ARREST! What Happens Now? Do NOT Speak to Police

More information

MINNESOTA JUDICIAL TRAINING UPDATE

MINNESOTA JUDICIAL TRAINING UPDATE MINNESOTA JUDICIAL TRAINING UPDATE CRIMINAL VOIR DIRE QUESTIONS ASKED BY THE COURT THE MN SUPREME COURT TASK FORCE ON JURY SELECTION HAS RECOMMENDED THAT JUDGES BE MORE PROACTIVE IN ASKING INITIAL QUESTIONS

More information

CRIMINAL LAW & YOUR RIGHTS MARCH 2008

CRIMINAL LAW & YOUR RIGHTS MARCH 2008 CRIMINAL LAW & YOUR RIGHTS MARCH 2008 1 What are your rights? As a human being and as a citizen you automatically have certain rights. These rights are not a gift from anyone, including the state. In fact,

More information

Being a witness in a criminal trial

Being a witness in a criminal trial Being a witness in a criminal trial If you have been the victim of an offence, or a witness to that offence, you may be asked to make a formal statement. The judge who hears the case can use your statement

More information

THE SUPREME COURT OF THE REPUBLIC OF PALAU HANDBOOK FOR TRIAL JURORS

THE SUPREME COURT OF THE REPUBLIC OF PALAU HANDBOOK FOR TRIAL JURORS THE SUPREME COURT OF THE REPUBLIC OF PALAU HANDBOOK FOR TRIAL JURORS I. Purpose of This Handbook The purpose of this handbook is to acquaint trial jurors with the general nature and importance of their

More information

The Police Have Left Word That They Want to Speak With You

The Police Have Left Word That They Want to Speak With You The Police Have Left Word That They Want to Speak With You What Does it Mean and What Should You Do? Don A. Murray, Esq. Shalley & Murray 125-10 Queens Blvd., Suite 10 Kew Gardens, NY 11415 273 Sea Cliff

More information

U.S. MILITARY INVESTIGATIONS: TYPES & PROCEDURES

U.S. MILITARY INVESTIGATIONS: TYPES & PROCEDURES U.S. Military Investigations: Types & Procedures Background Materials U.S. MILITARY INVESTIGATIONS: TYPES & PROCEDURES [ NOTE TO INSTRUCTOR: The words below that appear in plain text also appear in the

More information

How To Get Your Criminal History From The Justice Department

How To Get Your Criminal History From The Justice Department Criminal Records & Employment 1. What exactly is a criminal record? YOUR LEGAL RIGHTS A criminal record, formally known as a summary criminal history, or more commonly known as a rap sheet, is a list of

More information

The Facts About Forensic DNA Analysis and DNA Databases. dnasaves.org

The Facts About Forensic DNA Analysis and DNA Databases. dnasaves.org dnasaves.org The Facts About Forensic DNA Analysis and DNA Databases A guide to scientific methodologies, laws and regulations for forensic DNA sample analysis. The shared local, state and national forensic

More information

When you open the newspaper, what types of stories are you most interested in reading? If you answered crime stories, you are not alone.

When you open the newspaper, what types of stories are you most interested in reading? If you answered crime stories, you are not alone. Students will understand the 5 W s of the criminal justice system, identify ways that the media can shape its audience s perception of what is true and apply their understanding of media bias to their

More information

Glossary of Terms Acquittal Affidavit Allegation Appeal Arraignment Arrest Warrant Assistant District Attorney General Attachment Bail Bailiff Bench

Glossary of Terms Acquittal Affidavit Allegation Appeal Arraignment Arrest Warrant Assistant District Attorney General Attachment Bail Bailiff Bench Glossary of Terms The Glossary of Terms defines some of the most common legal terms in easy-tounderstand language. Terms are listed in alphabetical order. A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W

More information

Fifty years of media and forensic science. Prof Niamh Nic Daeid Professor of Forensic Science Centre for Forensic Science University of Strathclyde

Fifty years of media and forensic science. Prof Niamh Nic Daeid Professor of Forensic Science Centre for Forensic Science University of Strathclyde Fifty years of media and forensic science Prof Niamh Nic Daeid Professor of Forensic Science Centre for Forensic Science University of Strathclyde Fifty years of media and forensic science is there really

More information

Introduction to Forensic Science. So what is Forensic Science? Major Contributors. Chapter 1: Intro to FS

Introduction to Forensic Science. So what is Forensic Science? Major Contributors. Chapter 1: Intro to FS Introduction to Forensic Science Chapter 1: Intro to FS So what is? In its broadest definition, forensic science is the application of science to criminal and civil laws. We will put emphasize on the applications

More information

Forensic Science: Crime Scene Basics. T. Trimpe 2006 http://sciencespot.net

Forensic Science: Crime Scene Basics. T. Trimpe 2006 http://sciencespot.net Forensic Science: Crime Scene Basics T. Trimpe 2006 http://sciencespot.net Crime Scene Vocabulary CRIME SCENE: Any physical location in which a crime has occurred or is suspected of having occurred. PRIMARY

More information

Are DNA tests infallible?

Are DNA tests infallible? International Congress Series 1239 (2003) 873 877 Are DNA tests infallible? G. Penacino *, A. Sala, D. Corach Servicio de Huellas Digitales Genéticas and Cátedra de Genética y Biología Molecular, Fac.

More information

Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward

Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward American Society of Crime Laboratory Directors (ASCLD): Recognizes that the committee s recommendations pave the way for a significant

More information

Sexual Assault of a Child VOIR DIRE QUESTIONS

Sexual Assault of a Child VOIR DIRE QUESTIONS ATTORNEYS Sexual Assault of a Child VOIR DIRE QUESTIONS 1. What are your feelings or opinions about criminal defense attorneys? 2. Have you ever had a bad experience with a criminal defense attorney? If

More information

COMMUNITY UNIT SCHOOL DISTRICT 200. Course Description

COMMUNITY UNIT SCHOOL DISTRICT 200. Course Description Forensic Science High School Elective Course Description Forensic Science is a one semester high school level course that satisfies a CUSD200 graduation requirement in the area of science. Successful completion

More information

YOUR CONTACT DETAILS (ADDRESS, PHONE, EMAIL etc.):

YOUR CONTACT DETAILS (ADDRESS, PHONE, EMAIL etc.): Justice WA YOUR NAME: YOUR CONTACT DETAILS (ADDRESS, PHONE, EMAIL etc.): TODAY S DATE: INDICATE WHO IS COMPLETING THIS FORM: Defendant Family Member of Defendant (Relationship : ) Friend of Defendant Other:

More information

GENERAL ASSEMBLY OF NORTH CAROLINA SESSION 2011 SESSION LAW 2011-19 HOUSE BILL 27

GENERAL ASSEMBLY OF NORTH CAROLINA SESSION 2011 SESSION LAW 2011-19 HOUSE BILL 27 GENERAL ASSEMBLY OF NORTH CAROLINA SESSION 2011 SESSION LAW 2011-19 HOUSE BILL 27 AN ACT TO (1) CREATE THE NORTH CAROLINA FORENSIC SCIENCE ADVISORY BOARD, (2) ENCOURAGE EFFORTS TO ELIMINATE SOURCES OF

More information

DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIM ASSISTANCE PROFESSIONALS NEED TO KNOW

DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIM ASSISTANCE PROFESSIONALS NEED TO KNOW DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: WHAT VICTIM ASSISTANCE PROFESSIONALS NEED TO KNOW What Victim Assistance Professionals Need to Know 1 DNA & CRIME VICTIMS: What Victim Assistance Professionals Need to Know As the

More information

NOT ACTUAL PROTECTION: ACTUAL INNOCENCE STANDARD FOR CRIMINAL DEFENSE ATTORNEYS IN CALIFORNIA DOES NOT ELIMINATE ACTUAL LAWSUITS AND ACTUAL PAYMENTS

NOT ACTUAL PROTECTION: ACTUAL INNOCENCE STANDARD FOR CRIMINAL DEFENSE ATTORNEYS IN CALIFORNIA DOES NOT ELIMINATE ACTUAL LAWSUITS AND ACTUAL PAYMENTS NOT ACTUAL PROTECTION: ACTUAL INNOCENCE STANDARD FOR CRIMINAL DEFENSE ATTORNEYS IN CALIFORNIA DOES NOT ELIMINATE ACTUAL LAWSUITS AND ACTUAL PAYMENTS By Celeste King, JD and Barrett Breitung, JD* In 1998

More information

Rules of Professional Conduct For Attorneys Wisconsin Supreme Court Rule 20

Rules of Professional Conduct For Attorneys Wisconsin Supreme Court Rule 20 Rules of Professional Conduct For Attorneys Wisconsin Supreme Court Rule 20 Read It, Know It, Live It Presented by Mark Gumz with the assistance of Hannah Schieber SCR 20: More than just 40+ pages of fine

More information

http://www.bls.gov/oco/ocos053.htm

http://www.bls.gov/oco/ocos053.htm Lawyers The legal system affects nearly every aspect of our society, from buying a home to crossing the street. Lawyers form the backbone of this system, linking it to society in numerous ways. They hold

More information

A Victim s Guide to the Capital Case Process

A Victim s Guide to the Capital Case Process A Victim s Guide to the Capital Case Process Office of Victims Services California Attorney General s Office A Victim s Guide to the Capital Case Process Office of Victims Services California Attorney

More information

Criminal Law Review Conference - 3 December 2015. Lord Justice Treacy. Keynote address

Criminal Law Review Conference - 3 December 2015. Lord Justice Treacy. Keynote address Criminal Law Review Conference - 3 December 2015 Lord Justice Treacy Keynote address I am pleased to be here today as I think this a good opportunity for me as Chairman, to outline four broad themes which

More information

CRIMINAL COURT IN MINNESOTA: Understanding the Process so You can Sleep at Night

CRIMINAL COURT IN MINNESOTA: Understanding the Process so You can Sleep at Night RYAN PACYGA CRIMINAL DEFENSE 333 South 7 th Street, Suite 2850 Minneapolis, MN 55402 612-339-5844 www.arrestedmn.com More information on the YouTube channel Ryan Pacyga CRIMINAL COURT IN MINNESOTA: Understanding

More information

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF MISSOURI WESTERN DIVISION

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF MISSOURI WESTERN DIVISION IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF MISSOURI WESTERN DIVISION UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, ) ) Plaintiff, ) ) vs. ) Case No. 09-00296-03-CR-W-FJG ) ROBERT E. STEWART, ) ) Defendant.

More information

Information for Crime Victims and Witnesses

Information for Crime Victims and Witnesses Office of the Attorney General Information for Crime Victims and Witnesses MARCH 2009 LAWRENCE WASDEN Attorney General Criminal Law Division Special Prosecutions Unit Telephone: (208) 332-3096 Fax: (208)

More information

Scaled Questions During Jury Selection

Scaled Questions During Jury Selection Scaled Questions During Jury Selection By: Ben Rubinowitz and Evan Torgan One of the most crucial tasks a trial attorney must undertake is selecting a pool of jurors that will view her client's case in

More information

Introduction to Crime Scene Dynamics

Introduction to Crime Scene Dynamics Chapter 1 Introduction to Crime Scene Dynamics OVERVIEW The development of the field of forensics has been a process steeped in science and research. On the other hand, the practice of crime scene investigation

More information

If you are in doubt, or think you may not be qualified to serve on a jury for one of the above or any other reasons, please notify the judge.

If you are in doubt, or think you may not be qualified to serve on a jury for one of the above or any other reasons, please notify the judge. Jurors are randomly selected by the county computer system from a source which combines current Medina County voter registration list and residents of the county that hold a valid Texas drivers license

More information

DOLCEFINO CONSULTING

DOLCEFINO CONSULTING DOLCEFINO CONSULTING The Harris County District Attorney s Office is committed to provide or arrange for services to meet the material, emotional, and information needs experienced by victims and witnesses,

More information

COURT OF APPEALS EIGHTH DISTRICT OF TEXAS EL PASO, TEXAS

COURT OF APPEALS EIGHTH DISTRICT OF TEXAS EL PASO, TEXAS COURT OF APPEALS EIGHTH DISTRICT OF TEXAS EL PASO, TEXAS DAVID MORALES, Appellant, V. THE STATE OF TEXAS, Appellee. O P I N I O N No. 08-05-00201-CR Appeal from the 409th District Court of El Paso County,

More information

Maricopa County Attorney s Office Adult Criminal Case Process

Maricopa County Attorney s Office Adult Criminal Case Process The following is a brief description of the process to prosecute an adult accused of committing a felony offense. Most misdemeanor offenses are handled by municipal prosecutors; cases involving minors

More information

Franklin County State's Attorney Victim Services

Franklin County State's Attorney Victim Services Franklin County State's Attorney Victim Services FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS What type of services and information can I get through Victim Services Program? A Victim Advocate will be assigned to assist

More information

Morgan County Prosecuting Attorney Debra MH McLaughlin

Morgan County Prosecuting Attorney Debra MH McLaughlin Morgan County Prosecuting Attorney Debra MH McLaughlin Directions: From Fairfax Street Entrance, Enter Main Door, turn Right through door, up the narrow staircase. Office is at top of steps. (Old Circuit

More information

INFORMATION / FACT SHEET CRIME TO TRIAL PROCESS CRIMINAL COURT HEARINGS EXPLAINED

INFORMATION / FACT SHEET CRIME TO TRIAL PROCESS CRIMINAL COURT HEARINGS EXPLAINED INFORMATION / FACT SHEET CRIME TO TRIAL PROCESS CRIMINAL COURT HEARINGS EXPLAINED *(Please be advised that this is a general guide only and is by no means an exhaustive summary of all criminal court hearings.

More information

Summary of Qualifications: Education: Professional Experience: Kenneth R. Moses

Summary of Qualifications: Education: Professional Experience: Kenneth R. Moses Summary of Qualifications: Kenneth Moses has over forty years of experience in forensic evidence. He established the Crime Scene Investigations Unit of the San Francisco Crime Laboratory in 1983 and was

More information

CAREER: FORENSIC SCIENCE TECHNICIAN 1

CAREER: FORENSIC SCIENCE TECHNICIAN 1 CAREER: FORENSIC SCIENCE TECHNICIAN 1 Career: Forensic Science Technician Mary C. Cartwright Middlesex Community College CRJ 111-52 Heloisa DaCunha November 29, 2012 CAREER: FORENSIC SCIENCE TECHNICIAN

More information

Courts & Our Legal System

Courts & Our Legal System Courts & Our Legal System 2012 (Version 1.0) This booklet has been prepared, published and distributed by the Public Legal Education Association of Saskatchewan (PLEA). The purpose of PLEA and this booklet

More information

What is the "Code Of Service Discipline"?

What is the Code Of Service Discipline? This booklet has been designed to provide general information on disciplinary proceedings under the Code of Service Discipline, focusing on the rights and entitlements of CF members under the Canadian

More information

William F. Leo Forensic Identification Consultant www.fingerprintidentification.net. Credentials

William F. Leo Forensic Identification Consultant www.fingerprintidentification.net. Credentials William F. Leo Forensic Identification Consultant www.fingerprintidentification.net Bill@FingerprintIdentification.net Credentials Master of Science Degree Criminology Indiana State University Emphasis

More information

CROSS EXAMINATION OF AN EXPERT WITNESS IN A CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE CASE. Mark Montgomery

CROSS EXAMINATION OF AN EXPERT WITNESS IN A CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE CASE. Mark Montgomery CROSS EXAMINATION OF AN EXPERT WITNESS IN A CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE CASE Mark Montgomery Post Office Box 161 Durham, NC 27702 (919) 680-6249 mark.montgomery@mindspring.com Opinion Testimony by a Pediatrician/Nurse/Counselor/Social

More information

TRAYVON S LAW BILL SUMMARY

TRAYVON S LAW BILL SUMMARY TRAYVON S LAW BILL SUMMARY In light of the tragic death of Trayvon Martin and the overwhelming national awareness around numerous issues surrounding this case, the NAACP advocates for creating a set of

More information

Collecting a Buccal Swab An Art or a Cinch? By Chantel Marie Giamanco, Forensic Scientist Human Identification Technologies, Inc.

Collecting a Buccal Swab An Art or a Cinch? By Chantel Marie Giamanco, Forensic Scientist Human Identification Technologies, Inc. Collecting a Buccal Swab An Art or a Cinch? By Chantel Marie Giamanco, Forensic Scientist Human Identification Technologies, Inc. An increasing number of cases tried in the courtroom involve DNA evidence.

More information

UNDERSTANDING THE CRIMINAL JUSTICE SYSTEM Anne Benson

UNDERSTANDING THE CRIMINAL JUSTICE SYSTEM Anne Benson UNDERSTANDING THE CRIMINAL JUSTICE SYSTEM Anne Benson What is the Criminal Justice System? The criminal justice system is the system we have in the United States for addressing situations where it is believed

More information

Decided: May 11, 2015. S15A0308. McLEAN v. THE STATE. Peter McLean was tried by a DeKalb County jury and convicted of the

Decided: May 11, 2015. S15A0308. McLEAN v. THE STATE. Peter McLean was tried by a DeKalb County jury and convicted of the In the Supreme Court of Georgia Decided: May 11, 2015 S15A0308. McLEAN v. THE STATE. BLACKWELL, Justice. Peter McLean was tried by a DeKalb County jury and convicted of the murder of LaTonya Jones, an

More information

There is an inherent flaw in the United States criminal justice system. The

There is an inherent flaw in the United States criminal justice system. The The Problem of Truth: Discussion of a Lawyer s Duty There is an inherent flaw in the United States criminal justice system. The adversarial nature of formal dispute resolution upon which our common law

More information

Sexual Assault and the Criminal Justice System

Sexual Assault and the Criminal Justice System New York County District Attorney s Office Sex Crimes Prosecution Unit One Hogan Place Room 666 New York, NY 10013 212-335-9373 Lisa Friel, Chief Northern Manhattan Office Located in the Harlem State Office

More information

SUPERIOR COURT OF ARIZONA MARICOPA COUNTY LC2014-000424-001 DT 01/22/2015 THE HON. CRANE MCCLENNEN HIGHER COURT RULING / REMAND

SUPERIOR COURT OF ARIZONA MARICOPA COUNTY LC2014-000424-001 DT 01/22/2015 THE HON. CRANE MCCLENNEN HIGHER COURT RULING / REMAND Michael K. Jeanes, Clerk of Court *** Filed *** 01/26/2015 8:00 AM THE HON. CRANE MCCLENNEN STATE OF ARIZONA CLERK OF THE COURT J. Eaton Deputy GARY L SHUPE v. MONICA RENEE JONES (001) JEAN JACQUES CABOU

More information

ILLINOIS DEPARTMENT OF CENTRAL MANAGEMENT SERVICES CLASS SPECIFICATION FORENSIC SCIENTIST SERIES

ILLINOIS DEPARTMENT OF CENTRAL MANAGEMENT SERVICES CLASS SPECIFICATION FORENSIC SCIENTIST SERIES ILLINOIS DEPARTMENT OF CENTRAL MANAGEMENT SERVICES CLASS SPECIFICATION FORENSIC SCIENTIST SERIES CLASS TITLE POSITION CODE EFFECTIVE FORENSIC SCIENTIST TRAINEE 15897 5/1/10 FORENSIC SCIENTIST I 15891 5/1/10

More information

NEW HAMPSHIRE E SCHOOL BOARDS ASSOCIATION Theodore E. Comstock, Executive Director Barrett M. Christina, Staff Attorney 603-228-2061 www.nhsba.

NEW HAMPSHIRE E SCHOOL BOARDS ASSOCIATION Theodore E. Comstock, Executive Director Barrett M. Christina, Staff Attorney 603-228-2061 www.nhsba. NEW HAMPSHIRE E SCHOOL BOARDS ASSOCIATION Theodore E. Comstock, Executive Director Barrett M. Christina, Staff Attorney 603-228-2061 www.nhsba.org Investigating Allegations of Employee Misconduct October

More information

MINNESOTA S EXPERIENCE IN REVISING ITS JUVENILE CODE AND PROSECUTOR INPUT IN THE PROCESS September 1997

MINNESOTA S EXPERIENCE IN REVISING ITS JUVENILE CODE AND PROSECUTOR INPUT IN THE PROCESS September 1997 MINNESOTA S EXPERIENCE IN REVISING ITS JUVENILE CODE AND PROSECUTOR INPUT IN THE PROCESS September 1997 In 1991, Minnesota began a major effort to substantially revise the laws governing our juvenile justice

More information

GLOSSARY OF SELECTED LEGAL TERMS

GLOSSARY OF SELECTED LEGAL TERMS GLOSSARY OF SELECTED LEGAL TERMS Sources: US Courts : http://www.uscourts.gov/library/glossary.html New York State Unified Court System: http://www.nycourts.gov/lawlibraries/glossary.shtml Acquittal A

More information

How To Find Out If Watching Ctv Is A Reason To Convict Without Scientific Evidence

How To Find Out If Watching Ctv Is A Reason To Convict Without Scientific Evidence The CSI Effect : Does It Really Exist? by Honorable Donald E. Shelton C rime and courtroom proceedings have long been fodder for film and television scriptwriters. In recent years, however, the media s

More information

Court Findings of Ineffective Assistance of Counsel Claims in Post Conviction Appeals Among the First 255 DNA Exoneration Cases.

Court Findings of Ineffective Assistance of Counsel Claims in Post Conviction Appeals Among the First 255 DNA Exoneration Cases. Court Findings of Ineffective Assistance of Counsel Claims in Post Conviction Appeals Among the First 255 DNA Exoneration Cases Prepared by: Dr. Emily M. West Director of Research Innocence Project September

More information

TRAVELING FORENSIC EDUCATION PROGRAM 2012-2013

TRAVELING FORENSIC EDUCATION PROGRAM 2012-2013 TRAVELING FORENSIC EDUCATION PROGRAM 2012-2013 INDEX -Program Overview Page 3 -Forensics Lab Total Experience Page 4 -Crime to Courtroom Workshop Page 4 -Impressions Workshop Page 5 -Blood & DNA Workshop

More information

Response to Critiques of Mortgage Discrimination and FHA Loan Performance

Response to Critiques of Mortgage Discrimination and FHA Loan Performance A Response to Comments Response to Critiques of Mortgage Discrimination and FHA Loan Performance James A. Berkovec Glenn B. Canner Stuart A. Gabriel Timothy H. Hannan Abstract This response discusses the

More information