Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions: A multidimensional scaling analysis of the mismatch negativity

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions: A multidimensional scaling analysis of the mismatch negativity"

From this document you will learn the answers to the following questions:

  • What length is studied in Finnish?

  • What signal is the MMN sensitive to?

  • What can be used to determine the modulation of the MMN?

Transcription

1 Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 25 (2007) IOS Press Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions: A multidimensional scaling analysis of the mismatch negativity Bharath Chandrasekaran, Jackson T. Gandour and Ananthanarayan Krishnan Department of Speech Language & Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA Abstract. Purpose: An auditory electrophysiological study was conducted to explore the influence of language experience on the saliency of dimensions underlying cortical pitch processing. Methods: Mismatch negativity (MMN) responses to Mandarin tones were recorded in Chinese and English participants (n = 10 per group) using a passive oddball paradigm. Stimuli consisted of three tones (T1: high level; T2: high rising; T3: low falling-rising). There were three oddball conditions (standard/deviant): T1/T2, T1/T3, T2/T3. In the T1/T2 and T1/T3 conditions, each tonal pair represented a contrast between a level and a contour tone; the T2/T3 condition, a contrast between two contour tones. Twenty dissimilarity matrices were created using the MMN mean amplitude measured from the Fz location for each condition per participant, and analyzed by an individual differences multidimensional scaling model. Results: Two pitch dimensions were revealed, interpretively labeled as height and contour. The latter was found to be more important for Chinese than English subjects. Using individual weights on the contour dimension, a discriminant function showed that 17 out of 20 participants were correctly classified into their respective language groups. Conclusions: The MMN can serve as an index of pitch features that are differentially weighted depending on a listener s experience with lexical tones and their acoustic correlates within a particular tone space. Keywords: Mismatch negativity, pitch, multidimensional scaling, experience-dependent plasticity, lexical tone, Mandarin Chinese 1. Introduction Using the mismatch negativity (MMN) component of the auditory event-related potential (ERP), it is well established that language experience influences the automatic involuntary processing of speech sounds (N. Kraus & Cheour, 2000; Naatanen, 2001). Early work focused on segmental information, for example, the effects of the presence or absence of consonants or vowels in a phonemic inventory (Naatanen et al., 1997; Sharma & Dorman, 2000; Winkler et al., 1999) as well as other segmental aspects of phonology, including phono- Corresponding author: Jackson T. Gandour, PhD, Purdue University, Department of Speech Language and Hearing Sciences, 1353 Heavilon Hall, 500 Oval Drive, West Lafayette, IN , USA. Tel.: +1 (765) ; Fax: +1 (765) ; gandour@purdue.edu. tactics (Dehaene-Lambertz, Dupoux, & Gout, 2000) and phoneme boundaries (Sharma & Dorman, 1999; Ylinen, Shestakova, Huotilainen, Alku, & Naatanen, 2006). More recently, it has been demonstrated that the MMN response is also sensitive to suprasegmental information in the speech signal, including changes in loudness (Hungarian word stress, Honbolygo, Csepe, & Rago, 2004), duration (Finnish vowel length, Nenonen, Shestakova, Huotilainen, & Naatanen, 2003), and pitch (Mandarin lexical tones, Chandrasekaran, Krishnan, & Gandour, 2007). The MMN component of the ERP has also been used to study the representation of auditory attributes in sensory memory (Caclin et al., 2006, pp , review). A number of studies support the view that basic perceptual attributes (e.g., frequency, duration, intensity) are represented separately in auditory sensory memory. Of special interest are perceptual attributes that /07/$ IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

2 196 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions rely on several acoustic dimensions. A recent study of timbre, a multidimensional perceptual attribute, provides support for the notion that its dimensions are represented separately in auditory sensory memory (Caclin et al., 2006; Caclin, McAdams, Smith, & Winsberg, 2005). Pitch is a multidimensional perceptual attribute that similarly relies on several acoustic dimensions (e.g., height, slope, direction). Psychophysical evidence for the multidimensional nature of pitch perception in tone languages comes primarily from crosslanguage multidimensional scaling studies of dissimilarity ratings. Based on a sample of tone languages from the Far East (Thai, Cantonese, Mandarin, Taiwanese) and West Africa (Yoruba) and a non-tone language (English), it appears that three dimensions underlie a common perceptual space: average F 0 height, direction of F 0 movement, magnitude of F 0 slope or level vs. contour (Gandour, 1983; Gandour & Harshman, 1978). The relative importance of these dimensions, however, varies depending on a listeners familiarity with specific types of pitch patterns that occur in their native language. For example, the perceptual saliency of the contour dimension is greater for native speakers of tone languages than for speakers of English, while English listeners give greater weight to the height dimension than do tone language speakers. Such differences in perceptual saliency suggest that long-term experience enhances listeners attention to pitch dimensions that are phonetically relevant in a particular language. As a tone language, Mandarin Chinese has four contrastive lexical tones (ma 1 mother [T1], ma 2 hemp [T2], ma 3 horse [T3], ma 4 scold [T4]). Tones 1 to 4 can be described phonetically as high level, high rising, low falling rising, and high falling, respectively (Howie, 1976). In a recent crosslanguage study of the MMN elicited in response to Mandarin tones (Chandrasekaran et al., 2007), two oddball conditions were constructed with a common deviant, a low falling rising contour tone (T3). One condition consisted of two tones that are acoustically similar to one another (T2/T3: T2, high rising contour = standard). The other condition consisted of two tones that are acoustically dissimilar to one another (T1/T3: T1, high level = standard). The native group (Chinese) exhibited larger MMN responses to the high dissimilarity condition (T1/T3) than the non-native (English). In the case of the low dissimilarity condition (T2/T3), MMN responses were similar for both groups. Interestingly, MMN responses were larger in the T1/T3 condition than the T2/T3 condition for native Chinese listeners only. By virtue of these group differences, Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) inferred that early cortical processing of pitch may be shaped by the relative saliency of acoustic dimensions underlying the pitch patterns of a particular language. Yet they were unable to test this hypothesis due to limitations of the stimulus set. In order to directly assess the number and nature of pitch dimensions underlying these MMN responses, as well as their relative importance for individual subjects, we have chosen to apply INDSCAL (INDividual Differences SCALing) (Carroll & Chang, 1970; cf.harshman, 1970), a multidimensional scaling model that can be applied to two-mode three-way data, i.e., two or more square symmetric data matrices for pairs of stimuli from two or more subjects or other data sources. Instead of a single data matrix representing the average of a group of subjects, INDSCAL performs a metric multidimensional scaling analysis using a separate matrix for each individual in the group. The model assumes that the set of dimensions is common to all subjects. For example, when subjects or groups of subjects differ in their MMN responses, the model assumes that they do so either by assigning different relative importance to the dimensions or by using different subsets of the total number of dimensions. On the basis of subjects pairwise dissimilarities data, the output from INDSCAL consists of two matrices. The first matrix contains the coordinates of the stimulus objects on n dimensions in Euclidean space (group stimulus space). The distance between points in this space is represented by a weighted Euclidean function that reflects the relative importance or salience of different dimensions per subject. The dimensions can then be interpreted based on the distribution of the objects in Euclidian space. The second matrix contains each subject s weights on the different dimensions (subject space). An important property of the INDSCAL individually-weighted solution is that the axes of the group stimulus space are oriented in such a way that expansion or contraction results in a mathematically unique orientation of axes, i.e., an orientation that maximally accounts for individual differences. The axes cannot be rotated without worsening the overall fit of the model. The aim of the present study is to directly test the hypothesis of separate neural processing of pitch dimensions by applying INDSCAL analysis to the MMN responses. As such, it represents an extension of the observations reported in Chandrasekaran et al. (2007). The specific goals are (i) to determine the number and nature of pitch dimensions elicited by MMN responses to Mandarin tones and (ii) to assess to what extent sub-

3 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 197 jects differences in the relative importance attached to particular dimensions are influenced by their language experience. Since INDSCAL analysis requires square symmetric data matrices for stimulus pairs for all subjects, it was necessary to obtain an additional condition (T1/T2) from those who participated in the companion study (Chandrasekaran et al., 2007). By including T1/T2, which involves a comparison of a level tone (T1) with a high rising tone (T2), we satisfy INDSCAL s input requirements. All objects (T1, T2, T3) can now be compared pairwise to one another, resulting in three conditions (T1/T2, T1/T3, T2/T3) for each of the 20 participants (Chinese, English). Based on earlier crosslanguage multidimensional scaling studies of tone perception (Gandour, 1983; Gandour & Harshman, 1978), in addition to the findings of Chandrasekaran et al. (2007), we expect that two pitch dimensions ( height, contour ) will best account for pairwise dissimilarities in mean MMN amplitude between the three pairs of tones. By including two language groups, one native (Chinese), the other nonnative (English), we are able to determine the extent to which modulation of the MMN can be attributed to a listener s familiarity with the pitch dimensions underlying the Mandarin tone space. Since pitch contour is a critical phonetic feature of the Mandarin tone space, the contour dimension is expected to be more heavily weighted by Chinese subjects than by English. Discriminant analysis is employed to determine whether individual subjects can be classified into their respective language group on the basis of their dimension weights. To the extent that a discriminant function is highly successful in separating the two language groups, we can infer that experience-dependent plasticity, as reflected in the MMN, extends to the extraction of pitch dimensions. These predicted outcomes would be consistent with the view that there are separate representations of pitch dimensions in auditory sensory memory (cf. timbre dimensions (Caclin et al., 2006)). 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Participants Ten adult, native speakers of Mandarin Chinese (5 male; 5 female) and ten adult, native speakers of American English (5 male; 5 female) participated in the ERP experiment. Both groups were closely matched with respect to age (Chinese: M = 23.2; English: M = 25.2) and years of formal education (Chinese: M = 17.2; English: M = 18.2). They gave informed consent in compliance with a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of Purdue University. See Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) for further details about language background, musical experience, and audiometric profile of the participants Stimuli Stimuli consisted of a set of three Mandarin Chinese words that are distinguished minimally by tonal contour (pinyin Roman transliteration): yi 1 clothing [T1]; yi 2 aunt [T2]; yi 3 chair [T3]. Using a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer (Klatt, 1980; Klatt & Klatt, 1990), synthetic versions were created so that all four syllables were identical except for their tonal contours. Synthesis parameters for voice fundamental frequency (F 0 ) and duration were obtained from words produced in citation form by an adult male speaker (Y. Xu, 1997). F 0 contours of the stimuli are displayed in Fig. 1. Vowel formant frequencies were steady-state, and were held constant across the four syllables (in Hz): F1 = 300; F2 = 2500; and F3 = 3500 (Howie, 1976). Tonal durations were normalized to 250 ms. Voice amplitude was kept constant at 60 db. These three tones differed in F 0 height as measured by onset, offset, and average F 0, and in F 0 contour and/or direction as measured by slope from tonal onset to offset, from onset to turning point, and from turning point to offset (Table 1). The locations of the turning points (in ms) for T2 and T3 were 106 and 144, respectively. T1 may be described as a high level tone; T2 and T3 as high rising and low falling rising contour tones, respectively. With regard to F 0 contour and direction, T2 is more similar to T3 than to T1. In terms of F 0 height, T1 is more similar to T2 than to T3. See Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) for further information about the rationale underlying the selection of T1, T2, and T3. The experiment consisted of three oddball conditions (standard/deviant): T1/T2, T1/T3, T2/T3 (Fig. 2). In the T1/T2 and T1/T3 conditions, each tonal pair represented a dissimilar contrast between a level (T1) and a contour tone (T2, T3). In the T2/T3 condition, the tonal pair represented a similar contrast between two rising contour tones. By pairing T1 with T2, we were able to contrast a high level tone (T1) with a high rising contour tone (T2). By pairing T1 with T3, we were able to contrast a high level tone (T1) with a low rising contour tone (T3) and, by pairing T2 with T3, a high rising (T2) with a low rising contour tone (T3). These

4 198 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions Table 1 Acoustic characteristics of experimental stimuli F 0 parameters Tone Onset Offset Average Slope a Slope b Slope c TP F 0 Onset-Offset Onset-TP TP-Offset T T T Note. Onset, offset, and average F 0 values are expressed in Hertz (Hz). All slope values are expressed in Hz/ms. T1, T2, and T3 refer to the Mandarin high level, high rising, and low falling rising tones, respectively. TP, expressed in milliseconds, refers to turning point, i.e., time at which the contour changed direction. F 0 = voice fundamental frequency; F 0 = change in Hz from onset to turning point. a Overall slope, measured from pitch onset to offset. b Slope from the onset to TP. Since the level tone T1 has no clear turning point, slope was measured from onset to 125 ms (50% duration). Both T2 and T3 have negative slopes (i.e. falling F 0 contour). c Slope from the TP to offset. Since the level tone T1 has no clear turning point, slope was measured from 125 ms (50% duration) to offset. Both T2 and T3 have positive slopes (i.e. rising F 0 contour). Fig. 1. Average fundamental frequency contours of time-normalized Mandarin Chinese tonal stimuli (T1, T2, T3) adapted from Xu (1997). Superscript numbers (1 3) denote three Mandarin lexical tones: yi 1 clothing ; yi 2 aunt ; yi 3 chair. three conditions ensured that all three tones (T1, T2, T3) were compared to one another, a prerequisite for multidimensional scaling analysis Experimental protocol Subjects were seated in a recliner in an acoustically and electrically shielded booth facing a video monitor. They were instructed to ignore the auditory stimuli presented via earphones and to focus their attention exclusively on a self-selected, closed-caption silent movie. Five different stimulus sequences with an interstimulus onset-to-onset interval of 667 ms were presented in random order. Three were oddball conditions made up of a frequent stimulus or standard (p = 0.85) and an infrequent stimulus or deviant (p = 0.15). T3 was used as the deviant in two of the oddball conditions (T1/T3, T2/T3); T2 as the deviant in the third condition (T1/T2). Within the oddball sequences, the order of presentation of stimuli was pseudo-random, i.e.,

5 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 199 Fig. 2. Three oddball conditions used to elicit MMN responses: T3 presented in the context of T1 (T1/T3, left panel) and T2 (T2/T3, right panel); T2 presented in the context of T1 (T1/T2, middle panel). The frequent (p = 0.85) and rare (p = 0.15) stimuli are represented with dashed and solid lines, respectively. at least one standard stimulus preceded each deviant. The remaining two conditions were comprised of the deviants T3 and T2 presented alone (p = 1.00). See Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) for further information about data acquisition procedures Evoked potential recording For each subject, silver-chloride electrodes were mounted on frontal midline (Fz), left frontal (F3), and right frontal (F4) sites according to the location system. MMN responses are known to be robust at these frontal electrodes (Naatanen et al., 1997). In our previous study, Mandarin tones elicited consistent and robust responses at these three electrode sites (Chandrasekaran et al., 2007). In this experiment we did not seek to answer questions on source localization and/or lateralization of MMN responses. Therefore, a threeelectrode configuration (Fz, F3, F4) was sufficient. The Fz electrode was chosen for statistical analysis because the MMN response is known to be most robust and stable at this location. Also, Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) reported no effect of electrode location (Fz, F3, F4) on the mean amplitude and peak latency of the mismatch negativity responses. See Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) for further information about recording procedures Pre-processing and statistical analysis of MMN data The baseline for the grand averaged waveforms was defined as the average amplitude between 100 ms to 0 ms (onset of stimuli). To calculate the MMN, the deviant waveform obtained from the oddball paradigm was subtracted from the deviant presented in the 100% probability condition. This subtraction process, also called an identity MMN, effectively controls for any acoustical differences between stimuli (Kraus et al., 1995; Sharma & Dorman, 2000; Ylinen et al., 2006). MMN amplitude was measured as the mean voltage from a 100 ms window centered on the grand average MMN peak latency obtained from each difference waveform, within the predefined MMN window. See Chandrasekaran et al. (2007) for further details about measures of MMN amplitude and peak latency. MMN mean amplitudes and peak latencies were analyzed using a two-way mixed model ANOVA (subject as random effect) for the effects of language group (Chinese, English) and tonal condition (T1/T3, T1/T2, T2/T3). Partial and generalized eta-squared values were measured for all effects to determine effect size (Olejnik & Algina, 2003) INDSCAL analysis For the current application, the input consisted of 20 (subjects) 3 (stimulus tones) 3 (stimulus tones) symmetric data matrices. Each data matrix contained distance estimates, i.e., the normalized MMN mean amplitude, for each paired comparison of the three stimulus tones (T1 vs. T2, T1 vs. T3, T2 vs. T3). INDSCAL (Carroll & Chang, 1970) analyses of these 20 dissimilarity matrices were performed at n (where n = 1, 2) dimensionalities in order to determine the appropriate number of dimensions underlying the distances among the three tones or objects in an auditory electrophysiological space. The output consisted of two matrices, a 3 (stimulus tones) n dimensions matrix of coordinates of the three stimulus tones on n dimensions (represented visually in a group stimulus space ), and a 20 n

6 200 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions matrix of weights of each of the 20 individual subjects (represented visually in a subject space ). Several criteria were used to determine the number of dimensions underlying the auditory electrophysiological space. First, solutions obtained from the IND- SCAL iterative process were considered to have converged when they met the program s internal criterion for convergence (< 0.01). In this application, convergence implies that the n dimensions were common to the MMN responses of all subjects irrespective of language group. Only one- and two-dimensional solutions were evaluated because the maximum number of dimensions possible is one less than the number of objects (T1, T2, T3). Second, the variance accounted for (VAF) was used as an index of overall fit, i.e., a measure of the proportion of variance in the scaled data that is accounted for by the INDSCAL procedure, for both one-dimensional and two-dimensional solutions. Based on the VAF, either a one-dimensional or a twodimensional solution was chosen for further analysis. Third, the mean correlation (r) between the interstimulus distances from the two-dimensional solution and the original dissimilarity data obtained from the scaled MMN was used to determine how well the one or twodimensional solutions reflect the original MMN scaled mean amplitude data. Fourth, to assess the statistical stability of the INDSCAL output, crossvalidated analyses were performed for one- or two-dimensional spaces, each based on a random split-half of the total subject sample. If the INDSCAL solution for the total sample was truly unique, similar stimulus configurations should result from INDSCAL analyses of the random split-half samples. Further, to assess the stability of the INDSCAL solution, the group space of the IND- SCAL solution was rotated by 45 degrees. From this starting position, the program was allowed to converge. If the VAF is drastically reduced, relative to the original VAF, when the stimulus configuration is rotated, and converges back to the original configuration when iteration is allowed, the INDSCAL solution can be regarded as both unique and stable (J.D. Carroll, personal communication, October 18, 2006). Discriminant analysis was performed for the n- dimensional solution to determine whether individual subjects can be classified into their respective language group on the basis of a weighted linear combination of the n dimension weights. The discriminant function was crossvalidated using a k-fold crossvalidation, i.e., dimension weights from each subject were used as validation data for the function created from dimension weights of the remaining subjects. This was repeated until every one of the twenty subjects was used for validation. Finally, comparisons of the mean subject weights per language group on each of the n dimensions were performed by means of a one-way ANOVA. 3. Results 3.1. Mismatch negativity Chinese and English grand-average MMN standard and deviant waveforms for the three conditions (T1/T2, T1/T3, T2/T3) at the Fz electrode site are shown in Fig. 3. For all three conditions across groups, tones elicited robust MMN responses when presented in the low probability (p = 0.15) conditions relative to the 100% probability sequences. A two-way [group X condition] repeated measures ANOVA conducted on the mean MMN amplitude yielded a significant group (F 1,18 = 12.94, p<0.01, partial-η 2 = 0.42, generalized-η 2 = 0.26), condition (F 2,36 = 9.19, p< 0.01, partial-η 2 = 0.34, generalized-η 2 = 0.15), and interaction effect between group and condition (F 2,36 = 6.20, p<0.01, partial-η 2 = 0.26, generalized-η 2 = 0.12). Post hoc Tukey-adjusted multiple comparisons revealed that the MMN mean amplitude of the T1/T3 (C T1/T3 > E T1/T3, p<0.01) and T1/T2 (C T1/T2 > E T1/T2, p = 0.02) conditions were larger for the Chinese group relative to the English. The mean MMN amplitude of the T2/T3 condition, however, did not differ significantly between language groups (p = 1.00). Within the Chinese group, the mean MMN amplitude of the T2/T3 condition was significantly less than either the T1/T3 (C T2/T3 < C T1/T3, p<0.01) or T1/T2 (C T2/T3 < C T1/T2, p<0.01) condition. No significant difference was found in the mean MMN amplitude between the T1/T2 and T1/T3 conditions (p = 0.95). In contrast, within the English group, no significant differences in mean MMN amplitude were observed between any of the three tonal conditions. With respect to peak latency, a two-way [group X condition] repeated measures ANOVA yielded a significant condition effect (F 2,36 = 7.27, p < 0.01, partial-η 2 = 0.29, generalized-η 2 = 0.26). Neither group (F 1,18 = 0.42, p = 0.52, partial-η 2 = 0.02, generalized-η 2 = 0.01) nor interaction effects (F 2,36 = 0.76, p = 0.47, partial-η 2 = 0.04, generalized-η 2 = 0.02) was significant. Post-hoc Tukey-adjusted multiple comparisons (p = 0.01) revealed that, irrespective of group, the peak negativity for the T2/T3 condition occurred later than either the T1/T2 or T1/T3.

7 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 201 Fig. 3. Grand-averaged standard (high probability, p = 1.00) and deviant waveforms (low probability, p = 0.15) obtained from the Fz electrode location are displayed for the two language groups (Chinese, top panel; English, bottom panel) per experimental condition (T1/T2, left; T1/T3, middle; T2/T3, right). The standard ( identity ) waveforms were elicited by presenting T2 and T3 alone; the deviant waveforms were elicited by presenting T3 in the context of either T1 (T1/T3) or T2 (T2/T3), T2 in the context of T1 (T1/T2). Both groups show a larger MMN for the deviant relative to the standard, and a peak latency that occurs between ms post stimulus onset. The T1/T2 and the T1/T3 conditions, which involve a contrast between level and contour tones, elicit larger MMNs in the Chinese group relative to the English (cf. Table 3). However, no group differences are observed in the magnitude of the MMN for the T2/T3 condition, which contrasts two similarly rising contour tones Multidimensional scaling of the mismatch negativity Number of dimensions A comparison of the badness-of-fit of the INDSCAL solutions for one- and two-dimensional spaces indicated that two dimensions best describe the MMN data. The cumulative percentage of VAF by the twodimensional INDSCAL model was 74%, a 14% increase over the one-dimensional solution (60%). These results indicate that two dimensions are necessary to characterize the subjects MMN responses to T1, T2, and T3. The mean correlation between the interstimulus distances from the two-dimensional solution and the original dissimilarity data obtained from the scaled MMN mean amplitude measures, across all twenty subjects, was r = For the Chinese group, the mean correlation was r = 0.92; for the English, r = Thus, the INDSCAL model indicates that the data from both language groups can be described very well by this common set of two dimensions Interpretation of dimensions The group stimulus space of the two-dimensional INDSCAL solution for all 20 subjects dissimilarities data (left panel) and two cross-validated twodimensional solutions (right panel), each (a, b) based on a random half of the total subject sample, are shown in Fig. 4. For all three solutions, Dim 1 is interpretively labeled height, as measured by average F 0 and F 0 offset (Table 1) of each tone (T1, T2, T3). T3, the low falling-rising contour tone, is positioned toward one end of the axis, whereas T2, the high rising contour tone, and T1, the high level tone, are positioned toward the opposite end. For all three solutions, Dim 2 is inter-

8 202 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions Fig. 4. Dimensions 1 (height) and 2 (contour) of the two-dimensional INDSCAL stimulus space from the combined group of 20 Chinese and English participants (left panel), and from two (a, b) random split-halves (right panel). Each dimension is normalized so that the mean of the coordinate values equals zero and the sum of squared coordinates equals DIM = dimension. Similarity between the configurations of the crossvalidated and original INDSCAL solutions confirm the stability and validity of the group stimulus space. pretively labeled contour, as measured by changes in the magnitude of slope of each tone throughout its duration, especially from turning point to offset (Table 1). T1, the high level tone, is positioned toward one end of the axis, whereas the two rising contour tones (T2, T3), are positioned toward the opposite end. In this group stimulus space, it is further observed that the distance between T1 and T3 is much larger relative to that between T1 and T2, meaning that the mismatch negativity is especially sensitive to pitch height regardless of language group. Although Pearson product-moment correlations could not be computed due to the small number of objects (n = 3), the configuration of the group space from the two split-half solutions (Fig. 4, bottom panel) is similar to the solution obtained from all 20 subjects (Fig. 4, top panel). This result indicates that the orientation of the axes of the two-dimensional space is unique, and that the dimensions can be interpreted without rotation. As a further test of the stability of the solution, the axes of the group space was rotated by 45 degrees and the rotated stimulus configuration was used as the starting point for a new INDSCAL analysis with zero iterations. The VAF dropped to 0.07, indicating that the original INDSCAL group configuration was genuinely unique. Further, when the rotated solution was used as the starting configuration, and the program was allowed to iterate, the program converged on to the original solution, confirming the stability of the INDSCAL group configuration Dimension weights for subjects The subject space for the two-dimensional IND- SCAL solution for all 20 subjects is shown in Fig. 5. Subjects weights on the two dimensions are shown by the projection of symbols on the coordinate axes. If subjects assigned equal weights to the two dimensions, their symbols would be located at a 45 angle (equidistant) between the two axes of the subject space. Instead, subjects weights appear to project off the 45 angle, and moreover, are clustered according to language group. All but one of the Chinese subjects project higher on Dim 2 contour when compared to the majority of English subjects (9/10). On Dim 1 height, however, subjects projections do not appear to cluster by language group. Results of a one-way ANO- VA of the mean subject weights per language group confirmed that pitch contour had a greater influence on the mismatch negativity of the Chinese group relative to the English (F 1,18 = 20.81, p<0.01, η 2 = 0.54), whereas pitch height did not yield a language group effect (F 1,18 = 0.35, p = 0.55, η 2 = 0.02) Discriminant analysis of subject weights Discriminant functions were used to determine the extent to which individual subjects can be classified into their respective language groups based on Dim 1 weights, Dim 2 weights, or a weighted linear combination of the two dimension weights. The classification matrices from these three discriminant analyses show that Dim 2 contour yielded the highest correct

9 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 203 Fig. 5. Subject space of the two-dimensional INDSCAL configuration for the Chinese ( ) and English ( ) language groups. DIM = dimension. Only three (dotted-line rectangles) of 20 subjects are incorrectly classified with the other language group by means of discriminant analysis. The distribution of subjects reveals that MMN responses from the Chinese language group are more heavily weighted on the pitch contour dimension as compared to the English group. classification of subjects into their respective language groups (Table 2). Dim 2 was very effective in discriminating individuals by language group; 90% and 80% correct for Chinese and English subjects, respectively (canonical R 2 = 0.73). Because we expect only 50% correct classification by chance, an overall 85% accuracy rate suggests that Dim 2 is a useful predictor in separating the two groups. A linear-weighted combination of the two dimensions yielded a slightly lower overall 75% accuracy rate, 80% and 70% correct for Chinese and English subjects, respectively (canonical R 2 = 0.75). Dim 1 height, on the other hand, was not a useful predictor of language group membership; 50% chance level for both groups (canonical R 2 = 0.13). 4. Discussion 4.1. Influence of language experience on the MMN response: categories or dimensions? With the inclusion of MMN responses to a third condition (T1/T2), in addition to the two conditions (T1/T3, T2/T3) from Chandrasekaran et al. (2007), it can be seen that the native Chinese group exhibits larger MMN responses than the English group to the T1/T2 and T1/T3 conditions, but not to the T2/T3 (Fig. 3). Whereas the MMN responses to the T1/T2 and T1/T3 Table 2 Classification matrix from discriminant analysis of Chinese and English language groups for separate and combined dimensions from the INDSCAL group stimulus space Predicted group Original group n Chinese English Dimension 1 (Height) Chinese English Dimension 2 (Contour) Chinese English Dimensions (Height + Contour) Chinese English conditions are larger than that to the T2/T3 condition for the Chinese group, no condition effects are evident for the English group. By focusing on individual tones, however, we miss the phonetic generalization that both T1/T2 and T1/T3 involve a comparison between a level and contour tone, in contrast to T2/T3, which involves a comparison between two tones with similar contours. By applying an individual differences model of multidimensional scaling (Carroll & Chang, 1970; Harshman, 1970), we are now able to quantify not only the number and nature of the pitch dimensions underlying the auditory electrophysiological responses, but also the relative weighting of these dimensions as a function of language experience.

10 204 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 4.2. Underlying dimensions of preattentive tone processing INDSCAL analysis reveals that two dimensions underlie the scaled MMN dissimilarities data across Chinese and English subjects. As measured by average F 0 or F 0 offset (Table 1), Dim 1 appears to be organized on the basis of pitch height (Fig. 4). Prima facie, Dim 2, on the other hand, appears to reflect measures related to F 0 slope (onset to turning point; turning point to offset) and/or another measure of pitch height (F 0 onset). Instead we adduce both internal and external evidence to support our interpretation that Dim 2 is organized primarily on the basis of pitch contour. Because of the limited number of stimulus objects in this study, the question remains as to how many such pitch dimensions are represented separately in auditory sensory memory. In the current study, irrespective of group, the peak latency of the MMN occurs later for the T2/T3 condition (200 ms) relative to T1/T2 or T1/T3 ( ms). If the MMN is responding primarily to pitch onset, we would not expect to observe a condition effect for peak latency. Pitch onset is a static cue that occurs at a single point in time. Two conditions (T1/T2, T1/T3) involve a comparison between a level tone and a contour tone; the other condition, T2/T3, a comparison between two rising contour tones. The later MMN for the T2/T3 condition presumably reflects the greater time that is required to disambiguate these two tones based on a dynamic cue, i.e., pitch contour. Moreover, it is well known that the MMN occurs between ms post onset for phonetic stimuli (Rinne et al., 1999). In this study, the relatively later range of peak latency ( ms) reinforces the notion that the MMN response is integrated over time rather than being elicited by a single event at the onset of the stimulus. The MMN peak latency is a function of the magnitude of acoustic deviance between standard and deviant (Naatanen et al., 1997). Besides acoustic differences between stimuli, the MMN mean amplitude can also be shaped by language experience (Naatanen, 2001; Naatanen, Tervaniemi, Sussman, Paavilainen, & Winkler, 2001). Therefore, it is possible that the later occurring negativity for T2/T3 simply reflects greater acoustic similarity between T2 and T3 relative to T1/T2 and T1/T3 (Table 1, Fig. 4). Based on psychoacoustics of pitch onset alone, we would predict that the mean MMN amplitude for the English group to be larger in the T1/T3 and T1/T2 conditions relative to T2/T3. If pitch onset is the primary cue for Dim 2, we would further expect both groups to show larger MMN responses for the T1/T3 condition compared to T1/T2. These predictions are not borne out. MMN mean amplitude does not differ between any of the three conditions in the English group; nor does it differ between T1/T2 vs. T1/T3 across groups. Instead, our findings suggest that the MMN peak latency reflects multiple pitch dimensions, differentially weighted by language experience, and integrated over time. The presence of multiple dimensions in our speech stimuli, as compared to those that vary along a single dimension only, results in a later negativity. That is, pitch onset per se, a static cue based on a single point in time, cannot account for the longer time frame of the MMN. External behavioral data also support the labeling of Dim 2 as a pitch contour dimension. Based on earlier multidimensional scaling investigations of tone perception across five tone language groups (Cantonese, Mandarin, Taiwanese, Thai, Yoruba) and a non-tone language group (English), three dimensions emerge in the group stimulus space: height, direction, and contour (Gandour, 1983; Gandour & Harshman, 1978). Crosslanguage differences in perceptual saliency of dimensions can be related to the presence of specific types of lexical tones (e.g., level, contour) as well as to tone rules in the listeners phonological system (Huang, 2004; Hume & Johnson, 2001). Of relevance to Dim 2, the height dimension is found to be important to all language groups regardless of typology, whereas the direction and contour dimensions are relatively more important to speakers of tone languages. In the case of Mandarin Chinese vs. English (Gandour, 1983), the English group gave more weight to the height dimension than the Mandarin group, whereas the reverse group effect occurred on the direction dimension. Previous MDS investigations of tone perception constructed dissimilarity matrices from direct comparisons of tonal pairs (Gandour, 1983; Gandour & Harshman, 1978) or from an inverse of reaction time to tonal pairs (Huang, 2004). In the current study, the dissimilarity matrices for subjects were constructed from the differences in ERP responses obtained from passive listening conditions. Despite methodological differences in stimuli, task, and subjects, the two dimensions obtained from the ERPs (pitch height, pitch contour) are consistent with those described in the earlier literature. This convergence of behavioral and electrophysiological data bolsters the view that these two pitch dimensions are crucial for understanding language-dependent effects on tonal processing in the brain. Choosing a higher-dimensional solution may lead to an over-inflation of the variance accounted for (Hair,

11 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 205 Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998). In this study, however, the selection of a two-dimensional solution was essential for revealing crosslanguage differences in MMN sensitivity to Chinese tones. Previous MDS studies have consistently shown that at least two dimensions are used in the processing of tone, and that these two dimensions are most likely to reflect pitch height and pitch direction or contour. The one-dimensional solution accounted for much less variance than the twodimensional, thus justifying that a two-dimensional solution was necessary to account for the dissimilarities data. Taken together, the interpretability of the solutions, the variance accounted for, the distribution of subject weights by language group, and the discriminant analysis of subject weights all point to the twodimensional solution as offering the best insights into preattentive processing of lexical tone. A truly one-dimensional solution, on the other hand, constitutes a fairly uninteresting special case for the INDSCAL model (Arabie, Carroll, & DeSarbo, 1987, p. 35). In that case, any group differences in individual weights would be nothing more than a scaling factor. A two-dimensional solution, on the other hand, gives us an opportunity to evaluate the influence of language experience on early cortical processing of different pitch dimensions in the speech signal Influence of language experience on the dimensions of preattentive tone processing Our findings on the relative weighting of Dim 2 show that MMN responses of the Chinese group are more sensitive to pitch contour than the English group. A discriminant function using this pitch contour dimension is highly successful in separating the two language groups. Consistent with earlier MDS data (Gandour, 1983; Gandour & Harshman, 1978), the greater sensitivity of the MMN to Dim 2 in the Chinese group, relative to English, provides a neurobiological signature of the relevance of the pitch contour in early cortical stages of tonal processing. Such crosslanguage differences in the effects of the pitch contour dimension on the mismatch negativity are compatible with its role in language typology. In Pike s dichotomy (Pike, 1948), there are contour-tone and register-tone languages. The former refers to tonal systems in which tones are best described in terms of single points within a pitch range, the latter to those in which tones are better described on the basis of gliding pitch movements. Mandarin Chinese is classified as a contour-tone language. Since pitch contour is a critical phonetic feature of the Mandarin tone space, native speakers place more emphasis on it, relative to nonnative speakers, in early stages of pitch extraction from the auditory signal. The relative importance of Dim 1, on the other hand, is the same for Chinese and English speakers alike, indicating that this dimension is shared in common regardless of language typology. A discriminant function based on pitch height can achieve no better than chance rates of classifying Chinese and English subjects into their respective language group. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that the effect of language experience on electrophysiological responses varies depending on specific pitch dimensions that underlie the tonal categories rather than the categories themselves Comparison to crosslanguage studies of tone processing at the level of the cortex and the auditory brainstem Our finding that language experience influences preattentive processing of pitch information in the cerebral cortex is compatible with recent crosslanguage investigations of pitch processing at the brainstem level. As measured in the human frequency following response (FFR), pitch strength and pitch tracking accuracy have been reported to be greater for Chinese listeners than for English across all four Mandarin tones (Krishnan, Xu, Gandour, & Cariani, 2005). Our MMN data notwithstanding, their brainstem data suggest that there may be even enhancing or priming of linguisticallyrelevant pitch information well before the auditory signal reaches the cerebral cortex. Their stimuli were identical to those in this study, i.e., prototypical, curvilinear F 0 contours of Mandarin tones. In a subsequent FFR study (Xu, Krishnan, & Gandour, 2006), pitch strength and tracking accuracy were examined in linear rising and falling F 0 ramps representative of Mandarin T2 and T4. Interestingly, no crosslanguage differences were observed in pitch strength or accuracy for either tone, indicating that stimuli with linear rising/falling ramps elicit homogeneous pitch representations at the level of the brainstem. In the case of Mandarin T2 and T4, native listeners long-term learning experience has improved their ability to rapidly track nonlinear changes in pitch movement at the syllable level with a high degree of accuracy. No language-dependent effects, however, are observed in response to linear rising or falling F 0 ramps because they are not part of native Chinese listeners experience. Taken together, these findings lead us to conclude that pitch extraction at the brainstem level is critically dependent on specific

12 206 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions dimensions of pitch contours that native speakers have been exposed to in natural speech contexts. Both local reorganization (Krishnan et al., 2005), and corticofugal modulation of brainstem neural activity appear to be implicated in this experience dependent effect. It remains to be determined just exactly how fine-grained is the degree of linguistic specificity for pitch extraction at both cortical (MMN) and subcortical (FFR) stages of processing. Our crosslanguage effects are also compatible with PET (positron emission tomography) and fmri (functional magnetic resonance imaging) investigations of pitch processing in native vs. nonnative speakers of tone languages at the level of the cerebral cortex (see Gandour, 2006a; 2006b, for reviews). A major finding is that the left hemisphere (LH) is implicated in pitch processing only when the pitch contours are of linguistic relevance to the listener. For example, when asked to discriminate Thai tones, Chinese listeners fail to show activation of LH regions (Gandour et al., 2000; Gandour et al., 2002), indicating that the influence of experience-dependent parameters of the auditory signal is specific to a particular language. This influence of categorical representations on pitch processing has been further demonstrated using hybrid stimuli created by superimposing Thai tones onto Mandarin syllables (tonal chimeras) and Mandarin tones onto the same syllables (real words) (Xu et al., 2006). In the left planum temporale, a double dissociation occurs between language experience (Chinese, Thai) and neural representation of pitch, such that stronger activity is elicited in response to native as compared to non-native tones. These brain imaging experiments, however, all used speeded response discrimination tasks that carry considerable attention and memory demands. In this experiment, the task did not involve selective attention or working memory. Our ERP data are seen to complement those of PET and fmri. With its superior temporal resolution, our ERP data show that the influence of tonal categories not only modulates early automatic preattentive cortical activity, but moreover, that neural mechanisms operating on pitch information at this very early stage of processing may be based primarily on phonetic dimensions underlying these tonal categories. Future research is to be directed to the time course of cortical dynamics of tonal processing and its spatiallydistributed neural circuitry in the cerebral cortex as well as in subcortical areas Applying the INDSCAL model to crosslanguage comparisons of MMN sensitivity to speech stimuli A crucial assumption of the INDSCAL model is that subjects use a common set of dimensions, though they may differ in the relative weighting of each dimension. Previous INDSCAL applications have relied on subjective perceptions of similarities or dissimilarities between objects. Similarity and dissimilarity matrices have typically been constructed using reaction time information, paired comparisons, preference scales, or direct rankings. All these behavioral techniques encounter issues of high intra-subject as well as intersubject variability due in large part to working memory and selective attention demands related to the task. In the current study, we minimize intra-subject variability by applying the INDSCAL model directly to auditory electrophysiological responses elicited from stimuli presented in a passive oddball paradigm. The task is passive in the sense that subjects were instructed to ignore the auditory stimuli presented via earphones; to refrain from extraneous body movements; and to focus their attention exclusively on a self-selected, closedcaption silent movie. In such tasks, the MMN is considered preattentive because the task requires no voluntary attention or working memory. The majority of MMN studies of speech stimuli have been designed so that only a single dimension distinguishes the deviant from its standard. For example, in crosslanguage studies of vowel perception (Cheour et al., 1998; Naatanen et al., 1997), only the second formant was varied across stimuli. Other formants and voice fundamental frequency were held constant. Similarly, in a crosslanguage study of the perception of stop consonants (Sharma & Dorman, 1999), voice onset time (VOT) was the only variable that distinguished the standards and deviants. The perceptual saliency of these particular cues (F2/F1 ratio, VOT) notwithstanding, it is well known that speakers utilize multiple cues in processing speech stimuli. And moreover, these cues may be more or less important depending on native language experience, or even second language (L2) learning. In a perceptual training study of learning the three-way voicing contrast in Korean stop consonants (Francis & Nusbaum, 2002), MDS was used to show that after training, L2 listeners perceptual space undergoes restructuring in terms of the relative weighting of acoustic-phonetic dimensions. Of relevance to the issue of multiple cues, phonetic learning resulted in L2 listeners being able to direct their attention to a previously unattended dimension of phonetic contrast that

13 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 207 has no analog in their native language (English). Using the INDSCAL model, we can assess directly subjects relative weighting of multiple dimensions, and moreover, determine the extent to which any differential effects can be attributed to language experience. In the current study, the stimulus set consisted of three lexical tones that are distinguished by timevarying F 0 trajectories (Fig. 1). There are multiple spectral and temporal cues that can possibly separate the three tones (Table 1). If MMN responses to each condition are examined separately (T1/T2, T1/T3, T2/T3), we are then unable to generalize beyond the level of a tonal category. As a consequence, any similarities in MMN responses between stimulus pairs are seen to be accidental. It is only by applying INDSCAL to MMN mean amplitudes across all three conditions that we are able to extract the two underlying dimensions: pitch height and pitch contour. Though their relative weighting varies as a function of native vs. nonnative language experience, it is especially noteworthy that the pitch contour dimension is the one that clearly separates the two language groups. Not all dimensions are created equal when it comes to determining what language group a listener belongs to. Such differences in their relative importance can be attributed to abstract structural properties of a listener s phonological system (Gandour, 1983). Such insights are only possible by applying statistical methods (e.g., INDSCAL) that permit us to examine the relative weighting of features or dimensions that make up the speech categories Experience-dependent plasticity Second language learning The relative weighting of acoustic features is critical to understanding difficulties faced by learners of a second language. In a crosslanguage (English, German, Japanese) investigation of the perception of English /r/ vs./l/(iverson et al., 2003), Iverson et al. applied MDS analysis to perceptual judgments of stimuli varying in F2 and F3. Of these two formants, variation in F3 is the preeminent acoustic cue for distinguishing /r/ from /l/ (Goto, 1971; Miyawaki et al., 1975). In English and German, this segmental contrast is phonemic; it is nonphonemic in Japanese. English and German listeners placed relatively more emphasis on the F3 dimension. Japanese listeners, on the other hand, directed their attention primarily to F2. Their difficulties in the acquisition of this segmental contrast may be attributed to their reliance (or weighting) on a dimension (F2) that is irrelevant in differentiating English /r/vs. /l/. Similar difficulties in the L2 acquisition of a suprasegmental contrast are to be expected by nonnative learners of a tone language (Kiriloff, 1969; Shen, 1989). In this study, MMN responses of the Chinese group are more sensitive to pitch contour than the English group. Such findings agree with previous MDS studies showing that native Mandarin speakers rely on changes in F 0 contours more than height in distinguishing tones, whereas speakers of non-tone languages (English) tend to attach more importance to height (Gandour, 1983; Gandour, 1984). Since English has no contrastive tones, English listeners might be expected to focus their attention more on pitch height. Their difficulties in the acquisition of this suprasegmental contrast may be attributed to their reliance on a dimension (pitch height) that is of secondary importance in the Mandarin tone space. Interestingly, the perception of Mandarin tones by adult English L2 learners can be improved with auditory training (Wang, Spence, Jongman, & Sereno, 1999). Using multiple talkers, both male and female, L2 learners were able to achieve a 21% increase in the accuracy of tone perception after training. Wang et al. infer that this improvement results from trainees ability to focus more attention on pitch contour than height. At the cortical level, L2 acquisition of Chinese tones, as measured by tonal identification, involves both the expansion of left language-related areas and the recruitment of right hemisphere regions specialized for functions related to pitch (Wang, Sereno, Jongman, & Hirsch, 2003). Using INDSCAL, it is now possible to investigate whether the mismatch negativity, a measure of early preattentive cortical processing, is responsive to changes in the relative weighting of pitch dimensions that result from auditory training Speech specific or domain general? Our experimental design did not include comparisons of homologous non-speech stimuli with timevarying pitch. Hence we cannot determine conclusively whether the experience-dependent plasticity of the MMN is specific to speech or whether it is domain general. In a crosslanguage (Chinese, English) study of categorical perception (CP) of speech and nonspeech continuua ranging from level (T1) to rising (T2) (Xu et al., 2006), results show evidence of strong categorical perception of speech stimuli for Chinese but not English listeners. CP of nonspeech stimuli was comparable to that for speech stimuli for Chinese but weaker for English listeners. Their findings suggest that the percep-

14 208 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions tual salience of the pitch direction or contour dimension is greater for native speakers of Mandarin Chinese than for speakers of non-tone languages. Linguistic experience directs attention to linguistically-relevant properties of the auditory signal. We therefore conclude that Chinese listeners nonspeech performance must derive at least in part from their experience with listening to Chinese pitch patterns. These data lead us to infer that Chinese listeners native language experience has changed the way they process pitch patterns regardless of the stimulus context in which these patterns are embedded. Although the basis for cross-language differences in CP may emerge from linguistic experience, the effects of such experience are not specific to speech perception. More generally, we predict CP effects whenever listeners are asked to judge auditory features that are similar to linguistically-relevant speech parameters in their native language no matter whether they are presented in the context of natural speech or not. With respect to the mismatch negativity, language experience can modify the magnitude of the MMN responses to linguistically-relevant cues in nonspeech contexts (Tervaniemi et al., 2006). Native speakers of Finnish, for whom the length of a consonant or vowel conveys differences in word meaning (e.g., /tuli/ fire, /tuuli/ wind, /tulli/ customs ), showed larger MMN responses to nonspeech stimuli that differ in duration, relative to German speakers, for whom duration cues are not phonemic. Interestingly, the two language groups did not differ on nonspeech stimuli differing in frequency, presumably because pitch is not phonemic for either language group. Thus, it appears that preattentive cortical processing can be selectively tuned to those sound features of the auditory signal that are of phonological relevance in a particular language even in nonspeech sounds (Tervaniemi et al., 2006, p. 2541). In the case of voice fundamental frequency (F 0 ), the next step is to determine whether selective tuning extends to frequency as well as duration in nonspeech sounds. Musical experience can influence the preattentive processing of several dimensions of the auditory signal, including intensity (Tervaniemi, Castaneda, Knoll, & Uther, 2006), spatial location (Tervaniemi, Castaneda et al., 2006; Nager, Kohlmetz, Altenmuller, Rodriguez- Fornells, & Munte, 2003), and pitch (Fujioka, Trainor, Ross, Kakigi, & Pantev, 2004; Koelsch, Schroger, & Tervaniemi, 1999; Tervaniemi, Rytkonen, Schroger, Ilmoniemi, & Naatanen, 2001). Pop and jazz musicians, who predominantly learned by ear, showed superior ability to process musical contours than classical musicians, who relied on musical scores (Tervaniemi, Rytkonen, Schroger, Ilmoniemi, & Naatanen, 2001). In terms of the MMNm (magnetic version of the electrical MMN), musicians showed a larger MMNm in response to musical contours and intervals when compared to non-musicians (Fujioka, Trainor, Ross, Kakigi, & Pantev, 2004). A recent model of music processing, based on data from individuals with neurological impairments, incorporates functionally dissociable components for tonal encoding, contour, and interval analysis (Peretz & Coltheart, 2003). Tonal encoding is argued to be specific to music because it is based on a hierarchical organization of scale tones to one another (Peretz, 2006; Peretz & Coltheart, 2003). Our data suggest it is possible that lexical tones may be similarly organized in a hierarchical fashion depending on their acoustic-phonetic dimensions in a languagespecific tone space. Whether tonal encoding in music is comparable to that in speech is a matter for further empirical investigation Separate processing of pitch dimensions in auditory sensory memory On the basis of additivity of the MMN response and neurally separate generators for different dimensions of musical timbre, Caclin et al. (2006) suggest that the auditory sensory memory represents different features of the stimuli separately. Their results, however, do not completely preclude holistic processing of stimulus features. They allow that the MMN may reflect both separate and holistic analysis of auditory attributes, perhaps at different stages of processing. Our findings on pitch are compatible with theirs on timbre insomuch as we show that two separate pitch dimensions (height, contour) can be extracted from the MMN signal. By virtue of language-dependent enhancement of one of the pitch dimensions (contour) for native (Chinese) relative to non-native (English) speakers, we similarly argue that features of the signal are indeed processed separately in auditory sensory memory. Since the experiment was not designed to tease apart holistic from separate feature processing, we cannot rule out holistic processing at a later processing stage. 5. Conclusion By applying individual differences multidimensional scaling (INDSCAL) analysis to the MMN mean amplitude, we are able to extract dimensions underlying the preattentive processing of linguistically-relevant time-

15 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions 209 varying pitch patterns. For at least some of the processes indexed by the MMN, the relevant entities are pitch dimensions and not pitch as a whole. Two pitch dimensions, height and contour, underlie Chinese and English subject s processing of the three Mandarin tones (T1, T2, T3). Discriminant analysis indicates that pitch contour, not height, effectively separates native speakers of Chinese from English. Thus, not only can we extract the dimensions underlying the processing of standards and deviants that vary in more than one dimension, but we can also measure the extent to which individual differences in pitch processing are influenced by language experience, as reflected in the mismatch negativity. More broadly, INDSCAL gives us a tool for exploring the number and nature of neural dimensions underlying the MMN and their relative importance to one another as a function of one s auditory experience. Acknowledgments Research supported in part by the Purdue Research Foundation (J. G.) and the College of Liberal Arts (A. K.). Thanks to Eunjung Lim, Bruce Craig, and Yagna Kalyanaraman for their assistance with statistical analysis. B. C. is currently a predoctoral student in the Purdue University Life Sciences Integrative Neuroscience Program. References Arabie, P., Carroll, J. D., & DeSarbo, W. S. (1987). Three-way scaling and clustering. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Caclin, A., Brattico, E., Tervaniemi, M., Naatanen, R., Morlet, D., Giard, M.-H., et al. (2006). Separate neural processing of timbre dimensions in auditory sensory memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), Caclin, A., McAdams, S., Smith, B. K., & Winsberg, S. (2005). Acoustic correlates of timbre space dimensions: A confirmatory study using synthetic tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(1), Carroll, J. D., & Chang, J. J. (1970). Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional scaling via an N-way generalization of Eckart-Young decomposition. Psychometrika, 35, Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2007). Mismatch negativity to pitch contours is influenced by language experience. Brain Res, 1128(1), Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K., et al. (1998). Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nat Neurosci, 1(5), Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E., & Gout, A. (2000). Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: a crosslinguistic study. J Cogn Neurosci, 12(4), Francis, A. L., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2002). Selective attention and the acquisition of new phonetic categories. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 28(2), Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., & Pantev, C. (2004). Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure. J Cogn Neurosci, 16(6), Gandour, J. (1983). Tone perception in Far Eastern languages. Journal of Phonetics, 11, Gandour, J. (1984). Tone dissimilarity judgments by Chinese listeners. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 12, Gandour, J. (2006a). Brain mapping of Chinese speech prosody. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics: Chinese (Vol. 1, pp ). New York: Cambridge University Press. Gandour, J. (2006b). Tone: Neurophonetics. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd ed., Vol. 12, pp ). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. Gandour, J., & Harshman, R. (1978). Crosslanguage differences in tone perception: a multidimensional scaling investigation. Language and Speech, 21, Gandour, J., Wong, D., Hsieh, L., Weinzapfel, B., Van Lancker, D., & Hutchins, G.D. (2000). A crosslinguistic PET study of tone perception. J Cogn Neurosci, 12(1), Gandour, J., Wong, D., Lowe, M., Dzemidzic, M., Satthamnuwong, N., Tong, Y., et al. (2002). A cross-linguistic FMRI study of spectral and temporal cues underlying phonological processing. J Cogn Neurosci, 14(7), Goto, H. (1971). Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of sounds L and R. Neuropsychologia, 9(3), Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Harshman, R. (1970). Foundations of the PARAFAC procedure: Models and conditions for an explanatory multi-modal factor analysis (Vol. 10,085). Ann Arbor: University Microfilms (No. 10, 085). Honbolygo, F., Csepe, V., & Rago, A. (2004). Suprasegmental speech cues are automatically processed by the human brain: a mismatch negativity study. Neurosci Lett, 363(1), Howie, J. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Huang, T. (2004). Language-specificity in auditory perception of Chinese tones. Unpublished Dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH. Hume, E., & Johnson, K. (2001). A model of the interplay of speech perception and phonology. In E. Hume & K. Johnson (Eds.), The role of speech perception in phonology (pp. 3-25). New York: Academic Press. Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesch, E., Tohkura, Y., Kettermann, A., et al. (2003). A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition, 87(1), Kiriloff, C. (1969). On the auditory discrimination of tones in Mandarin. Phonetica, 20, Klatt, D. H. (1980). Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67(3), Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L. C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87(2), Koelsch, S., Schroger, E., & Tervaniemi, M. (1999). Superior pre-attentive auditory processing in musicians. Neuroreport, 10(6), Kraus, N., & Cheour, M. (2000). Speech sound representation in the brain. Audiol Neurootol, 5(3-4),

16 210 B. Chandrasekaran et al. / Neuroplasticity in the processing of pitch dimensions Kraus, N., McGee, T., Carrell, T., King, C., Tremblay, K., & Nicol, T. (1995). Central auditory system plasticity associated with speech discrimination training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, Krishnan, A., Xu, Y., Gandour, J., & Cariani, P. (2005). Encoding of pitch in the human brainstem is sensitive to language experience. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 25(1), Miyawaki, K., Liberman, A. M., Strange, W., Verbrugge, R., Liberman, A. M., Jenkins, J. J., et al. (1975). Effect of linguistic experience Discrimination of [R] and [L] by native speakers of Japanese and English. Perception & Psychophysics, 18(5), Naatanen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38(1), Naatanen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., et al. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature, 385(6615), Naatanen, R., Tervaniemi, M., Sussman, E., Paavilainen, P., & Winkler, I. (2001). Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex. Trends Neurosci, 24(5), Nager, W., Kohlmetz, C., Altenmuller, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Munte, T. F. (2003). The fate of sounds in conductors brains: an ERP study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 17(1), Nenonen, S., Shestakova, A., Huotilainen, M., & Naatanen, R. (2003). Linguistic relevance of duration within the native language determines the accuracy of speech-sound duration processing. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 16(3), Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychol Methods, 8(4), Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100(1), Peretz, I., & Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity of music processing. Nat Neurosci, 6(7), Pike, K. L. (1948). Tone languages. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Rinne, T., Alho, K., Alku, P., Holi, M., Sinkkonen, J., Virtanen, J., et al. (1999). Analysis of speech sounds is left-hemisphere predominant at ms after sound onset. Neuroreport, 10(5), Sharma, A., & Dorman, M. F. (1999). Cortical auditory evoked potential correlates of categorical perception of voice-onset time. J Acoust Soc Am, 106(2), Sharma, A., & Dorman, M. F. (2000). Neurophysiologic correlates of cross-language phonetic perception. J Acoust Soc Am, 107(5 Pt 1), Shen, X. S. (1989). Toward a register approach in teaching Mandarin tones. Journal of Chinese Language Teachers Association, 24, Tervaniemi, M., Castaneda, A., Knoll, M., & Uther, M. (2006). Sound processing in amateur musicians and nonmusicians: event-related potential and behavioral indices. Neuroreport, 17(11), Tervaniemi, M., Jacobsen, T., Rottger, S., Kujala, T., Widmann, A., Vainio, M., et al. (2006). Selective tuning of cortical soundfeature processing by language experience. Eur J Neurosci, 23(9), Tervaniemi, M., Rytkonen, M., Schroger, E., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Naatanen, R. (2001). Superior formation of cortical memory traces for melodic patterns in musicians. Learning & Memory, 8(5), Wang, Y., Sereno, J. A., Jongman, A., & Hirsch, J. (2003). fmri evidence for cortical modification during learning of Mandarin lexical tone. J Cogn Neurosci, 15(7), Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(6), Winkler, I., Kujala, T., Tiitinen, H., Sivonen, P., Alku, P., Lehtokoski, A., et al. (1999). Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. Psychophysiology, 36(5), Xu, Y. (1997). Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of Phonetics, 25, Xu, Y., Gandour, J., Talavage, T., Wong, D., Dzemidzic, M., Tong, Y., et al. (2006). Activation of the left planum temporale in pitch processing is shaped by language experience. Human Brain Mapping, 27(2), Xu, Y., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2006). Specificity of experience-dependent pitch representation in the brainstem. Neuroreport, 17(15), Ylinen, S., Shestakova, A., Huotilainen, M., Alku, P., & Naatanen, R. (2006). Mismatch negativity (MMN) elicited by changes in phoneme length: a cross-linguistic study. Brain Res, 1072(1),

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES

L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES L2 EXPERIENCE MODULATES LEARNERS USE OF CUES IN THE PERCEPTION OF L3 TONES Zhen Qin, Allard Jongman Department of Linguistics, University of Kansas, United States qinzhenquentin2@ku.edu, ajongman@ku.edu

More information

Workshop Perceptual Effects of Filtering and Masking Introduction to Filtering and Masking

Workshop Perceptual Effects of Filtering and Masking Introduction to Filtering and Masking Workshop Perceptual Effects of Filtering and Masking Introduction to Filtering and Masking The perception and correct identification of speech sounds as phonemes depends on the listener extracting various

More information

Program. 11:15 Prof. Heikki Hämäläinen, Department of Psychology, University of Turku: 12:15 Does aging involve hemispatial neglect?

Program. 11:15 Prof. Heikki Hämäläinen, Department of Psychology, University of Turku: 12:15 Does aging involve hemispatial neglect? AUDITORY AND VISUAL INFORMATION PROCESSING IN THE HUMAN BRAIN A symposium organized by the Finnish Graduate School of Psychology October 8 9, 2008, Helsinki, Finland This symposium is open to all doctoral

More information

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics

ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics ERP indices of lab-learned phonotactics Claire Moore-Cantwell, Joe Pater, Robert Staubs, Benjamin Zobel and Lisa Sanders RUMMIT UMass Amherst, April 6th 2013 Introduction: learning phonology in the lab

More information

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain).

Functional neuroimaging. Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). Functional neuroimaging Imaging brain function in real time (not just the structure of the brain). The brain is bloody & electric Blood increase in neuronal activity increase in metabolic demand for glucose

More information

Thirukkural - A Text-to-Speech Synthesis System

Thirukkural - A Text-to-Speech Synthesis System Thirukkural - A Text-to-Speech Synthesis System G. L. Jayavardhana Rama, A. G. Ramakrishnan, M Vijay Venkatesh, R. Murali Shankar Department of Electrical Engg, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore 560012,

More information

Sensory Dissonance Using Memory Model

Sensory Dissonance Using Memory Model kristoffer Jensen Sensory Dissonance Using Memory Model Introduction In its traditional form, music is a sequence of notes and other sounds. As such, it has rhythm, i.e. the notes have onset times at regular

More information

Video-Based Eye Tracking

Video-Based Eye Tracking Video-Based Eye Tracking Our Experience with Advanced Stimuli Design for Eye Tracking Software A. RUFA, a G.L. MARIOTTINI, b D. PRATTICHIZZO, b D. ALESSANDRINI, b A. VICINO, b AND A. FEDERICO a a Department

More information

Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes

Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 638 642. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes

More information

The Effects of Speaking Rate on Mandarin Tones. C2008 Yun H. Stockton

The Effects of Speaking Rate on Mandarin Tones. C2008 Yun H. Stockton The Effects of Speaking Rate on Mandarin Tones BY C2008 Yun H. Stockton Submitted to the graduate degree program in Linguistics and the Graduate Faculty of the University of Kansas in partial fulfillment

More information

THREE DIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF AMINO ACID CHARAC- TERISTICS

THREE DIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF AMINO ACID CHARAC- TERISTICS THREE DIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF AMINO ACID CHARAC- TERISTICS O.U. Sezerman 1, R. Islamaj 2, E. Alpaydin 2 1 Laborotory of Computational Biology, Sabancı University, Istanbul, Turkey. 2 Computer Engineering

More information

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology 1.Obtaining Knowledge 1. Correlation 2. Causation 2.Hypothesis Generation & Measures 3.Looking into

More information

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An ontogenetic study of infant speech perception

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An ontogenetic study of infant speech perception Dept. for Speech, Music and Hearing Quarterly Progress and Status Report An ontogenetic study of infant speech perception Lacerda, F. and Aurelius, G. and Landberg,. and Roug-Hellichius, L-L. journal:

More information

Artificial Neural Network for Speech Recognition

Artificial Neural Network for Speech Recognition Artificial Neural Network for Speech Recognition Austin Marshall March 3, 2005 2nd Annual Student Research Showcase Overview Presenting an Artificial Neural Network to recognize and classify speech Spoken

More information

Review of Fundamental Mathematics

Review of Fundamental Mathematics Review of Fundamental Mathematics As explained in the Preface and in Chapter 1 of your textbook, managerial economics applies microeconomic theory to business decision making. The decision-making tools

More information

CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS.

CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS. CHAPTER 6 PRINCIPLES OF NEURAL CIRCUITS. 6.1. CONNECTIONS AMONG NEURONS Neurons are interconnected with one another to form circuits, much as electronic components are wired together to form a functional

More information

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive Statistics Descriptive Statistics Primer Descriptive statistics Central tendency Variation Relative position Relationships Calculating descriptive statistics Descriptive Statistics Purpose to describe or summarize

More information

LAB 7 MOSFET CHARACTERISTICS AND APPLICATIONS

LAB 7 MOSFET CHARACTERISTICS AND APPLICATIONS LAB 7 MOSFET CHARACTERISTICS AND APPLICATIONS Objective In this experiment you will study the i-v characteristics of an MOS transistor. You will use the MOSFET as a variable resistor and as a switch. BACKGROUND

More information

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex

2 Neurons. 4 The Brain: Cortex 1 Neuroscience 2 Neurons output integration axon cell body, membrane potential Frontal planning control auditory episodes soma motor Temporal Parietal action language objects space vision Occipital inputs

More information

Overview of Methodology. Human Electrophysiology. Computing and Displaying Difference Waves. Plotting The Averaged ERP

Overview of Methodology. Human Electrophysiology. Computing and Displaying Difference Waves. Plotting The Averaged ERP Human Electrophysiology Overview of Methodology This Week: 1. Displaying ERPs 2. Defining ERP components Analog Filtering Amplification Montage Selection Analog-Digital Conversion Signal-to-Noise Enhancement

More information

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores

II. DISTRIBUTIONS distribution normal distribution. standard scores Appendix D Basic Measurement And Statistics The following information was developed by Steven Rothke, PhD, Department of Psychology, Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago (RIC) and expanded by Mary F. Schmidt,

More information

Least-Squares Intersection of Lines

Least-Squares Intersection of Lines Least-Squares Intersection of Lines Johannes Traa - UIUC 2013 This write-up derives the least-squares solution for the intersection of lines. In the general case, a set of lines will not intersect at a

More information

UNDERSTANDING THE TWO-WAY ANOVA

UNDERSTANDING THE TWO-WAY ANOVA UNDERSTANDING THE e have seen how the one-way ANOVA can be used to compare two or more sample means in studies involving a single independent variable. This can be extended to two independent variables

More information

Additional sources Compilation of sources: http://lrs.ed.uiuc.edu/tseportal/datacollectionmethodologies/jin-tselink/tselink.htm

Additional sources Compilation of sources: http://lrs.ed.uiuc.edu/tseportal/datacollectionmethodologies/jin-tselink/tselink.htm Mgt 540 Research Methods Data Analysis 1 Additional sources Compilation of sources: http://lrs.ed.uiuc.edu/tseportal/datacollectionmethodologies/jin-tselink/tselink.htm http://web.utk.edu/~dap/random/order/start.htm

More information

Early vs. Late Onset Hearing Loss: How Children Differ from Adults. Andrea Pittman, PhD Arizona State University

Early vs. Late Onset Hearing Loss: How Children Differ from Adults. Andrea Pittman, PhD Arizona State University Early vs. Late Onset Hearing Loss: How Children Differ from Adults Andrea Pittman, PhD Arizona State University Heterogeneity of Children with Hearing Loss Chronological age Age at onset Age at identification

More information

MATTHEW K. GORDON (University of California, Los Angeles) THE NEUTRAL VOWELS OF FINNISH: HOW NEUTRAL ARE THEY? *

MATTHEW K. GORDON (University of California, Los Angeles) THE NEUTRAL VOWELS OF FINNISH: HOW NEUTRAL ARE THEY? * Linguistica Uralica 35:1 (1999), pp. 17-21 MATTHEW K. GORDON (University of California, Los Angeles) THE NEUTRAL VOWELS OF FINNISH: HOW NEUTRAL ARE THEY? * Finnish is well known for possessing a front-back

More information

Functional Auditory Performance Indicators (FAPI)

Functional Auditory Performance Indicators (FAPI) Functional Performance Indicators (FAPI) An Integrated Approach to Skill FAPI Overview The Functional (FAPI) assesses the functional auditory skills of children with hearing loss. It can be used by parents,

More information

Timbre. Chapter 9. 9.1 Definition of timbre modelling. Hanna Järveläinen Giovanni De Poli

Timbre. Chapter 9. 9.1 Definition of timbre modelling. Hanna Järveläinen Giovanni De Poli Chapter 9 Timbre Hanna Järveläinen Giovanni De Poli 9.1 Definition of timbre modelling Giving a definition of timbre modelling is a complicated task. The meaning of the term "timbre" in itself is somewhat

More information

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES.

COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. 277 CHAPTER VI COMPARISONS OF CUSTOMER LOYALTY: PUBLIC & PRIVATE INSURANCE COMPANIES. This chapter contains a full discussion of customer loyalty comparisons between private and public insurance companies

More information

A Segmentation Algorithm for Zebra Finch Song at the Note Level. Ping Du and Todd W. Troyer

A Segmentation Algorithm for Zebra Finch Song at the Note Level. Ping Du and Todd W. Troyer A Segmentation Algorithm for Zebra Finch Song at the Note Level Ping Du and Todd W. Troyer Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, Dept. of Psychology University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742

More information

Factor Analysis. Chapter 420. Introduction

Factor Analysis. Chapter 420. Introduction Chapter 420 Introduction (FA) is an exploratory technique applied to a set of observed variables that seeks to find underlying factors (subsets of variables) from which the observed variables were generated.

More information

Facebook Friend Suggestion Eytan Daniyalzade and Tim Lipus

Facebook Friend Suggestion Eytan Daniyalzade and Tim Lipus Facebook Friend Suggestion Eytan Daniyalzade and Tim Lipus 1. Introduction Facebook is a social networking website with an open platform that enables developers to extract and utilize user information

More information

An Arabic Text-To-Speech System Based on Artificial Neural Networks

An Arabic Text-To-Speech System Based on Artificial Neural Networks Journal of Computer Science 5 (3): 207-213, 2009 ISSN 1549-3636 2009 Science Publications An Arabic Text-To-Speech System Based on Artificial Neural Networks Ghadeer Al-Said and Moussa Abdallah Department

More information

Automatic Detection of Emergency Vehicles for Hearing Impaired Drivers

Automatic Detection of Emergency Vehicles for Hearing Impaired Drivers Automatic Detection of Emergency Vehicles for Hearing Impaired Drivers Sung-won ark and Jose Trevino Texas A&M University-Kingsville, EE/CS Department, MSC 92, Kingsville, TX 78363 TEL (36) 593-2638, FAX

More information

Multivariate Analysis of Ecological Data

Multivariate Analysis of Ecological Data Multivariate Analysis of Ecological Data MICHAEL GREENACRE Professor of Statistics at the Pompeu Fabra University in Barcelona, Spain RAUL PRIMICERIO Associate Professor of Ecology, Evolutionary Biology

More information

Waves: Recording Sound Waves and Sound Wave Interference (Teacher s Guide)

Waves: Recording Sound Waves and Sound Wave Interference (Teacher s Guide) Waves: Recording Sound Waves and Sound Wave Interference (Teacher s Guide) OVERVIEW Students will measure a sound wave by placing the Ward s DataHub microphone near one tuning fork A440 (f=440hz). Then

More information

Statistics. Measurement. Scales of Measurement 7/18/2012

Statistics. Measurement. Scales of Measurement 7/18/2012 Statistics Measurement Measurement is defined as a set of rules for assigning numbers to represent objects, traits, attributes, or behaviors A variableis something that varies (eye color), a constant does

More information

An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers

An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers 언 어 치 료 연 구, 제14 권 제4호 Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders 2005, Vol.14, No.4, 29 ~ 36 An fmri study on reading Hangul and Chinese Characters by Korean Native Speakers Hyo-Woon Yoon(Brain Science Research

More information

Regression III: Advanced Methods

Regression III: Advanced Methods Lecture 16: Generalized Additive Models Regression III: Advanced Methods Bill Jacoby Michigan State University http://polisci.msu.edu/jacoby/icpsr/regress3 Goals of the Lecture Introduce Additive Models

More information

Example: Credit card default, we may be more interested in predicting the probabilty of a default than classifying individuals as default or not.

Example: Credit card default, we may be more interested in predicting the probabilty of a default than classifying individuals as default or not. Statistical Learning: Chapter 4 Classification 4.1 Introduction Supervised learning with a categorical (Qualitative) response Notation: - Feature vector X, - qualitative response Y, taking values in C

More information

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY Objectives Understand Empirical Research Cycle Knowledge of Research Methods Conceptual Understanding of Basic Statistics PSYC 353 11A rsch methods 01/17/11 [Arthur]

More information

Self Organizing Maps: Fundamentals

Self Organizing Maps: Fundamentals Self Organizing Maps: Fundamentals Introduction to Neural Networks : Lecture 16 John A. Bullinaria, 2004 1. What is a Self Organizing Map? 2. Topographic Maps 3. Setting up a Self Organizing Map 4. Kohonen

More information

Speech-Language Pathology Curriculum Foundation Course Linkages

Speech-Language Pathology Curriculum Foundation Course Linkages FACULTY OF HEALTH PROFESSIONS School of Human Communication Disorders Speech-Language Pathology Curriculum Foundation Course Linkages Phonetics (HUCD 5020) a. Vowels b. Consonants c. Suprasegmentals d.

More information

Social Media Mining. Data Mining Essentials

Social Media Mining. Data Mining Essentials Introduction Data production rate has been increased dramatically (Big Data) and we are able store much more data than before E.g., purchase data, social media data, mobile phone data Businesses and customers

More information

Data, Measurements, Features

Data, Measurements, Features Data, Measurements, Features Middle East Technical University Dep. of Computer Engineering 2009 compiled by V. Atalay What do you think of when someone says Data? We might abstract the idea that data are

More information

Torgerson s Classical MDS derivation: 1: Determining Coordinates from Euclidean Distances

Torgerson s Classical MDS derivation: 1: Determining Coordinates from Euclidean Distances Torgerson s Classical MDS derivation: 1: Determining Coordinates from Euclidean Distances It is possible to construct a matrix X of Cartesian coordinates of points in Euclidean space when we know the Euclidean

More information

Correlational Research. Correlational Research. Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP EDS 250. Descriptive Research 1. Correlational Research: Scatter Plots

Correlational Research. Correlational Research. Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP EDS 250. Descriptive Research 1. Correlational Research: Scatter Plots Correlational Research Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP California State University, Sacramento 1 Correlational Research A quantitative methodology used to determine whether, and to what degree, a relationship

More information

PERCENTAGE ARTICULATION LOSS OF CONSONANTS IN THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL CLASSROOMS

PERCENTAGE ARTICULATION LOSS OF CONSONANTS IN THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL CLASSROOMS The 21 st International Congress on Sound and Vibration 13-17 July, 2014, Beijing/China PERCENTAGE ARTICULATION LOSS OF CONSONANTS IN THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL CLASSROOMS Dan Wang, Nanjie Yan and Jianxin Peng*

More information

Emotion Detection from Speech

Emotion Detection from Speech Emotion Detection from Speech 1. Introduction Although emotion detection from speech is a relatively new field of research, it has many potential applications. In human-computer or human-human interaction

More information

6.4 Normal Distribution

6.4 Normal Distribution Contents 6.4 Normal Distribution....................... 381 6.4.1 Characteristics of the Normal Distribution....... 381 6.4.2 The Standardized Normal Distribution......... 385 6.4.3 Meaning of Areas under

More information

The effect of mismatched recording conditions on human and automatic speaker recognition in forensic applications

The effect of mismatched recording conditions on human and automatic speaker recognition in forensic applications Forensic Science International 146S (2004) S95 S99 www.elsevier.com/locate/forsciint The effect of mismatched recording conditions on human and automatic speaker recognition in forensic applications A.

More information

15.062 Data Mining: Algorithms and Applications Matrix Math Review

15.062 Data Mining: Algorithms and Applications Matrix Math Review .6 Data Mining: Algorithms and Applications Matrix Math Review The purpose of this document is to give a brief review of selected linear algebra concepts that will be useful for the course and to develop

More information

PLASTIC REGION BOLT TIGHTENING CONTROLLED BY ACOUSTIC EMISSION MONITORING

PLASTIC REGION BOLT TIGHTENING CONTROLLED BY ACOUSTIC EMISSION MONITORING PLASTIC REGION BOLT TIGHTENING CONTROLLED BY ACOUSTIC EMISSION MONITORING TADASHI ONISHI, YOSHIHIRO MIZUTANI and MASAMI MAYUZUMI 2-12-1-I1-70, O-okayama, Meguro, Tokyo 152-8552, Japan. Abstract Troubles

More information

Fairfield Public Schools

Fairfield Public Schools Mathematics Fairfield Public Schools AP Statistics AP Statistics BOE Approved 04/08/2014 1 AP STATISTICS Critical Areas of Focus AP Statistics is a rigorous course that offers advanced students an opportunity

More information

A Sound Analysis and Synthesis System for Generating an Instrumental Piri Song

A Sound Analysis and Synthesis System for Generating an Instrumental Piri Song , pp.347-354 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/ijmue.2014.9.8.32 A Sound Analysis and Synthesis System for Generating an Instrumental Piri Song Myeongsu Kang and Jong-Myon Kim School of Electrical Engineering,

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind

Cognitive Neuroscience. Questions. Multiple Methods. Electrophysiology. Multiple Methods. Approaches to Thinking about the Mind Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches to Thinking about the Mind Cognitive Neuroscience Evolutionary Approach Sept 20-22, 2004 Interdisciplinary approach Rapidly changing How does the brain enable cognition?

More information

MUSICAL INSTRUMENT FAMILY CLASSIFICATION

MUSICAL INSTRUMENT FAMILY CLASSIFICATION MUSICAL INSTRUMENT FAMILY CLASSIFICATION Ricardo A. Garcia Media Lab, Massachusetts Institute of Technology 0 Ames Street Room E5-40, Cambridge, MA 039 USA PH: 67-53-0 FAX: 67-58-664 e-mail: rago @ media.

More information

MANAGING QUEUE STABILITY USING ART2 IN ACTIVE QUEUE MANAGEMENT FOR CONGESTION CONTROL

MANAGING QUEUE STABILITY USING ART2 IN ACTIVE QUEUE MANAGEMENT FOR CONGESTION CONTROL MANAGING QUEUE STABILITY USING ART2 IN ACTIVE QUEUE MANAGEMENT FOR CONGESTION CONTROL G. Maria Priscilla 1 and C. P. Sumathi 2 1 S.N.R. Sons College (Autonomous), Coimbatore, India 2 SDNB Vaishnav College

More information

INTERPRETING THE ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (ANOVA)

INTERPRETING THE ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (ANOVA) INTERPRETING THE ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (ANOVA) As with other parametric statistics, we begin the one-way ANOVA with a test of the underlying assumptions. Our first assumption is the assumption of

More information

Trigonometric functions and sound

Trigonometric functions and sound Trigonometric functions and sound The sounds we hear are caused by vibrations that send pressure waves through the air. Our ears respond to these pressure waves and signal the brain about their amplitude

More information

Modelling, Extraction and Description of Intrinsic Cues of High Resolution Satellite Images: Independent Component Analysis based approaches

Modelling, Extraction and Description of Intrinsic Cues of High Resolution Satellite Images: Independent Component Analysis based approaches Modelling, Extraction and Description of Intrinsic Cues of High Resolution Satellite Images: Independent Component Analysis based approaches PhD Thesis by Payam Birjandi Director: Prof. Mihai Datcu Problematic

More information

Current Standard: Mathematical Concepts and Applications Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary

Current Standard: Mathematical Concepts and Applications Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary Shape, Space, and Measurement- Primary A student shall apply concepts of shape, space, and measurement to solve problems involving two- and three-dimensional shapes by demonstrating an understanding of:

More information

SPEECH AUDIOMETRY. @ Biswajeet Sarangi, B.Sc.(Audiology & speech Language pathology)

SPEECH AUDIOMETRY. @ Biswajeet Sarangi, B.Sc.(Audiology & speech Language pathology) 1 SPEECH AUDIOMETRY Pure tone Audiometry provides only a partial picture of the patient s auditory sensitivity. Because it doesn t give any information about it s ability to hear and understand speech.

More information

Multivariate Analysis of Variance. The general purpose of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is to determine

Multivariate Analysis of Variance. The general purpose of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is to determine 2 - Manova 4.3.05 25 Multivariate Analysis of Variance What Multivariate Analysis of Variance is The general purpose of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) is to determine whether multiple levels

More information

Lecture 1-10: Spectrograms

Lecture 1-10: Spectrograms Lecture 1-10: Spectrograms Overview 1. Spectra of dynamic signals: like many real world signals, speech changes in quality with time. But so far the only spectral analysis we have performed has assumed

More information

Tonal Detection in Noise: An Auditory Neuroscience Insight

Tonal Detection in Noise: An Auditory Neuroscience Insight Image: http://physics.ust.hk/dusw/ Tonal Detection in Noise: An Auditory Neuroscience Insight Buddhika Karunarathne 1 and Richard H.Y. So 1,2 1 Dept. of IELM, Hong Kong University of Science & Technology,

More information

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS

Chapter Seven. Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS Chapter Seven Multiple regression An introduction to multiple regression Performing a multiple regression on SPSS Section : An introduction to multiple regression WHAT IS MULTIPLE REGRESSION? Multiple

More information

Signal to Noise Instrumental Excel Assignment

Signal to Noise Instrumental Excel Assignment Signal to Noise Instrumental Excel Assignment Instrumental methods, as all techniques involved in physical measurements, are limited by both the precision and accuracy. The precision and accuracy of a

More information

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data.

Masters research projects. 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data. Masters research projects 1. Adapting Granger causality for use on EEG data. Background. Granger causality is a concept introduced in the field of economy to determine which variables influence, or cause,

More information

Analysing Questionnaires using Minitab (for SPSS queries contact -) Graham.Currell@uwe.ac.uk

Analysing Questionnaires using Minitab (for SPSS queries contact -) Graham.Currell@uwe.ac.uk Analysing Questionnaires using Minitab (for SPSS queries contact -) Graham.Currell@uwe.ac.uk Structure As a starting point it is useful to consider a basic questionnaire as containing three main sections:

More information

with functions, expressions and equations which follow in units 3 and 4.

with functions, expressions and equations which follow in units 3 and 4. Grade 8 Overview View unit yearlong overview here The unit design was created in line with the areas of focus for grade 8 Mathematics as identified by the Common Core State Standards and the PARCC Model

More information

Association Between Variables

Association Between Variables Contents 11 Association Between Variables 767 11.1 Introduction............................ 767 11.1.1 Measure of Association................. 768 11.1.2 Chapter Summary.................... 769 11.2 Chi

More information

Measurement with Ratios

Measurement with Ratios Grade 6 Mathematics, Quarter 2, Unit 2.1 Measurement with Ratios Overview Number of instructional days: 15 (1 day = 45 minutes) Content to be learned Use ratio reasoning to solve real-world and mathematical

More information

Canonical Correlation Analysis

Canonical Correlation Analysis Canonical Correlation Analysis LEARNING OBJECTIVES Upon completing this chapter, you should be able to do the following: State the similarities and differences between multiple regression, factor analysis,

More information

Statistical tests for SPSS

Statistical tests for SPSS Statistical tests for SPSS Paolo Coletti A.Y. 2010/11 Free University of Bolzano Bozen Premise This book is a very quick, rough and fast description of statistical tests and their usage. It is explicitly

More information

It s All in the Brain!

It s All in the Brain! It s All in the Brain! Presented by: Mari Hubig, M.Ed. 0-3 Outreach Coordinator Educational Resource Center on Deafness What is the Brain? The brain is a muscle In order to grow and flourish, the brain

More information

Dr. Abdel Aziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology Mansoura Faculty of Medicine

Dr. Abdel Aziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology Mansoura Faculty of Medicine Physiological Basis of Hearing Tests By Dr. Abdel Aziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology Mansoura Faculty of Medicine Introduction Def: Hearing is the ability to perceive certain pressure vibrations in the

More information

Convention Paper Presented at the 112th Convention 2002 May 10 13 Munich, Germany

Convention Paper Presented at the 112th Convention 2002 May 10 13 Munich, Germany Audio Engineering Society Convention Paper Presented at the 112th Convention 2002 May 10 13 Munich, Germany This convention paper has been reproduced from the author's advance manuscript, without editing,

More information

Bachelors of Science Program in Communication Disorders and Sciences:

Bachelors of Science Program in Communication Disorders and Sciences: Bachelors of Science Program in Communication Disorders and Sciences: Mission: The SIUC CDS program is committed to multiple complimentary missions. We provide support for, and align with, the university,

More information

Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus

Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus Brain Maps The Sensory Homunculus Our brains are maps. This mapping results from the way connections in the brain are ordered and arranged. The ordering of neural pathways between different parts of the

More information

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY

RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH METHODS IN I/O PSYCHOLOGY Objectives Understand Empirical Research Cycle Knowledge of Research Methods Conceptual Understanding of Basic Statistics PSYC 353 11A rsch methods 09/01/11 [Arthur]

More information

AP1 Waves. (A) frequency (B) wavelength (C) speed (D) intensity. Answer: (A) and (D) frequency and intensity.

AP1 Waves. (A) frequency (B) wavelength (C) speed (D) intensity. Answer: (A) and (D) frequency and intensity. 1. A fire truck is moving at a fairly high speed, with its siren emitting sound at a specific pitch. As the fire truck recedes from you which of the following characteristics of the sound wave from the

More information

INCREASE YOUR PRODUCTIVITY WITH CELF 4 SOFTWARE! SAMPLE REPORTS. To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.pearsonassess.

INCREASE YOUR PRODUCTIVITY WITH CELF 4 SOFTWARE! SAMPLE REPORTS. To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.pearsonassess. INCREASE YOUR PRODUCTIVITY WITH CELF 4 SOFTWARE! Report Assistant SAMPLE REPORTS To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.pearsonassess.com In Canada, call 1-800-387-7278 In United Kingdom,

More information

Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression

Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression Slide 1 Section 14 Simple Linear Regression: Introduction to Least Squares Regression There are several different measures of statistical association used for understanding the quantitative relationship

More information

Bayesian probability theory

Bayesian probability theory Bayesian probability theory Bruno A. Olshausen arch 1, 2004 Abstract Bayesian probability theory provides a mathematical framework for peforming inference, or reasoning, using probability. The foundations

More information

Charlesworth School Year Group Maths Targets

Charlesworth School Year Group Maths Targets Charlesworth School Year Group Maths Targets Year One Maths Target Sheet Key Statement KS1 Maths Targets (Expected) These skills must be secure to move beyond expected. I can compare, describe and solve

More information

Strategic Online Advertising: Modeling Internet User Behavior with

Strategic Online Advertising: Modeling Internet User Behavior with 2 Strategic Online Advertising: Modeling Internet User Behavior with Patrick Johnston, Nicholas Kristoff, Heather McGinness, Phuong Vu, Nathaniel Wong, Jason Wright with William T. Scherer and Matthew

More information

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention

An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention An Introduction to ERP Studies of Attention Logan Trujillo, Ph.D. Post-Doctoral Fellow University of Texas at Austin Cognitive Science Course, Fall 2008 What is Attention? Everyone knows what attention

More information

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION

PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION PRIMING OF POP-OUT AND CONSCIOUS PERCEPTION Peremen Ziv and Lamy Dominique Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University zivperem@post.tau.ac.il domi@freud.tau.ac.il Abstract Research has demonstrated

More information

Prelinguistic vocal behaviors. Stage 1 (birth-1 month) Stage 2 (2-3 months) Stage 4 (7-9 months) Stage 3 (4-6 months)

Prelinguistic vocal behaviors. Stage 1 (birth-1 month) Stage 2 (2-3 months) Stage 4 (7-9 months) Stage 3 (4-6 months) 3 stages of phonological development 1. Prelinguistic vocal behaviors 2. Phonology of the first 50 words 3. Emergence of rules Prelinguistic vocal behaviors Reflexive/vegetative sounds (birth-1 month)

More information

Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults

Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults Auditory memory and cerebral reorganization in post-linguistically deaf adults Implications for cochlear implantation outcome D Lazard, HJ Lee, E Truy, AL Giraud Ecole Normale Supérieure, Inserm U960,

More information

Grade 5 Math Content 1

Grade 5 Math Content 1 Grade 5 Math Content 1 Number and Operations: Whole Numbers Multiplication and Division In Grade 5, students consolidate their understanding of the computational strategies they use for multiplication.

More information

Speech and Network Marketing Model - A Review

Speech and Network Marketing Model - A Review Jastrzȩbia Góra, 16 th 20 th September 2013 APPLYING DATA MINING CLASSIFICATION TECHNIQUES TO SPEAKER IDENTIFICATION Kinga Sałapa 1,, Agata Trawińska 2 and Irena Roterman-Konieczna 1, 1 Department of Bioinformatics

More information

T-test & factor analysis

T-test & factor analysis Parametric tests T-test & factor analysis Better than non parametric tests Stringent assumptions More strings attached Assumes population distribution of sample is normal Major problem Alternatives Continue

More information

Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance

Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance Chapter VIII Customers Perception Regarding Health Insurance This chapter deals with the analysis of customers perception regarding health insurance and involves its examination at series of stages i.e.

More information

Algebra 1 2008. Academic Content Standards Grade Eight and Grade Nine Ohio. Grade Eight. Number, Number Sense and Operations Standard

Algebra 1 2008. Academic Content Standards Grade Eight and Grade Nine Ohio. Grade Eight. Number, Number Sense and Operations Standard Academic Content Standards Grade Eight and Grade Nine Ohio Algebra 1 2008 Grade Eight STANDARDS Number, Number Sense and Operations Standard Number and Number Systems 1. Use scientific notation to express

More information

ACOUSTIC CHARACTERISTICS OF CLEARLY SPOKEN ENGLISH TENSE AND LAX VOWELS

ACOUSTIC CHARACTERISTICS OF CLEARLY SPOKEN ENGLISH TENSE AND LAX VOWELS ACOUSTIC CHARACTERISTICS OF CLEARLY SPOKEN ENGLISH TENSE AND LAX VOWELS Keith King Wui Leung 1, Allard Jongman 2, Yue Wang 1, Joan A. Sereno 2 1. Language and Brain Lab, Department of Linguistics, Simon

More information

Infant speech perception activates Broca s area: a developmental magnetoencephalography study

Infant speech perception activates Broca s area: a developmental magnetoencephalography study BRAIN IMAGING NEUROREPORT Infant speech perception activates Broca s area: a developmental magnetoencephalography study Toshiaki Imada a,yang Zhang b, Marie Cheour c,d,samutaulu e,anttiahonen e and Patricia

More information

The pitch levels of female speech in two Chinese villages

The pitch levels of female speech in two Chinese villages The pitch levels of female speech in two Chinese villages Diana Deutsch a) Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 ddeutsch@ucsd.edu Jinghong Le School

More information

Phonetic and phonological properties of the final pitch accent in Catalan declaratives

Phonetic and phonological properties of the final pitch accent in Catalan declaratives Abstract Phonetic and phonological properties of the final pitch accent in Catalan declaratives Eva Estebas-Vilaplana * This paper examines the phonetic and phonological properties of the last pitch accent

More information