Sociality, Spirituality, and Meaning Making: Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Sociality, Spirituality, and Meaning Making: Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study"

Transcription

1 Review of General Psychology Copyright 2005 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2005, Vol. 9, No. 2, /05/$12.00 DOI: / Sociality, Spirituality, and Meaning Making: Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study John T. Cacioppo, Louise C. Hawkley, Edith M. Rickett, and Christopher M. Masi University of Chicago Scientific theories in the natural sciences posit invisible forces operating with measurable effects on physical bodies, but the scientific study of invisible forces acting on human bodies has made limited progress. The topics of sociality, spirituality, and meaning making are cases in point. The authors discuss some of the possible reasons for this as well as contemporary developments in the social sciences and neurosciences that may make such study possible and productive. In approximately 600 BCE, the Greek philosopher Heraclitus referred to the mind as an overwhelming space whose boundaries could never be fully comprehended. For the next 2,300 years, little changed in this regard. Indeed, before the enlightenment of the 18th century, scholars generally believed that thought was instantaneous and that action was governed by an indivisible mind separate from the body. As a result of the belief that the mind was infinitely fast and essentially unanalyzable, there was no point in trying to understand it using scientific means. The human spirit encompassing the qualities of kindness, mercy, empathy, trust, compassion, justice, love, friendship, devotion, and hope was championed in art, literature, and religion but was at best ignored by the scientific community. The past three centuries have been a period of unparalleled advance in science. Scientific theories of magnetism, gravity, and dark matter have emerged to posit invisible forces operating with measurable effects on physical bodies. During this same period, the scientific study of invisible forces acting on human bodies has John T. Cacioppo and Louise C. Hawkley, Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive and Social Neuroscience, University of Chicago; Edith M. Rickett, Department of Psychology, University of Chicago; Christopher M. Masi, Department of Medicine and Center for Cognitive and Social Sciences, University of Chicago. This research was supported by National Institute on Aging Grant PO1 AG Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John T. Cacioppo, Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 South University Avenue, Chicago, IL cacioppo@uchicago.edu made limited progress. Research on the dimensions of sociality, spirituality, and meaning making for instance, has been blunted by biases against what were regarded as soft or religious topics, misguided by metaphors such as the human brain as a solitary computer, and overlooked in a funding climate that demands time and attention be given to societal, psychological, and physiological deficits rather than capacities. Although the history and causes of such biases are complex, little is achieved to mitigate these biases when the constructs of sociality, spirituality, and meaning making are defined imprecisely, their predicted effects are not falsifiable or are difficult to replicate, or the underlying mechanisms for such effects are not delineated within a framework that is recognized by the scientific community. The dawn of the 21st century may herald a paradigm shift in constructs deemed amenable to scientific inquiry. With new developments and instruments in genetics, neuroscience, brain science, and behavioral science, long-standing scientific biases are being challenged by rigorous quantitative analysis. Fueling these challenges is the recognition that the traditional focus by founding and federal funding agencies on maladies and disease misses the mark on some of the most complex yet pressing and enduring questions of humankind. The guiding metaphors are also undergoing dramatic transformations. Now that the human genome has been sequenced and has been found to involve fewer genes than anticipated, it has become apparent that humans are not inextricably determined by their genotype irrespective of their social environment. In addition, the ex- 143

2 144 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI tended period of utter dependency of the human infant has led to the recognition that human genetic transmission is based not on an individual s selfish ability to reproduce but on the success of offspring to reproduce (Dawkins, 1976). Accordingly, we have a better appreciation of how humans have evolved to be an inherently social, meaning-making species. Moreover, the notion of the solitary computer the dominant metaphor for the human mind for more than a quarter century suddenly seems dated. Computers today are massively interconnected devices with capacities that extend far beyond the resident hardware and software of a solitary computer. How ironic that although the telereceptors of the human brain have provided wireless broadband interconnectivity to humans for millennia, it took the advent of technological innovations for it to become patently obvious that the isolated computer is a poor metaphor for the human mind. The qualities of kindness, mercy, empathy, trust, compassion, justice, love, friendship, devotion, and hope qualities attributed in art, literature, and religion to the human spirit did not comport well with the dominant metaphors of the mind in the latter part of the 20th century. Today, these same qualities are simple to incorporate into a metaphor of the mind as a massively networked portable computer, given the recognition that human genetic transmission relies in part on cooperation and nurturance. Even useful metaphors have limited utility, however, if the constructs under study are poorly defined. In our study of sociality, spirituality, and meaning making, we define sociality as the need for the company of others, measurable as the tendency to form social connections, to react to perceived social isolation, and to exhibit the long-term effects of social connectedness on health, successful aging, and well-being. By spirituality we mean a theoretical construct that represents what is common in human qualities such as kindness, mercy, virtue, empathy, trust, compassion, justice, love, friendship, devotion, and hope. Accordingly, sociality may underlie spirituality in at least two senses. As we explain subsequently, we posit that the qualities ascribed in art, literature, and religion to the human spirit evolved because humans are fundamentally a social species. Human virtues are social virtues, potentially lending spirituality and sociality a common substrate. In addition, sociality may serve as a model for spirituality, as when people form a personal relationship with a deity. As in the case of sociality, the nature and existence of the deity with whom a person forms a relationship is less important than the person s mental representations of the deity and relationship to the deity. The measurable effects of spirituality also mirror those for sociality, including the tendency to form a mental representation that relates the self to a deity; to exhibit the long-term effects of such a relationship on health, effective aging, and wellbeing; and to react to perceived separation from or loss of worth in the eyes of the deity with behavioral, physiological, and emotional responses. Spirituality and sociality overlap in measurable ways such as religious affiliation, church attendance, and church participation; accordingly, these variables are also investigated. Central to sociality and spirituality is meaning making, defined as the construction of an account or recital of an event or a series of events, either true or fictitious, that serves to organize or structure life. Our definition of meaning making is compatible with current theory in perception and cognition, in which even visual percepts are generated according to a wholly empirical strategy that signifies to the individual the empirical significance of the stimulus rather than its properties as such (Purves, Lotto, & Nundy, 2002). We begin with a brief evolutionary argument for the importance of sociality in the survival of the human species. We then describe a neuroscientific approach to the study of molar constructs such as sociality, spirituality, and meaning making, and we review recent findings from our program of research on social isolation and loneliness. Sociality, Spirituality, and Meaning Making as Components of Human Nature The genetic constitution of Homo sapiens derives not simply from an individual s reproductive success but more critically from the success of one s children to reproduce. The human infant is born to an extended period of complete dependency. If infants do not elicit nurturance and protection from caregivers, or if caregivers are not motivated to provide such care over an extended period of time, the infants perish along with the genetic legacy of the parents. Hunter/gatherers who, in times of danger

3 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 145 or famine, chose not to return to share their food with mother and child may have survived to hunt another day, but the genetic constitution that enabled them to feel so little humanity also made it less likely their genes were propagated. In contrast, those who yearned to return despite personal hardship, and individuals who protected and nurtured those close to them, were more likely to have offspring who survived to propagate. Even as adults, humans are not particularly stealthy, strong, or fast relative to other species. It is their collective action their ability to think, communicate, and work together that makes Homo sapiens such a formidable species. Because human collective action provides an evolutionary advantage over other species and because genetic transmission is based not on one s ability to reproduce but on the success of one s children to reproduce, Homo sapiens are thought to have evolved to be an inherently social, meaning-making species with qualities ascribed in art, literature, and religion to a human spirit. In short, we posit that tens of thousands of years of evolution have deeply planted sociality, spirituality, and meaning making in our genome and in our societies. Accordingly, humans are posited to have evolved a brain and biology whose functioning benefits from the formation and maintenance of social bonds, human/social virtues, and organizational life narratives. The deprivation of any of these ingredients such as ruptures of social connectedness that result from relocation distant from friends and family produces feelings of isolation and dysphoria, physiological alterations, and a motivation to reinstate connections. Given the evolutionary origins of these effects, there should also be heritable individual differences in the extent to which social disconnectedness produces such effects. The extant evidence clearly shows that humans quickly learn to attend to faces, perceive communicative displays, comprehend social hierarchies, and form causal attributions (see Cacioppo & Berntson, 2004). People develop a theory of mind by which the traits, intentions, and emotions of others are inferred; they communicate with others and they sometimes hide or miscommunicate their own mental contents from others; they form relationships, unions, and alliances; and they search for meaning in events and patterns. Meaning making and sociality are such fundamental components of human nature that people perceive these characteristics in the movements of simple inanimate objects. Heider and Simmel (1944), for instance, produced a short film of the movement of a small triangle, a small circle, and a large triangle around and into a large rectangle. The animated film consisted only of these geometric shapes, yet everyone who viewed the film saw a social drama complete with intentions, plans, and emotions. Only SM, an individual without functioning amygdala an almondshaped pair of nuclei deep in the medial temporal lobes of the brain failed to perceive these movements as occurring within a social arena (Adolphs, 1999). Given our evolutionary history, we would contend that people s conceptual representation of sociality has a specifiable, generalizable, and possibly universal structure (Hawkley, Browne, & Cacioppo, 2004). In the first study underlying this claim, 2,531 undergraduates completed a scale consisting of 20 questions that differentiated people who perceived themselves to be socially isolated from those who perceived themselves to be socially integrated (Hawkley et al., 2004). An exploratory factor analysis on half of the sample revealed three correlated dimensions generalizable across gender, and a confirmatory factor analysis on the other half of the sample corroborated this finding: The college students beliefs, feelings, and ideas about their social connectedness were structured as three separable but related (oblique) dimensions: isolation, relational connectedness, and collective connectedness. In a population-based follow-up study, we tested a very different sample of individuals, a statistically representative sample of 230 men and women (one third African American, one third Hispanic, one third Caucasian) from Cook County, Illinois, born between 1935 and 1952 (M age 57.5 years; Hawkley et al., 2004). Despite the fact that this sample was an older urban sample of varying ethnic backgrounds, socioeconomic status (SES), and occupations, the confirmatory factor analysis supported the same three-factor structure found for the college students. Their feelings of social disconnectedness consisted of three related factors: feelings of isolation/intimate connectedness, feelings of relational connectedness, and feelings of collective connectedness. Importantly, we also found statistically separable predictors of each of these facets of soci-

4 146 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI ality. In our urban sample of older adults, we found marital status to predict feelings of isolation, contact with friends and family to predict feelings of relational connectedness, and membership in voluntary groups to predict collective connectedness (Hawkley et al., 2004). Tests of the universality of this structure (isolation, relational connections, collective connections) are needed, but it is worth noting that psychological theories of the self, which traditionally have focused on a person s sense of unique identity differentiated from others, now distinguish among the personal self (individual level of analysis), relational self (interpersonal level of analysis), and collective self (group level of analysis; Brewer & Gardner, 1996). Individuals who score low on personal, relational, and collective dimensions of sociality also perceive themselves to be social isolates and report intense feelings of loneliness and dysphoria (Cacioppo, Ernst, et al., 2000; Cacioppo et al., in press; Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980). Individual differences in the net content of this structure are about 50% heritable and 50% environmental (Boomsma, Willemsen, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004; S. McGuire & Clifford, 2000). In an early study of the origins of these feelings, S. McGuire and Clifford (2000) examined the heritability of loneliness in children. In their first study, 69 biologically related sibling pairs and 64 unrelated pairs in adoptive families in the Colorado Adoption Project completed an 8-item loneliness scale when they were 9, 10, 11, and 12 years of age. In a second study, 22 monozygotic twins, 40 dizygotic twins, and 80 full siblings 8 14 years of age completed a 16-item scale to assess loneliness in relation to their schoolmates. Results revealed significant genetic (h 2 values of 55% and 48%, respectively, in Studies 1 and 2) and unshared environmental contributions to individual differences in loneliness. In the S. McGuire and Clifford (2000) studies, the sample sizes were relatively small, and because the studies involved adopted children, the representativeness of the sample for a population estimate is uncertain. Moreover, heritability estimates of complex traits such as loneliness may change across the life span as the frequency, duration, and range of exposure to environmental influences accrue. We therefore extended this work using data from the Netherlands Twin Register Study (Boomsma, Cacioppo et al., 2004). Data on loneliness from 7,665 young adult and adult Dutch twins (average age: 24 years) were analyzed with genetic structural equation models. The estimate of genetic contributions to variation in loneliness in adults was 47%, with the remaining variance explained by unique environmental factors. Thus, the heritability estimates in adults were similar to those found previously in children, and no evidence was found for sex or age differences in genetic architecture or configural effects of the genes. In an ongoing effort to more specifically identify the genetic locus, Boomsma, Willemsen, Dolan, Hawkley, and Cacioppo (in press) conducted a complete genome scan and found evidence for two quantitative trait loci, suggesting that at least two sets of genes with additive effects are involved. The striking development of the human cerebral cortex, especially the frontal and temporal regions, is believed to be largely responsible for evolutionary advances in social and cognitive capacities. The cerebral cortex is a mantle of between 2.6 and 16 billion neurons, with each neuron receiving 10,000 to 100,000 connections from other neurons (e.g., Pakkenberg, 1966). The human frontal (front part of the brain) and temporal (right and left sides) lobes constitute 32% and 23% of the cerebral cortex, respectively, arguably rendering the sensorimotor cortices that dominate most mammalian brains to minority status in the human brain. The expansion of the frontal regions in the human brain is largely responsible for the human capacity for reasoning, planning, and performing mental simulations, and an intact frontal region contributes to the human ability to reason, remember, and work together, thereby contributing to the evolutionary success of humans (Bar-On, Tranel, Denburg, & Bechara, 2003; Kringelbach & Rolls, 2004). The temporal and parietal regions, in turn, play essential roles in social perception, social reasoning, and communication (cf. Adolphs, 1999, 2001; Berntson, Boysen, & Cacioppo, 1993; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003). Despite these evolutionary advances, human cognition and meaning making are often biased and counterfactual. The sensory load from the physical environment is minor in comparison with the quantity and complexity of the information that comes from other individuals, groups, alliances, and cultures, including the potential for benevolence or treachery posed by each. Human cognition is not an objective in-

5 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 147 formation process but instead is rife with the operation of self-interest, self-enhancement, and self-protective processes. Because humans encounter more information than can possibly be processed, they tend to economize on thought when forming beliefs that are not personally relevant (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) and tend to search for and attend to evidence that confirms what they already believe to be true (Snyder & Swann, 1978). Information processing is also biased in ways that protect the self from symbolic as well as actual threats and that promote reproductive success (e.g., Jones & Berglas, 1978). In fact, people are not particularly good at knowing the causes of their feelings or behavior (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). People overestimate their strengths and underestimate their faults (M. Ross & Sicoly, 1979). They overestimate the importance of their input, the pervasiveness of their beliefs, and the likelihood that a desired event will occur (W. J. McGuire, 1981), all while underestimating the contributions of others and the likelihood that risks in the world apply to them (L. Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Events that unfold unexpectedly are not reasoned as much as they are rationalized (Aronson, 1968), and the act of remembering is far more of a biased reconstruction than an accurate record of actual events (McDonald & Hirt, 1997; Roediger, Buckner, & McDermott, 1999). Many of these biases in social cognition are spontaneous, do not require cognitive effort, and represent normative processing. A crucial consequence of the nuances of the biased fashion in which humans make meaning is that humans have much more influence in the creation of their lives and social relationships than most realize (Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 1996). If an individual is led to believe a new acquaintance is fun and nice, for instance, the individual behaves in a fashion that draws out pleasant and enjoyable behaviors from the person. If an individual is led to think a child is intelligent, the individual does and says things that make a smarter child than would result if the individual was led to believe the child was of average intelligence. Another instance of biased meaning making is the tendency of people to self-handicap when they think they will fail at an important task. By subtly producing insurmountable obstacles to success, they can attribute their subsequent failure to these obstacles rather than to themselves. Importantly in the present context, bias in social cognition is also at work when people who feel socially connected construe the world as presenting challenges to be overcome with the aid of others and react to interpersonal conflicts in peaceful and constructive rather than offensive and aggressive ways, thereby producing an environment that others want to inhabit (Cacioppo & Hawkley, in press). In contrast, lonely individuals are more likely to construe their world (including the behavior of others) as potentially threatening or punitive. Consequently, lonely individuals are more likely to be socially anxious, hold more negative expectations for their treatment by others, and adopt a prevention focus rather than a promotion focus in their social interactions. These individuals are also more likely to appraise stressors as threats rather than challenges and to cope with stressors in a passive, isolative fashion rather than an active fashion that includes actively seeking the help and support of others. Together, these differences in social cognition predictably result in an increased likelihood of lonely individuals acting in self-protective and, paradoxically, self-defeating ways (Cacioppo & Hawkley, in press). In each instance, the individuals may be oblivious to the fact that the way in which they perceived and thought about their social world contributed to their social realities. We noted at the outset that biases against the scientific study of constructs such as sociality, spirituality, and meaning making are attributable to imprecise definitions, nonfalsifiable or nonreplicable effects, a focus on associations rather than underlying mechanisms, and the absence of a broad scientific framework within which to study abstract constructs of this sort. We next address the latter issue. Specifically, we argue that a neuroscience perspective may provide a framework that allows theoretical constructs and associations to be rigorously defined, tested, and parsed so as to investigate their underlying mechanisms. Social Neuroscience Perspective During the latter half of the 20th century, the nature of the human mind was plumbed through clever experimental designs that used measures of verbal reports, judgments, and reaction time. These methodologies proved limited, however.

6 148 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI Social cognition and interactions range from affect laden to habitual, and nuances deriving from these features may prove difficult to capture fully using subjective measures and response latencies to semantic (e.g., lexical decision) tasks alone (LeDoux, 2000; Zajonc, 1980). A new approach, termed social neuroscience, first introduced just over a decade ago (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1992), represents a new development in the study of the human mind, including how sociality, spirituality, and the meaning of life might modulate brain and biology. Human nature is recognized as being complex. To simplify the study of human nature, neuroscientists in the past century tended to ignore or hold constant social influences, whereas cognitive and social scientists tended to ignore the biological constraints on and mechanisms through which cognition, affect, and conation are expressed. In the neurosciences, the architects of development and behavior were conceived as anatomical structures and genetic strings sculpted by the forces of evolution operating over millennia, the builders were cast as encapsulated within living cells far from the reach of social influences, and the brain was treated as an analytical informationprocessing machine. The additional information that might be attributable to the social world was thought to be best considered last, if the need arose. Social factors, the reasoning often went, had minimal implications for basic development, structure, or processes of the brain or mind, in which case the consideration of social factors is entirely irrelevant. And even if relevant, consideration of social factors may render the study of the human mind and behavior too complicated to sustain scientific progress. The attitude toward the neurosciences among cognitive and social scientists throughout most of the 20th century was no less antagonistic (Berntson & Cacioppo, 2000; Cacioppo, 2002). World wars, the Great Depression, and civil injustices made it amply clear that social and cultural forces were too important to address to await the full explication of cellular and molecular mechanisms. Biological constraints, mechanisms, and insights tended to be ignored, often under the misguided auspice of protecting the behavioral sciences from the onslaught of reductionism. The specialized knowledge and fundamental research that were required to cultivate descriptive taxonomies, theoretical formulations, and methodologies coupled with an early emphasis on isolated scientific work all but ensured that social and biological perspectives would evolve insulated from developments in the other. For decades, the central precept of molecular biology was that all of the information we need to construct a mammalian body, whether man or mouse, is contained in the approximately hundred thousand genes of mammalian DNA and that a set of master genes activates the DNA necessary to produce the appropriate proteins for development and behavior (Crick, 1970; Panksepp, 1998). In this scheme, DNA encodes the sequence of amino acids in proteins and peptides using the sequence of nucleotides in the gene as a template. The DNA sequential code is transferred to messenger RNA (mrna) via transcription, a process involving enzymes (RNA polymerases), followed by translation from mrna to polypeptide chains (protein pieces) and proteins. The process of DNA to RNA transcription has been assumed to be restricted to the confines of living cells outside the influence of personal and social ties. As neuroscientific approaches have been applied to more complex questions, however, these presumably basic principles have begun to be questioned. Recent research suggests that even DNA to RNA transcription can be subject to modulation by the social environment (Cacioppo, Berntson, Sheridan, & McClintock, 2000). In an illustrative study, we investigated the DNA to RNA transcription for a growth hormone that occurs within a type of immune cell called a lymphocyte. The production of this growth hormone is of interest because it is thought to be involved in the effectiveness of lymphocytes to combat pathogens. We recently found that caregivers of spouses with Alzheimer s disease (AD) had markedly suppressed lymphocyte growth hormone mrna levels relative to age- and gender-matched controls (Wu et al., 1999). It is reasonable to assume that the spouses of AD patients were essentially randomly assigned to caregiver or noncaregiver roles by their spouse s unexpected development of AD, in which case these results indicate that social roles can modulate DNA to RNA transcription processes. The behavior of strains of mice with specific genes inactivated (i.e., knockout mice) has been known to depend on genetic background, whereas the effects of the social context have

7 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 149 been thought to be unimportant. Contrary to this latter belief, however, Crabbe, Wahlsten, and Dudek (1999) found that the specific behavioral effects associated with a given knockout could vary dramatically across experimenters, testing environments, and laboratories. The implication of these and related studies is that aspects of genetic expression, which were thought to be encapsulated within each living cell far from the reach of personal ties or social influences, are in fact subject to modulation by the social environment. In the twilight of the 20th century, neuroscientists and cognitive scientists began to collaborate more systematically, united by the common view that information processing could best be understood by appeal to the brain as well as its emergent manifestation in mind and by the goal of understanding how the mind works. These collaborations have helped unravel puzzles of the mind including aspects of perception, imagery, attention, and memory. Many aspects of human nature require a more comprehensive approach, however. These include aspects of the human spirit such as sociality, altruism, affiliation, attachment, kin recognition, social identification, communication, cooperation, commerce, empathy, morality, contagion, love, nurturance, kindness, mercy, compassion, justice, friendship, and hope. All can be conceptualized as invisible forces emanating from the operation of the human brain within a social arena with measurable effects not only on subjective wellbeing but on brain, biology, and health. Brain imaging studies clearly point to the attention given to social stimuli. If an individual views a novel picture of an evocative image (e.g., snowcapped mountains) or an equally pleasant, arousing, and statistically infrequent image of a person (e.g., a smiling baby), the social stimulus elicits a larger amplitude late positive electrocortical (event-related brain) potential that peaks about 550 ms after stimulus onset (Ito & Cacioppo, 2000). In addition, the differential event-related response emerges even when the task is simply to classify whether the picture is pleasant or unpleasant. That is, the brain is spontaneously extracting information about the presence of conspecifics even when the explicit task has nothing to do with this distinction (Ito & Cacioppo, 2000). Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have similarly suggested that social perception and social reasoning are robust elicitors of localized regions of brain activation and reflect distinct aspects of social and emotional processing (e.g., see Blakemore, Winston, & Frith, 2004; Cacioppo & Berntson, 2004). The field of social neuroscience, of course, stretches far beyond social cognition, human studies, or brain imaging methodologies (see Cacioppo & Berntson, in press-b; Cacioppo, Berntson, et al., 2002). As the 21st century dawns, there is a recognition that much of the groundwork for multidisciplinary scientific collaborations has been laid by the giants of the preceding three centuries. Neuroscientists, cognitive scientists, and social scientists are moving beyond simplifying assumptions and are placing less emphasis on the arbitrary division between the social and the biological sciences to work collaboratively toward developing more comprehensive theories of mind, brain, biology, and behavior. Through the efforts of such individuals, the broad multidisciplinary perspective of social neuroscience is gaining momentum, making more feasible the rigorous scientific study of questions such as how sociality, spirituality, and the meaning of life operate. Throughout this article, we have described such examples from our own research, ranging from behavioral genetics to brain imaging. In the remainder of the article, we survey results from our multidisciplinary research to examine the mechanisms underlying the association between social connectedness and longevity and well-being. Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study In 2002, we began a population-based longitudinal study of 230 English-speaking Blacks/ African Americans, non-black Hispanics, and non-hispanic Caucasians between the ages of 50 and 67 years from Cook County, Illinois, to investigate the social, behavioral, cognitive, emotional, brain, autonomic, neuroendocrine, cellular, and molecular transduction pathways by which the social world affects well-being and health (e.g., Cacioppo & Berntson, in press-a, in press-b; Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2003; Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Berntson, 2003). The Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study (CHASRS) was built on our previous research on thousands of young adults (Cacioppo, Ernst, et al., 2000; Cacioppo, Hawkley, Berntson, et al., 2002; Cacioppo, Hawkley,

8 150 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI Crawford, et al., 2002; Hawkley et al., 2004) and older adults (Cacioppo et al., 1998; Uchino, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Cacioppo, 1994) and on meta-analyses of the literature (e.g., Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). Among our findings from this earlier work was that socially connected individuals are more likely to meet everyday stressors by active coping and recruiting others help. Individuals who feel socially isolated are more likely to construe their world (including the behavior of others) as threatening or punitive. They are more likely to appraise stressors as threats rather than challenges and to cope with stressors in a passive fashion by isolating themselves from others and withdrawing from the problem situation. Together, these differences in meaning making result in an increased likelihood of socially connected individuals acting in a humane, selfless fashion, reinforcing their connections with others and enhancing their self-esteem, and of lonely individuals acting in selfprotective and, paradoxically, socially acidic and self-defeating ways (Cacioppo, Ernst, et al., 2000; Cacioppo & Hawkley, in press; Hawkley, Burleson, Berntson, & Cacioppo, 2003). Why might lonely individuals be more emotionally withdrawn in social settings? Personality differences such as shyness, sociability, negativity, and fear of negative evaluation may provide a partial explanation. In addition, because social settings can be overwhelming, social effectiveness depends on an individual s ability to exert voluntary control over his or her attentional focus. Moreover, regulating one s attention can help one garner social approval. Do lonely individuals differ in their ability to voluntarily control their attentional focus? As a means of exploring self-regulation, participants in our study of loneliness in young adults performed a dichotic listening task while at the clinical research center. The dichotic listening task required that participants identify the consonant vowel pair presented to their right or left ear. Because the auditory system is predominantly crossed and because language is leftlateralized in most right-handed individuals, right-handed individuals tend to perform better when verbal stimuli are presented to the right than left ear. (All of the participants in this study were right-handed.) Superimposed on this general right-ear advantage, however, are the effects of attention, as individuals generally perform better when verbal stimuli are presented to the ear to which they are focusing their attention. The former effect is said to be data driven or bottom-up, whereas the latter is said to be conceptually driven or top-down. As predicted, we found an overall right-ear advantage across loneliness groups (lonely, normal, and nonlonely) and instructional conditions (Cacioppo, Ernst, et al., 2000). In addition, however, a significant main effect of attentional instruction showed that individuals performed better with left-ear stimuli when they were instructed to focus on the stimuli presented to their left ear than in the other conditions. Moreover, lonely individuals tended to show the strongest right-ear advantage in the no-instruction condition, presumably reflecting the potency of bottom-up (stimulus-driven) attentional processing, but failed to shift to the leftear advantage when instructed to focus on their left ear. Specific planned contrasts confirmed that all three groups showed a significant rightear advantage during the focus on right-ear condition, but only the normal and nonlonely individuals were able to shift to a significant left-ear advantage in the focus on left-ear condition. Together, these results are consistent with the notion that attentional control appears comparable in lonely and nonlonely individuals until voluntary attentional control conflicts with automatic attentional processes, at which point lonely individuals show an attentional deficit. This result raises the possibility that lonely individuals may feel threatened or overwhelmed and therefore withdraw from the social environment, especially new or complex social environments, because they have less control over the focus of their attention than nonlonely individuals. In fact, Baumeister and colleagues recently found that experimental manipulations of social exclusion could produce a similar effect on dichotic listening and self-regulation (Baumeister & DeWall, in press). A measure of the importance of social connectedness is that it predicts morbidity and mortality from broad-based causes (e.g., Seeman, 2000). The reasons for this effect remain unclear. As noted earlier, few differences in traditional health behaviors (e.g., smoking, exercise, or nutrition) have been found to differentiate lonely and nonlonely individuals, for instance (Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford, et al., 2002; Seeman, 2000). We therefore have explored other possible mechanisms by which loneliness may have deleterious effects on health: health

9 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 151 behaviors, cardiovascular activation, cortisol levels, and sleep (Cacioppo et al., 2003). In one study, we assessed autonomic activity, salivary cortisol levels, sleep quality, and health behaviors in undergraduate students selected, on the basis of pretests, to be among the top or bottom quintile in feelings of loneliness (Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford, et al., 2002). We found that the total peripheral resistance to blood flow through the circulatory system in the body was higher in lonely than nonlonely participants, whereas cardiac contractility (the force of a heartbeat), heart rate, and cardiac output were higher in nonlonely than lonely participants. Such differences, although not detrimental in the robust cardiovascular system of young adults, may constitute a source of wear and tear on the vascular system and on regulatory controls of blood pressure, with cardiovascular dysfunctions not appearing until later in life. We also found that lonely individuals reported poorer sleep than nonlonely individuals. Differences in sleep efficiency in young adults, as with differences in cardiovascular function, may have minimal health consequences in the short term but may influence significant health outcomes over time, especially in later life when physiological functions become more fragile. In a follow-up study, we assessed blood pressure, heart rate, salivary cortisol levels, sleep quality, and health behaviors in older adults whose loneliness was assessed at the time of testing at their residence (Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford, et al., 2002). Results indicated greater age-related increases in blood pressure and poorer sleep quality in lonely than nonlonely older adults. These results suggest that the stress and dysphoria associated with feeling isolated contribute to wear and tear on the body and, over time, to the degradation of central and peripheral regulatory systems. Humans are not static mechanical devices that simply wear out, but instead, human physiology includes anabolic processes that promote repair and maintenance (e.g., wound healing) and growth (e.g., muscular development) in response to stressors. Sleep is the quintessential restorative behavior, however, and sleep involves no obvious social interaction. We therefore next asked whether the restorative power of sleep was greater for people who felt low rather than high levels of loneliness (Cacioppo, Hawkley, Berntson, et al., 2002). All participants were tested one night in the clinical research center of an academic hospital and several additional nights in their residence. Results revealed that lonely and nonlonely individuals allocated the same amount of time in bed for sleep, but lonely individuals evinced poorer sleep efficiency and more time awake after sleep onset than nonlonely individuals. These results suggest that lonely individuals may be less resilient than nonlonely individuals in part because they sleep more poorly. These results also raise the possibility that feelings of social connectedness may influence the extent to which wear and tear on the brain and body can be slowed or reversed by inherent anabolic processes. That is, sociality not only may influence the selection of health behaviors but may modulate the salubrity of restorative behaviors. We designed the CHASRS to include detailed medical histories, health assessments, measures of health care use, health behavior measures (both self-report and objective), sleep quality indexes, personality measures (e.g., Big Five), life event assessments, nutrition and exercise measures, markers of metabolic and inflammatory processes, endocrine and immune assays, baseline autonomic (especially cardiovascular) assessments and tests, tests of memory and cognitive function, exposure and reactivity to stressors, mood, volunteerism, optimism, hopefulness, church attendance, and religiosity. Finally, economic and sociological components (e.g., neighborhood characteristics, education, income, and social networks) have been incorporated into the design of the CHASRS. We have used these data, for instance, to examine the social, behavioral, cognitive, and emotional pathways through which neighborhoods affect health and well-being. Using data from the CHASRS, we recently found that neighborhood contexts predict self-reported health in older adulthood even after controlling for age and gender (Wen, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004). More important, we found that the impact of neighborhood SES (e.g., education and income) on health was mediated in our population-based study through the subjective perception (meaning making) of neighborhood. The data further showed that these neighborhood effects on health were mediated independently by individual SES, loneliness/connectedness, and depression but not by size of social networks or perceived support.

10 152 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI As a complement to the Chicago populationbased longitudinal study and in collaboration with Linda Waite and M. E. Hughes, a threeitem measure of loneliness was included in the 2002 wave of the Health and Retirement Study, a longitudinal study of the later life course in a nationally representative sample of individuals born in 1947 or earlier (e.g., Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004). At each wave (interview), detailed information is collected about the respondent s health, family relationships, employment, income and wealth, and demographic background. A nationally representative sample of 2,182 individuals completed the module administered as part of the CHASRS. The three-item survey scale of loneliness was found to have adequate internal consistency and to correlate highly with the full revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al., 2004). Analyses of the Health and Retirement Study and the Chicago population-based sample further indicated that objective social isolation and loneliness are related and that loneliness in both samples was similarly associated with poorer self-reported health, poorer self-reported emotional health, and a greater number of chronic health conditions. As noted earlier, the hypothesis that the health behaviors of patients account for higher levels of morbidity and mortality among lonely or isolated individuals has received weak support. Care for patients has come under scrutiny in the past decade because studies have shown that health care in the United States falls short of basic quality standards and that health care provision is influenced subtly by various extraneous factors such as gender, race, and age. We reasoned that if the health care provided to patients who appear to be socially isolated is of lower quality than that provided to patients with family and friends, then at least part of the explanation for the morbidity/mortality relationship may be attributable to the decisions and behaviors of health care providers rather than to the health behaviors of the patients themselves (Cacioppo, Brown, & Hawkley, 2004). As an initial exploration of this hypothesis, we conducted a national study, randomly sampling 600 physicians whose practice centers on the treatment of older adults. The physicians were asked a series of questions comparing the care of hospital inpatients with constructive and involved family members or friends and inpatients who were socially isolated, along with another series of questions comparing the care of inpatients with difficult or hostile family members or friends and inpatients who were socially isolated. In each series of questions, the physicians indicated the health care they observed to be provided by nurses, doctors, ancillary staff (e.g., physical therapists and nurses aides), and themselves. To allow the same survey question to be used for physicians from different medical specialties and to address the care provided by various health care agents (doctors, nurses, and ancillary staff), respondents were asked simply to indicate who received better care. Finally, we assessed attributes of the physicians such as medical specialty, region of the country in which they practiced, years of experience, and type of medical facility (e.g., hospital) to determine whether these factors moderated physicians beliefs about the effects of their patients social milieu on health care provision. Survey results indicated that physicians reported that they and other physicians, nurses, and ancillary staff provide better medical care to inpatients with constructive family and friends than inpatients without family or friends, whereas inpatients with hostile family and friends receive treatment comparable to that received by socially isolated inpatients. These findings held for outpatients as well as inpatients and regardless of the physician s medical specialty, type of hospital in which the physician practiced, years of experience, and region of the country. These results indicate that social isolation may be associated with broad-based morbidity and mortality at least in part because the care provided by physicians and health care professionals differs. Results from the CHASRS also confirmed that sociality has profound effects on physiological functioning and sleep. As in our earlier studies, we have observed that lonely older adults exhibit evidence of chronically elevated sympathetic activation (e.g., higher overnight urinary epinephrine and resting blood pressure), greater arterial stiffness, and less efficient or effective sleep. These results suggest that social isolation and loneliness have the potential to simultaneously increase physiological load via sympathetic activation and decrease the capacity to recover from that load via reduced quality of sleep.

11 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 153 Conclusion Although the ancestral heritage of Homo sapiens has placed an emphasis on sociality, Western thought and culture value individual excellence over collective achievement. The evidence we have reviewed suggests our ancestral heritage is not easily ignored even if its effects are not immediately obvious. Specifically, we have argued that the human brain has evolved to facilitate social information processing and action and that human contact and nurturance are necessary for normal brain development and function. Loneliness is in part heritable, with individual differences in the level of social connectedness required to feel right. Feelings of social connectedness, or the lack thereof (i.e., loneliness), were further found to be represented conceptually as three facets of a single overarching construct, the facets being isolation/intimate connectedness, relational connectedness, and collective connectedness. These facets are correlated but are nevertheless related distinctively and predictably to life circumstances. In addition, data from the CHASRS indicate that an individual s self-esteem and sense of purpose in life reflect the individual s perceived intimate, relational, and collective connectedness. Dysphoria/depression, too, has been found to be a distinct but related theoretical construct commonly triggered by social rejection or disturbances (Cacioppo et al., in press). How we think about people in everyday life may be profoundly affected by feelings of social connectedness as well. For instance, lonely individuals are more likely than nonlonely individuals to construe their world, including the behavior of others, as threatening or punitive. Consequently, lonely individuals are more likely to be socially anxious, hold more negative expectations for their treatment by others, and adopt a prevention focus rather than a promotion focus in their social interactions. Lonely, relative to nonlonely, individuals are also more likely to appraise stressors as threats rather than challenges and to cope with stressors in a passive, isolative fashion rather than an active fashion that includes actively seeking the help and support of others. Together, these differences in social cognition result predictably in an increased likelihood of lonely individuals acting in self-protective and, paradoxically, self-defeating ways. These dispositions, in turn, activate social neurobehavioral mechanisms that may contribute to the association between loneliness and mortality. In summary, we would argue that it is time to move beyond the solitary computer as a metaphor for the human mind. Computers today are massively interconnected devices with capacities that extend far beyond the resident hardware and software of a solitary computer. In this development, contemporary computers are becoming a better metaphor for the human mind, as the telereceptors of the brain have provided wireless broadband interconnectivity to humans for millennia. References Adolphs, R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 11, Aronson, E. (1968). Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In R. P. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M. Newcomb, M. J. Rosenberg, & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook (pp. 5 27). Chicago: Rand McNally. Bar-On, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N. L., & Bechara, A. (2003). Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence. Brain, 126, Baumeister, R. F., & DeWall, C. N. (in press). The inner dimension of social exclusion: Intelligent thought and self-regulation among rejected persons. In K. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Sydney Social Psychology Symposium. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Berntson, G. G., Boysen, S. T., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1993). Neurobehavioral organization and the cardinal principle of evaluative bivalence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 702, Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). Psychobiology and social psychology: Past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, Blakemore, S., Winston, J., & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience: Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, Boomsma, D. I., Willemsen, G., Dolan, C. V., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (in press). Genetic and environmental contributions to loneliness in adults: The Netherlands Twin Register Study. Behavior Genetics. Manuscript submitted for publication. Boomsma, D. I., Willemsen, G., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Genetic and environmental

12 154 CACIOPPO, HAWKLEY, RICKETT, AND MASI contributions to loneliness in adults: The Netherlands Twin Register Study. Manuscript submitted for publication. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Who is this we? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). The risks and rewards of irrational thinking. University of Chicago Record, 37(3), Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1992). Social psychological contributions to the decade of the brain: Doctrine of multilevel analysis. American Psychologist, 47, Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (2004). Key readings in social neuroscience. New York: Psychology Press. Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (in press-a). The brain, homeostasis, and health: Balancing demands of the internal and external milieu. In H. S. Friedman & R. Cohen Silver (Eds.), Oxford handbook of health psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Cacioppo, J. T. & Berntson, G. G. (in press-b). Essays in social neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Adolphs, R., Carter, C. S., Davidson, R. J., McClintock, M. K., et al. (2002). Foundations in social neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Malarkey, W. B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Sheridan, J., Poehlmann, K. M., et al. (1998). Automatic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to psychological stress: The reactivity hypothesis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., & McClintock, M. K. (2000). Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: Social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psychological Bulletin, 126, Cacioppo, J. T., Brown, K., & Hawkley, L. C. (2004). Physicians reports of medical care as a function of their patients social isolation. Manuscript submitted for publication. Cacioppo, J. T., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Mc- Clintock, M. K., Malarkey, W. B., Hawkley, L. C., et al. (2000). Lonely traits and concomitant physiological processes: The MacArthur social neuroscience studies. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2003). Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 46, S39 S52. Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (in press). People thinking about people: The vicious cycle of being a social outcast in one s own mind. In K. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying. New York: Psychology Press. Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Berntson, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Berntson, G. G., Ernst, J. M., Gibbs, A. C., Stickgold, R., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). Lonely days invade the nights: Social modulation of sleep efficiency. Psychological Science, 13, Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski, R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M., Bernston, G. G., Nouriani, B. & Spiegel, D. (in press). Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Research in Personality. Crabbe, J. C., Wahlsten, D., & Dudek, B. C. (1999, June 4). Genetics of mouse behavior: Interactions with laboratory environment. Science, 284, Crick, F. (1970). Central dogma of molecular biology. Nature, 227, Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press. Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, Hawkley, L. C., Browne, M. W., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Dimensions of loneliness: Perceived social isolation, relational connectedness, and collective connectedness. Manuscript submitted for publication. Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A 3-item version of the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale for use in telephone surveys: Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study (CHASRS). Manuscript submitted for publication. Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence of implicit and explicit categorization processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-handicapping

13 SPECIAL ISSUE: SOCIALITY, SPIRITUALITY, AND MEANING MAKING 155 strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, LeDoux, J. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, McDonald, H. E., & Hirt, E. R. (1997). When expectancy meets desire: Motivational effects in reconstructive memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, McGuire, S., & Clifford, J. (2000). Genetic and environmental contributions to loneliness in children. Psychological Science, 11, McGuire, W. J. (1981). The probabilogical model of cognitive structure and attitude change. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but prescient. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, Pakkenberg, H. (1966). The number of nerve cells in the cerebral cortex of man. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 128, Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, Purves, D., Lotto, R. B., & Nundy, S. (2002). Why we see what we do. American Scientist, 90, Roediger, H. L., Buckner, R. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Components of processing. In J. K. Foster & M. Jelicic (Eds.), Memory: Systems, process, or function? (pp ). New York: Oxford University Press. Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect : An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1979). Egocentric biases in availability and attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporoparietal junction in theory of mind. NeuroIMage, 19, Seeman, T. E. (2000). Health promoting effects of friends and family on health outcomes in older adults. American Journal of Health Promotion, 14, Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1978). Hypothesistesting processes in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, Uchino, B. N., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Construals of preillness relationship quality predict cardiovascular response in family caregivers of Alzheimer s disease victims. Psychology and Aging, 9, Wen, M., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Neighborhood environment, individual resources, and health in older adults: Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study (CHASRS). Manuscript submitted for publication. Wu, H., Wang, J., Cacioppo, J. T., Glaser, R., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Malarkey, W. B. (1999). Chronic stress associated with spousal caregiving for patients with Alzheimer s dementia is associated with downregulation of B-lymphocyte GH mrna. Journal of Gerontology, 54, M212 M215. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, Received June 5, 2004 Accepted September 28, 2004

Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1

Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1 Okami Study Guide: Chapter 3 1 Chapter in Review 1. Heredity is the tendency of offspring to resemble their parents in various ways. Genes are units of heredity. They are functional strands of DNA grouped

More information

DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY

DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY DRAFT TJ PROGRAM OF STUDIES: AP PSYCHOLOGY COURSE DESCRIPTION AP Psychology engages students in a rigorous appraisal of many facets of our current understanding of psychology. The course is based on the

More information

Study Plan in Psychology Education

Study Plan in Psychology Education Study Plan in Psychology Education CONTENTS 1) Presentation 5) Mandatory Subjects 2) Requirements 6) Objectives 3) Study Plan / Duration 7) Suggested Courses 4) Academics Credit Table 1) Presentation offers

More information

APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula

APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula APA National Standards for High School Psychology Curricula http://www.apa.org/ed/natlstandards.html I. METHODS DOMAIN Standard Area IA: Introduction and Research Methods CONTENT STANDARD IA-1: Contemporary

More information

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254

Psychology. Kansas Course Code # 04254 High School Psychology Kansas Course Code # 04254 The American Psychological Association defines Psychology as the study of the mind and behavior. The discipline embraces all aspects of the human experience

More information

Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1

Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1 Grade 12 Psychology (40S) Outcomes Unedited Draft 1 Theme 1: Introduction and Research Methods Topic 1: Introduction 1.1.1 Define psychology, and list and explain its goals. 1.1.2 Describe and compare

More information

Introducing Social Psychology

Introducing Social Psychology Introducing Social Psychology Theories and Methods in Social Psychology 27 Feb 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu What is social psychology? A field within psychology that strives to understand the social dynamics

More information

There is a growing focus on moving upstream to protect mental health and reduce the incidence of mental illness.

There is a growing focus on moving upstream to protect mental health and reduce the incidence of mental illness. An Upstream Approach to Improving Psychological Wellbeing Dr Brian Marien Founder and Director of Positive Health Strategies brian.marien@positivegroup.org www.positivegroup.org Prevention or cure? Zola,

More information

Development Through the Lifespan. Psychosocial Stages During Infancy and Toddlerhood. First Appearance of Basic Emotions

Development Through the Lifespan. Psychosocial Stages During Infancy and Toddlerhood. First Appearance of Basic Emotions Development Through the Lifespan Chapter 6 Emotional and Social Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited

More information

Guide to the Focus in Mind, Brain, Behavior For History and Science Concentrators Science and Society Track Honors Eligible 2015-2016

Guide to the Focus in Mind, Brain, Behavior For History and Science Concentrators Science and Society Track Honors Eligible 2015-2016 Guide to the Focus in Mind, Brain, Behavior For History and Science Concentrators Science and Society Track Honors Eligible 2015-2016 Department of the History of Science Science Center 371 The Focus in

More information

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology.

Obtaining Knowledge. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology. Lecture 7 Methods of Scientific Observation and Analysis in Behavioral Psychology and Neuropsychology 1.Obtaining Knowledge 1. Correlation 2. Causation 2.Hypothesis Generation & Measures 3.Looking into

More information

Animal Models of Human Behavioral and Social Processes: What is a Good Animal Model? Dario Maestripieri

Animal Models of Human Behavioral and Social Processes: What is a Good Animal Model? Dario Maestripieri Animal Models of Human Behavioral and Social Processes: What is a Good Animal Model? Dario Maestripieri Criteria for assessing the validity of animal models of human behavioral research Face validity:

More information

Quality of Life The Priorities of Older People with a Cognitive Impairment

Quality of Life The Priorities of Older People with a Cognitive Impairment Living in a Nursing Home Quality of Life The Priorities of Older People with a Cognitive Impairment Compiled by Suzanne Cahill PhD and Ana Diaz in association with Dementia Services Information and Development

More information

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介 COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科 目 簡 介 COURSES FOR 4-YEAR UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMMES PSY2101 Introduction to Psychology (3 credits) The purpose of this course is to introduce fundamental concepts and theories in psychology

More information

Al Ahliyya Amman University Faculty of Arts Department of Psychology Course Description Psychology

Al Ahliyya Amman University Faculty of Arts Department of Psychology Course Description Psychology Al Ahliyya Amman University Faculty of Arts Department of Psychology Course Description Psychology 0731111 Psychology And Life {3}[3-3] Defining humans behavior; Essential life skills: problem solving,

More information

Dr V. J. Brown. Neuroscience (see Biomedical Sciences) History, Philosophy, Social Anthropology, Theological Studies.

Dr V. J. Brown. Neuroscience (see Biomedical Sciences) History, Philosophy, Social Anthropology, Theological Studies. Psychology - pathways & 1000 Level modules School of Psychology Head of School Degree Programmes Single Honours Degree: Joint Honours Degrees: Dr V. J. Brown Psychology Neuroscience (see Biomedical Sciences)

More information

From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development

From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development By Jack P. Shonkoff, MD This chapter explains the work of the Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Development

More information

Influenced by - Alfred Binet intelligence testing movement

Influenced by - Alfred Binet intelligence testing movement SA1 Trait Psychology Influenced by - Alfred Binet intelligence testing movement Origins - Psychologists became interested in seeing whether the success achieved with mental measurement might be repeated

More information

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress

Chapter Fourteen. Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress Chapter Fourteen Emotion, Reward, Aggression, and Stress EMOTIONS! Emotions generally include a Physical component, and a Subjective component or quality, and a Valence Emotions a product of Evolution?

More information

PSYC PSYCHOLOGY. 2011-2012 Calendar Proof

PSYC PSYCHOLOGY. 2011-2012 Calendar Proof PSYC PSYCHOLOGY PSYC1003 is a prerequisite for PSYC1004 and PSYC1004 is a prerequisite for all remaining Psychology courses. Note: See beginning of Section F for abbreviations, course numbers and coding.

More information

Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1

Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1 Chapter 1 Assignment Part 1 Careers in Psychology 1. Which of the following psychological professionals must always have a medical degree? a. psychologist b. psychiatric social worker c. psychiatrist d.

More information

Summary of research findings

Summary of research findings Summary of research findings Clinical Findings from the Mind Body Medical Institute at Harvard Medical School. Chronic pain patients reduce their physician visits by 36%. The Clinical Journal of Pain,

More information

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF. Interpretive Report. Developed by. Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF. Interpretive Report. Developed by. Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF Interpretive Report Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff Client Information Client Name : Sample Client Client ID

More information

AP Psychology 2008-2009 Academic Year

AP Psychology 2008-2009 Academic Year AP Psychology 2008-2009 Academic Year Course Description: The College Board Advanced Placement Program describes Advanced Placement Psychology as a course that is designed to introduce students to the

More information

Participating in Alzheimer s Disease Clinical Trials and Studies

Participating in Alzheimer s Disease Clinical Trials and Studies Participating in Alzheimer s Disease Clinical Trials and Studies FACT SHEET When Margaret was diagnosed with earlystage Alzheimer s disease at age 68, she wanted to do everything possible to combat the

More information

B.A. Programme. Psychology Department

B.A. Programme. Psychology Department Courses Description B.A. Programme Psychology Department 2307100 Principles of Psychology An introduction to the scientific study of basic processes underlying human and animal behavior. Sensation and

More information

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology 2014-2016

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology 2014-2016 Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in 2014-2016 B.A. Degree Completion Program in Program Prerequisites: PSY 101 General and a minimum G.P.A. of 2.5. major program: 36 credits Description: is the

More information

Design Philosophy. Should the School Building be Part of the Pedagogy? (From the Perspective of an Architect)

Design Philosophy. Should the School Building be Part of the Pedagogy? (From the Perspective of an Architect) Design Philosophy Should the School Building be Part of the Pedagogy? (From the Perspective of an Architect) The futurist thinker Dr. Jonas Salk, American medical researcher and virologist, is best known

More information

Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06

Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06 Course Information: HCS Curriculum: Social Studies 6 12 Hoover City Schools Secondary Curriculum Social Studies, 2005-06 Course Title: Psychology, IB Grade Level: 11-12 Course Description: This course

More information

College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology

College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology 100 INTRODUCTION TO CHOLOGY. (4) An introduction to the study of behavior covering theories, methods and findings of research in major areas of psychology. Topics covered will include the biological foundations

More information

Assignment Discovery Online Curriculum

Assignment Discovery Online Curriculum Assignment Discovery Online Curriculum Lesson title: Nature Versus Nurture Grade level: 9-12, with adaptation for younger students Subject area: Human Body Contemporary Studies Behavioral Science Duration:

More information

School of Social Work

School of Social Work MSW Core Curriculum for Generalist Practice St. Catherine University/University of St. Thomas Core and Advanced Competencies of the MSW Program The SCU/UST MSW curriculum prepares its graduates for advanced

More information

PSYCHOLOGY AND PROJECT MANAGEMENT

PSYCHOLOGY AND PROJECT MANAGEMENT PSYCHOLOGY AND PROJECT MANAGEMENT Human aspects of the project management Ver 1.0 2 360 Degree Approach of IT Infrastructure Projects PSYCHOLOGY AND PROJECT MANAGEMENT Abstract Project Management usually

More information

Department of Psychology

Department of Psychology Colorado State University 1 Department of Psychology Office in Behavioral Sciences Building, Room 201 (970) 491-3799 colostate.edu/depts/psychology (http://www.colostate.edu/depts/ Psychology) Professor

More information

The role of big data in medicine

The role of big data in medicine The role of big data in medicine November 2015 Technology is revolutionizing our understanding and treatment of disease, says the founding director of the Icahn Institute for Genomics and Multiscale Biology

More information

why happiness is good for business

why happiness is good for business why happiness is good for business Martyn Newman Ph.D. Randstad consulting psychologist In today s business world, driven by competitive advantage, success and wealth, happiness can easily be relegated

More information

Types of Psychology. Alex Thompson. Psychology Class. Professor Phelps

Types of Psychology. Alex Thompson. Psychology Class. Professor Phelps Running Head: PSYCHOLOGY 1 Types of Psychology Alex Thompson Psychology Class Professor Phelps March 4, 2014 PSYCHOLOGY 2 Types of Psychology Developmental psychology Developmental psychology entails the

More information

Psychology (PSYC) Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences

Psychology (PSYC) Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences Psychology (PSYC) Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences PSYC 1001 [0.5 credit] Introduction to Psychology I A survey of topics associated with psychology's role as a natural science,

More information

SINCLAIR COMMUNITY COLLEGE SCHOOL AND COMMUNITY PARTNERSHIPS College Credit Plus Course Descriptions 1

SINCLAIR COMMUNITY COLLEGE SCHOOL AND COMMUNITY PARTNERSHIPS College Credit Plus Course Descriptions 1 College Credit Plus Course Descriptions 1 ENGLISH & SOCIAL SCIENCES COM-2201: Introduction to Mass Communication An extensive examination of media theory and social effects. Topics covered include history,

More information

Why Study Psychology at The University of Western Ontario?

Why Study Psychology at The University of Western Ontario? Why Study Psychology at The University of Western Ontario? The Psychology Department at Western has over 50 faculty members representing seven Areas of Research Excellence. They work with talented graduate

More information

Specialisation Psychology

Specialisation Psychology Specialisation Psychology Semester 1 Semester 2 An Introduction to Doing Research Politics, Power and Governance I Philosophy of the Social Sciences Economics, Markets and Organisations I Rhetoric Law,

More information

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in B.A. Degree Completion Program in Program Prerequisites: PSY 101 General and a minimum G.P.A. of 2.5. major program: 36 credits Description: is the scientific

More information

Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016. 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu

Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016. 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu Behavioral Sciences INDIVIDUAL PROGRAM INFORMATION 2015 2016 866.Macomb1 (866.622.6621) www.macomb.edu Behavioral Sciences CREDENTIAL TITLE PROGRAM OPTIONS CREDIT HOURS REQUIRED Certificate Behavioral

More information

Undergraduate Psychology Major Learning Goals and Outcomes i

Undergraduate Psychology Major Learning Goals and Outcomes i Undergraduate Psychology Major Learning Goals and Outcomes i Goal 1: Knowledge Base of Psychology Demonstrate familiarity with the major concepts, theoretical perspectives, empirical findings, and historical

More information

Chapter 13. Prejudice: Causes and Cures

Chapter 13. Prejudice: Causes and Cures Chapter 13 Prejudice: Causes and Cures Prejudice Prejudice is ubiquitous; it affects all of us -- majority group members as well as minority group members. Prejudice Prejudice is dangerous, fostering negative

More information

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology

Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in Psychology Bachelor of Arts Degree Completion Program in 2015-2017 B.A. Degree Completion Program in Program Prerequisites: Associate s degree and a minimum G.P.A. of 2.5. 42 residency credits are required for an

More information

Medical College of Georgia Augusta, Georgia School of Medicine Competency based Objectives

Medical College of Georgia Augusta, Georgia School of Medicine Competency based Objectives Medical College of Georgia Augusta, Georgia School of Medicine Competency based Objectives Medical Knowledge Goal Statement: Medical students are expected to master a foundation of clinical knowledge with

More information

FUNDAMENTALS OF FAMILY THEORY 3. THE EIGHT INTERLOCKING CONCEPTS

FUNDAMENTALS OF FAMILY THEORY 3. THE EIGHT INTERLOCKING CONCEPTS FUNDAMENTALS OF FAMILY THEORY 3. THE EIGHT INTERLOCKING CONCEPTS 3.1. Bowen Family Systems Theory Bowen Family Systems Theory, also known as Bowen Natural Systems Theory, describes the natural emotional

More information

Psychology UNDERGRADUATE

Psychology UNDERGRADUATE Psychology Chair: Basma Faour, Ed.D. The Department of Psychology offers a B.A. program in General Psychology and M.A. programs in General Psychology, Clinical Psychology, Counseling, Industrial/Organizational

More information

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 2014-2015

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 2014-2015 COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 2014-2015 Course Definitions, Designators and Format Courses approved at the time of publication are listed in this bulletin. Not all courses are offered every term. Refer to the online

More information

From the 2011 Accreditation Council for Occupational Therapy Education (ACOTE ) Standards and Interpretive Guide:

From the 2011 Accreditation Council for Occupational Therapy Education (ACOTE ) Standards and Interpretive Guide: From the 2011 Accreditation Council for Occupational Therapy Education (ACOTE ) Standards and Interpretive Guide: FOUNDATIONAL CONTENT REQUIREMENTS: Program content must be based on a broad foundation

More information

Lone Star College-Tomball Community Library 30555 Tomball Parkway Tomball, TX 77375 http://www.lonestar.edu/library.

Lone Star College-Tomball Community Library 30555 Tomball Parkway Tomball, TX 77375 http://www.lonestar.edu/library. Lone Star College-Tomball Community Library 30555 Tomball Parkway Tomball, TX 77375 http://www.lonestar.edu/library.htm 832-559-4211 PSYCHOLOGY Scholarly ELECTRONIC Electronic JOURNAL Journals LIST The

More information

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040 School of Psychology Important Degree Information: B.Sc./M.A. Honours The general requirements are 480 credits over a period of normally 4 years (and not more than 5 years) or part-time equivalent; the

More information

UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL

UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL UMEÅ INTERNATIONAL SCHOOL OF PUBLIC HEALTH Master Programme in Public Health - Programme and Courses Academic year 2015-2016 Public Health and Clinical Medicine Umeå International School of Public Health

More information

Psychology. Academic Requirements. Academic Requirements. Career Opportunities. Minor. Major. Mount Mercy University 1

Psychology. Academic Requirements. Academic Requirements. Career Opportunities. Minor. Major. Mount Mercy University 1 Mount Mercy University 1 Psychology The psychology major presents a scientific approach to the study of individual behavior and experience. The goal of the major is to provide an empirical and theoretical

More information

MANAGING FOR THE UNEXPECTED

MANAGING FOR THE UNEXPECTED Ross Thought in Action MANAGING FOR THE UNEXPECTED Kathleen M. Sutcliffe Professor, University of Michigan, Stephen M. Ross School of Business Marlys K. Christianson Assistant Professor, University of

More information

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION

CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION CHAPTER 4: PREJUDICE AND DISCRIMINATION CHAPTER OVERVIEW Chapter 4 introduces you to the related concepts of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination. The chapter begins with definitions of these three

More information

Course Descriptions: M.A. in Clinical- Community Psychology

Course Descriptions: M.A. in Clinical- Community Psychology Course Descriptions: M.A. in Clinical- Community Psychology PSYC 501 Theories of Psychotherapy This course will offer in- depth discussion on the historical, philosophical and theoretical foundations of

More information

AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic

AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic AP Biology Essential Knowledge Student Diagnostic Background The Essential Knowledge statements provided in the AP Biology Curriculum Framework are scientific claims describing phenomenon occurring in

More information

Factors of Adolescent Self-Concept: Mass Mediated, Peer, and Family Communication. David J. Feliciano. University of West Florida

Factors of Adolescent Self-Concept: Mass Mediated, Peer, and Family Communication. David J. Feliciano. University of West Florida Running head: FACTORS OF ADOLESCENT SELF-CONCEPT 1 Factors of Adolescent Self-Concept: Mass Mediated, Peer, and Family Communication David J. Feliciano University of West Florida FACTORS OF ADOLESCENT

More information

Department of Psychology

Department of Psychology 402 Phone: (512) 245-2526 Fax: (512) 245-3153 Degree Programs Offered BA, major in Psychology BS, major in Psychology Minors Offered Psychology Forensic Psychology Sports Psychology Department of Psychology

More information

Master of Arts, Counseling Psychology Course Descriptions

Master of Arts, Counseling Psychology Course Descriptions Master of Arts, Counseling Psychology Course Descriptions Advanced Theories of Counseling & Intervention (3 credits) This course addresses the theoretical approaches used in counseling, therapy and intervention.

More information

Reducing human error in revenue management decision-making

Reducing human error in revenue management decision-making Reducing human error in revenue management decision-making Recent revenue management (RM) research has demonstrated what appear to be systematic deviations from optimization models of decision-making.

More information

Biological Sciences Initiative. Human Genome

Biological Sciences Initiative. Human Genome Biological Sciences Initiative HHMI Human Genome Introduction In 2000, researchers from around the world published a draft sequence of the entire genome. 20 labs from 6 countries worked on the sequence.

More information

Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel

Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel Oregon Association of Vocational Special Needs Personnel Brain Development and Traumainformed Services for Emerging Adults Hood River, OR February 21, 2015 2015 Conference: Making Connections Julie M.

More information

Social Work Field Education Core Competencies and Practice Behaviors

Social Work Field Education Core Competencies and Practice Behaviors Social Work Field Education Core Competencies and Practice Behaviors The School of Social Work Field Education Program addresses each of the Council on Social Work Education (CSWE) Core Competencies and

More information

Perceptions and Preferences of Wellness Travel Destinations of American Travelers

Perceptions and Preferences of Wellness Travel Destinations of American Travelers Journal of Tourism and Hospitality Management June 2015, Vol. 3, No. 1, pp. 23-28 ISSN: 2372-5125 (Print), 2372-5133 (Online) Copyright The Author(s). All Rights Reserved. Published by American Research

More information

It is widely accepted by those in the scientific community that women have been

It is widely accepted by those in the scientific community that women have been 1 It is widely accepted by those in the scientific community that women have been systematically disregarded and discriminated against in science. However, the extent to which this has undermined aspirations

More information

Guidelines for Integrative Core Curriculum Themes and Perspectives Designations

Guidelines for Integrative Core Curriculum Themes and Perspectives Designations Guidelines for Integrative Core Curriculum Themes and Perspectives Designations The purpose of this document is to provide guidelines for faculty wishing to have new or existing courses carry Themes and

More information

Graduate and Postdoctoral Affairs School of Biomedical Sciences College of Medicine. Graduate Certificate. Metabolic & Nutritional Medicine

Graduate and Postdoctoral Affairs School of Biomedical Sciences College of Medicine. Graduate Certificate. Metabolic & Nutritional Medicine Graduate and Postdoctoral Affairs School of Biomedical Sciences College of Medicine Graduate Certificate in Metabolic & Nutritional Medicine Graduate Certificate Metabolic & Nutritional Medicine Purpose

More information

SPIRITUALITY S ROLE IN ADDICTION RECOVERY

SPIRITUALITY S ROLE IN ADDICTION RECOVERY SPIRITUALITY S ROLE IN ADDICTION RECOVERY DEFINITION OF DISEASE AMERICAN SOCIETY ADDICTION MEDICINE-ADDICTION IS A PRIMARY, CHRONIC DISEASE OF THE BRAIN REWARD, MEMORY, MOTIVATION AND RELATED CIRCUITS.

More information

Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, 2015-16

Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, 2015-16 Instructor Information Julie P. Telenko Email: jtelenko@wcpss.net Website: rhsjtelenko.weebly.com Twitter: @jtelenkorhs Advanced Placement Psychology Syllabus Rolesville High School, 2015-16 Student and

More information

What will I study? Year One core modules currently include:

What will I study? Year One core modules currently include: What will I study? Year One core modules currently include: Introduction to Psychology You will examine how psychology research is conducted and interpreted; a range of perspectives in psychology, including

More information

KEY FINDINGS AND IMPLICATIONS

KEY FINDINGS AND IMPLICATIONS May 2015 Publication #2015-20 The Developing Brain: IMPLICATIONS FOR YOUTH PROGRAMS OVERVIEW The brain, with more than 100 billion neurons, is our body s most complex organ. There is increasing global

More information

Psychology. Administered by the Department of Psychology within the College of Arts and Sciences.

Psychology. Administered by the Department of Psychology within the College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology Dr. Spencer Thompson, Professor, is the Chair of Psychology and Coordinator of Child and Family Studies. After receiving his Ph.D. in Developmental Psychology at the University of California,

More information

Statement of the Chief Medical Health Officer

Statement of the Chief Medical Health Officer Statement of the Chief Medical Health Officer June, 2011 Health Concerns About Cellular Phone Transmission Antennae and Base Stations In 2005, in response to community concerns and after reviewing the

More information

& Global Climate Change

& Global Climate Change PSychology & Global Climate Change addressing a multifaceted phenomenon and set of challenges EXECUTIVE SUMMARY A Report of the American Psychological Association Task Force on the Interface Between Psychology

More information

Chapter Five Socialization. Human Development: Biology and Society. Social Isolation

Chapter Five Socialization. Human Development: Biology and Society. Social Isolation Chapter Five Socialization Socialization is the lifelong process of social interaction through which individuals acquire a self-identify and the physical, mental, and social skills needed for survival

More information

Master s Entry into Nursing. Academic Manual 2015-2016

Master s Entry into Nursing. Academic Manual 2015-2016 Master s Entry into Nursing Academic Manual 2015-2016 TABLE OF CONTENTS Overview of the Master s Entry into Nursing (MEN) Program 2 Outcomes 2 Plan of Study 3-4 Course Descriptions 5-11 Overview of the

More information

UPS 411.201 GENERAL EDUCATION: GOALS FOR STUDENT LEARNING

UPS 411.201 GENERAL EDUCATION: GOALS FOR STUDENT LEARNING University Policy Statement California State University, Fullerton GENERAL EDUCATION: GOALS FOR STUDENT LEARNING The Goals of General Education General education is central to a university education, and

More information

Simulating the Structural Evolution of Software

Simulating the Structural Evolution of Software Simulating the Structural Evolution of Software Benjamin Stopford 1, Steve Counsell 2 1 School of Computer Science and Information Systems, Birkbeck, University of London 2 School of Information Systems,

More information

Course Descriptions Psychology

Course Descriptions Psychology Course Descriptions Psychology PSYC 1520 (F/S) General Psychology. An introductory survey of the major areas of current psychology such as the scientific method, the biological bases for behavior, sensation

More information

AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY An Introduction to MODULE - I 1 AN INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY As human beings our curiosity drives us to know the reasons behind various events happening around us. Whenever we meet somebody or see someone

More information

Autism and the Brain

Autism and the Brain Autism and the Brain Jill Bailer and Barbara Behnke Jane Long Middle School, Houston, Texas, and Winamac High School, Winamac, Indiana In collaboration with Julia Whitney 1 and Celene Mulholland 2 1 Battelle

More information

Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders

Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders Learners with Emotional or Behavioral Disorders S H A N A M. H A T Z O P O U L O S G E O R G E W A S H I N G T O N U N I V E R S I T Y S P E D 2 0 1 S U M M E R 2 0 1 0 Overview of Emotional and Behavioral

More information

THE DEPARTMENT OF SOCIAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES POLITICAL SCIENCE PROGRAM HANDBOOK

THE DEPARTMENT OF SOCIAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES POLITICAL SCIENCE PROGRAM HANDBOOK THE DEPARTMENT OF SOCIAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES POLITICAL SCIENCE PROGRAM HANDBOOK MAY 2015 Political Science Objectives 1. To study the structure and functions of government. 2. To understand political

More information

Finding Balance in Your Grieving. Dr. Jo Christner, Psy.D. The death of your spouse most likely turned your whole world upside down

Finding Balance in Your Grieving. Dr. Jo Christner, Psy.D. The death of your spouse most likely turned your whole world upside down Finding Balance in Your Grieving Dr. Jo Christner, Psy.D. The death of your spouse most likely turned your whole world upside down out of balance. Everything seemed to change in your life especially you.

More information

CHAOS, COMPLEXITY, AND FLOCKING BEHAVIOR: METAPHORS FOR LEARNING

CHAOS, COMPLEXITY, AND FLOCKING BEHAVIOR: METAPHORS FOR LEARNING GIVING WINGS TO NEW IDEAS Wingspread Journal, Summer 1996 CHAOS, COMPLEXITY, AND FLOCKING BEHAVIOR: METAPHORS FOR LEARNING by Stephanie Pace Marshall Sir Isaac Newton saw the universe as an orderly clock.

More information

Kansas Board of Regents Precollege Curriculum Courses Approved for University Admissions

Kansas Board of Regents Precollege Curriculum Courses Approved for University Admissions Kansas Board of Regents Precollege Curriculum Courses Approved for University Admissions Original Publication April 6, 2011 Revision Dates June 13, 2011 May 23, 2012 Kansas Board of Regents Precollege

More information

PSYD CURRICULUM AND COURSE DESCRIPTIONS

PSYD CURRICULUM AND COURSE DESCRIPTIONS PSYD CURRICULUM AND COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 0999A D The Evolving Professional This sequence begins with practice in the acquisition of the growth oriented counseling skills necessary for working with individuals,

More information

Addiction is a Brain Disease

Addiction is a Brain Disease Addiction is a Brain Disease By ALAN I. LESHNER, MD A core concept evolving with scientific advances over the past decade is that drug addiction is a brain disease that develops over time as a result of

More information

NEOSHO COUNTY COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER COURSE SYLLABUS. Division: Applied Science (AS) Liberal Arts (LA) Workforce Development (WD)

NEOSHO COUNTY COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER COURSE SYLLABUS. Division: Applied Science (AS) Liberal Arts (LA) Workforce Development (WD) NEOSHO COUNTY COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER COURSE SYLLABUS COURSE IDENTIFICATION Course Code/Number: PSYC 155 Course Title: General Psychology Division: Applied Science (AS) Liberal Arts (LA) Workforce Development

More information

Chapter 2 A Systems Approach to Leadership Overview

Chapter 2 A Systems Approach to Leadership Overview Chapter 2 A Systems Approach to Leadership Overview 2.1 Introduction Developing a high performance organisation is an appropriate and achievable goal in today s business environment. High performance organisations

More information

Epilepsy 101: Getting Started

Epilepsy 101: Getting Started American Epilepsy Society 1 Epilepsy 101 for nurses has been developed by the American Epilepsy Society to prepare professional nurses to understand the general issues, concerns and needs of people with

More information

PSYCHOLOGY 101 General Psychology Spring 2008

PSYCHOLOGY 101 General Psychology Spring 2008 PSYCHOLOGY 101 General Psychology Spring 2008 Instructor: Dorothy Bach, MSCP, MHC Location: Chaminade BS 101 Phone: 739-8557 Office Hours: By appointment Time: MWF 11:00-11:50 email: dbach@chaminade.edu

More information

ACADEMIC DIRECTOR: Carla Marquez-Lewis Email Contact: THE PROGRAM Career and Advanced Study Prospects Program Requirements

ACADEMIC DIRECTOR: Carla Marquez-Lewis Email Contact: THE PROGRAM Career and Advanced Study Prospects Program Requirements Psychology (BA) ACADEMIC DIRECTOR: Carla Marquez-Lewis CUNY School of Professional Studies 101 West 31 st Street, 7 th Floor New York, NY 10001 Email Contact: Carla Marquez-Lewis, carla.marquez-lewis@cuny.edu

More information

ADVANCED DIPLOMA IN COUNSELLING AND PSYCHOLOGY

ADVANCED DIPLOMA IN COUNSELLING AND PSYCHOLOGY ACC School of Counselling & Psychology Pte Ltd www.acc.edu.sg Tel: (65) 6339-5411 9 Penang Road #13-22 Park Mall SC Singapore 238459 1) Introduction to the programme ADVANCED DIPLOMA IN COUNSELLING AND

More information

430 CURRICULUM AND SYLLABUS for Classes XI & XII

430 CURRICULUM AND SYLLABUS for Classes XI & XII 430 CURRICULUM AND SYLLABUS for Classes XI & XII PSYCHOLOGY CLASS XI (THEORY) One Paper Time: 3 Hours 70 Marks 180 Periods Unit Unitwise Weightage Marks I What is Psychology? 6 I Methods of Enquiry in

More information

LONG-TERM CARE IN AMERICA: AMERICANS OUTLOOK AND PLANNING FOR FUTURE CARE

LONG-TERM CARE IN AMERICA: AMERICANS OUTLOOK AND PLANNING FOR FUTURE CARE Research Highlights LONG-TERM CARE IN AMERICA: AMERICANS OUTLOOK AND PLANNING FOR FUTURE CARE INTRODUCTION In the next 25 years, the U.S. population is expected to include 82 million Americans over the

More information